Next Article in Journal
Exploring the Influences of Nurses’ Partnership with Parents, Attitude to Families’ Importance in Nursing Care, and Professional Self-Efficacy on Quality of Pediatric Nursing Care: A Path Model
Next Article in Special Issue
Gestational Pesticide Exposure and Child Respiratory Health
Previous Article in Journal
Screen Time and Sleep of Rural and Urban South African Preschool Children
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis Investigating the Relationship between Exposures to Chemical and Non-Chemical Stressors during Prenatal Development and Childhood Externalizing Behaviors
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

A Meta-Analysis of Stressors from the Total Environment Associated with Children’s General Cognitive Ability

by
Frances M. Nilsen
1,*,
Jazmin D.C. Ruiz
1,2 and
Nicolle S. Tulve
1
1
Office of Research and Development, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency Research Triangle Park, Durham, NC 27709, USA
2
Honeywell International, Buffalo, NY 14210, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(15), 5451; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155451
Submission received: 10 June 2020 / Revised: 17 July 2020 / Accepted: 20 July 2020 / Published: 29 July 2020

Abstract

:
General cognitive ability, often referred to as ‘general intelligence’, comprises a variety of correlated abilities. Childhood general cognitive ability is a well-studied area of research and can be used to predict social outcomes and perceived success. Early life stage (e.g., prenatal, postnatal, toddler) exposures to stressors (i.e., chemical and non-chemical stressors from the total (built, natural, social) environment) can impact the development of childhood cognitive ability. Building from our systematic scoping review (Ruiz et al., 2016), we conducted a meta-analysis to evaluate more than 100 stressors related to cognitive development. Our meta-analysis identified 23 stressors with a significant increase in their likelihood to influence childhood cognitive ability by 10% or more, and 80 stressors were observed to have a statistically significant effect on cognitive ability. Stressors most impactful to cognition during the prenatal period were related to maternal health and the mother’s ability to access information relevant to a healthy pregnancy (e.g., diet, lifestyle). Stressors most impactful to cognition during the early childhood period were dietary nutrients (infancy), quality of social interaction (toddler), and exposure to toxic substances (throughout early childhood). In conducting this analysis, we examined the relative impact of real-world exposures on cognitive development to attempt to understand the inter-relationships between exposures to both chemical and non-chemical stressors and early developmental life stages. Our findings suggest that the stressors observed to be the most influential to childhood cognitive ability are not permanent and can be broadly categorized as activities/behaviors which can be modified to improve childhood cognition. This meta-analysis supports the idea that there are complex relationships between a child’s total environment and early cognitive development.

1. Introduction

General cognitive ability, often referred to as ‘general intelligence’, comprises a variety of correlated abilities including spatial and verbal abilities, information processing speed, and memory [1,2]. Childhood general cognitive ability is a well-studied area of research and can be used to predict social outcomes and perceived success [2,3]. As such, the World Health Organization (WHO) reports that conditions that allow cognitive ability to flourish are vital to children’s lifelong success. The field of cognitive development is moving towards a more comprehensive framework for characterizing cognition [4]. Recent studies have shown the importance of both child-specific inherent characteristics and environmental factors, as well as the interactions between factors related to early childhood cognitive development [5]. However, research generally emphasizes the child or the environment, and the peripheral factors that influence cognitive ability are seldom taken into account [4,5,6,7,8,9,10]. The complex inter-relationships between chemical and non-chemical stressors and their possible collective/synergistic/antagonistic effect(s) on cognitive development have yet to be fully elucidated.
Early life stage (e.g., prenatal, postnatal, toddler) exposures to chemical and non-chemical stressors from the total (built, natural, social) environment can impact the development of childhood cognitive ability [1,11,12,13,14]. While identifying multiple chemical and non-chemical stressors from the home, community, and social environments is a methodological challenge, many studies have examined specific groups of stressors [15,16,17,18]. Exposure to certain chemicals during critical windows of development causes neurotoxicity through alterations in brain structure and function and may lead to diminished cognitive development [17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26]. Exposures to social stressors, particularly those related to socioeconomics, have been shown to affect childhood cognitive ability in a variety of cohorts [7,27,28,29,30,31]. Activities, such as diet and sleep patterns, have been shown to be correlated to cognitive outcomes in children [32,33,34,35,36,37]. Stressors experienced during early life stages that coincide with sensorimotor development (infancy) and pre-operational development (toddler and early childhood) could affect school-age readiness and cognitive performance into adolescence when inherent factors (i.e., genetics) gain increasing influence on cognitive ability [38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47].
In a systematic scoping review, Ruiz et al. [48] evaluated more than 100 stressors related to cognitive development, including inherent characteristics, and activities and behaviors. To better understand how development of cognitive ability is affected by the interactions of multiple stressors, Ruiz et al. [48] probed the links between chemical and non-chemical stressors, inherent characteristics, and activities and behaviors. Ruiz et al. concluded that: (1) positive impacts to cognition were largely attributable to non-chemical stressors related to the mother and child; (2) negative impacts were attributed to both chemical and non-chemical stressors in the child’s total environment; and (3) the true etiology of cognitive ability is likely a combination of stressors experienced throughout a child’s life course [48]. To date, this review is the most comprehensive analysis of the interactions of chemical and non-chemical stressors from the total environment on childhood cognitive development. Eleven studies published since 2016 have used Ruiz et al. [48] as a springboard to examine the effects of specific stressors such as neighborhood chaos, industrial pollution, environmental contaminants, socioeconomic status, and poverty on childhood cognitive development [6,7,28,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56]. Recent publications related to childhood cognitive development that do not cite Ruiz et al. [48] focus on the effects of postnatal care, maternal mental health, and physical brain characteristics [57,58,59,60].
Our objective was to identify key stressors by conducting a meta-analysis on those examined in the Ruiz et al. [48] systematic scoping review. The children’s health and well-being conceptual framework put forth by Tulve et al. [17] was used to organize the stressors into life stage-specific categories and discuss the impacts of each life stage and stressor combination on childhood cognitive ability. In doing this meta-analysis, we aimed to understand the effects of real-world exposures on cognitive development as opposed to their effects in a controlled setting such as randomized control and intervention studies.

2. Methods

2.1. Data Collection and Extraction

Ruiz et al. [48] identified more than 100 factors that influenced a child’s cognitive development and described the complex relationships that exist between those factors. Ruiz et al. [48] conducted a search of the peer-reviewed literature in PubMed, Web of Science, and PsycINFO databases using a combination of words associated with cognitive health (e.g., neurodevelopment or cognitive development) OR ((cognition or learning) AND association) AND children. Included studies were published in English and conducted in children from birth to 18 years of age.

2.1.1. Inclusion Criteria

The inclusion criteria from Ruiz et al. [48] are as follows:
  • Observational studies, randomized control trials, review or meta-analysis;
  • Time of exposure to a determinant or stressor occurred at or before health outcome was assessed;
  • Health outcome was measured in children under 18 years old;
  • General cognitive outcome was measured using current and earlier versions of evidence-based assessments of cognitive functioning classified as well established [19] and expressed as a continuous variable or categorized as below average or significant cognitive delay (i.e., >1 or >2 standard deviations (SD) below the mean).
  • Study included a measure of association and statistical significance;
  • Majority of study participants were healthy children without any existing developmental disabilities, neonatal morbidities, pathologies associated with cognitive deficits, or rare disorders.
All 258 studies included in the Ruiz et al. [48] review were examined for this analysis. Pearson’s r data were extracted from the original publications and converted to odds ratios (OR) for meta-analysis to determine the relationship between each factor and general cognitive ability (Table 1) [61]. Studies were excluded from this analysis if there was not enough statistical information to extract Pearson’s r, if there were not enough of the same type of stressors for a statistical comparison, or if there were too many covariates included in the original analysis that could not be separated into distinct categories, or if there were no other ‘like’ stressors to group the covariates with. Overall, the lack of statistical information was the most common reason for exclusion. A total of 185 studies were used in this analysis.

2.1.2. Data Grouping

The extracted data were organized into five categories that we refer to as “factors”. The five factor categories are: activities/behaviors, social, inherent, maternal, and chemical stressors. By including the maternal factors category, we were able to separate prenatal stressors from childhood stressors for specific analyses, as well as examine the relationship between maternal stressors and childhood cognitive ability. Since many of the stressors could be categorized into more than one factor group, we also provide the secondary factor categories. The factor groups and associated subgroupings are provided in Table 1.

2.2. Statistical Analyses

Analyses were conducted and figures created using R Studio (version 1.2.5033) (RStudio, Boston, MA, USA) with the tidyverse, ggplot2, and metafor packages [62,63,64,65]. The code used in this work is provided in the accompanying R-markdown document created with the rmarkdown package [66,67].

2.2.1. Meta-Analysis

The factor groups and associated subgroupings were analyzed using the random-effects meta-analysis (RMA) with the restricted maximum likelihood (REML) estimator. The random-effects method was selected because the included studies are diverse and report different metrics of childhood cognitive ability with data acquired through both survey and measurement. Therefore, this method provides a more appropriate estimate of variance than other estimators [68,69].
The output of the meta-analysis is reported as an odds ratio (OR) since the OR is a measure of association between exposure to a stressor and an outcome and can be used to compare the odds of various exposures eliciting the outcome of interest [70]. When the OR = 1, this indicates that the exposure does not affect the odds of the outcome occurring. When the OR > 1, higher odds of the outcome occurring are indicated, and lower odds of the outcome are indicated when the OR < 1 [70]. When the range of values used to determine the OR crosses below 0 or above 1 it indicates the strength of the data in terms of ‘effect/no effect’ and shows the range of the data included in the OR determination. Due to the diversity of the data, we report the OR as an indication of the likelihood of an effect on childhood cognitive ability rather than an indication of positive/negative association, so as not to overstate the results of our analysis.

2.2.2. Data Visualization—Violin Plots

To present the analyzed data, we used violin plots because these types of plots provide the opportunity to visualize the density of the ORs within our large dataset. In the violin plots, the factor group/subgroup is provided along the y-axis and the OR scale is provided along the x-axis. The violin width represents the distribution of the ORs in each analysis and the violin height represents the OR density. The widest section of each violin indicates where the probability is greatest of the OR being that value based on the included data. The dots in the center of the violin represent the mean OR and the error bars represent the mean standard error (SE) from that dataset. The included reference count, lines of extracted data (i.e., sampling events), and collective sample size (i.e., individuals) from the included references/sampling events are included in Table 1 for reference.

2.2.3. Sensitivity Analysis

Data robustness was examined post-hoc using seven meta-analytic estimation methods [71]. The seven estimation methods were the REML, the maximum likelihood (ML), the fixed-effects (FE), the Hedges (HE), the DerSimonian–Laird (DL), the empirical Bayesian (EB), and the Paul–Mandel (PM) estimators. Data robustness is expressed as a percentage that reflects the number of estimators that yielded ORs within 1% of each other, which is below the 10% threshold generally used (Table 2).

2.2.4. Publication Bias

The risk of publication bias in the meta-analyses was estimated using two methods for comparison to the log OR from each original study: the inverse number of participants (1/n) and the standard error (SE). Since there is a correlation between the SE and the magnitude of the OR (which can lead to inflated type 1 error), we used the 1/n method of bias assessment. However, the results of both methods are presented, as the SE method is more common in the literature [72,73,74] (Table 2). We present these two statistical methods rather than the funnel plot that is often used with meta-analyses as the data included herein are quite diverse, and may lead to an incorrect assumption about the data based on the 1/SE calculation, which can also leads to an inflated type 1 error rate [75,76].

3. Results and Discussion

A total of 185 references from the Ruiz et al. [48] scoping review were included in this meta-analysis. From these 185 references, we extracted 408 maternal stressors, 245 inherent characteristics, 128 activities and behaviors, 125 social stressors, and 110 chemical stressors (Table 1). Stressors were grouped into either factor or stressor level groups for statistical analyses. Within each factor, there were at least three exposure routes composed of more specific stressors, which also included at least three sampling events. All stressor groups were statistically assessed using the RMA method, except for nitrogen dioxide (NO2) since two of the three sampling events were extracted from the same publication.
Stressors were grouped according to life stage (i.e., prenatal, childhood) and the results are presented longitudinally for ease of comprehension. Prenatal exposures include maternal factors and factors inherent to the child, since we believe that child-specific inherent characteristics are linked to maternal stressors experienced during pregnancy. Childhood factors include activities and behaviors and social and chemical stressors experienced after birth and span the birth–toddler life stages [17].
Analyses to determine publication bias were conducted on each factor group individually and no statistically significant publication bias was detected (Table 2). Lack of publication bias is most likely due to the inclusion of data with both positive and negative relationships to childhood cognitive ability rather than only positive-skewed data. The robustness of the included data is a testament to the number of publications included in the original scoping review, such that inclusion of a wide range of stressors resulted in a robust analysis regardless of the estimator used. Sensitivity analysis revealed robust sensitivity of the data in the meta-analyses. These metrics attest to the quality of the included data, with large sample sizes and included numerous sampling events to provide robust, unbiased results (Table 2).

3.1. Prenatal Exposures

The prenatal data presented here includes maternal factors that span the preconception and prenatal life stages and child-specific inherent characteristics that are determined during the prenatal period [17].

3.1.1. Maternal Factors

There were 84 references with maternal factors, including 408 sampling events of several types of stressors: substance use (147 sampling events), socioeconomic metrics (139 sampling events), and maternal health (122 sampling events) [29,77,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92,93,94,95,96,97,98,99,100,101,102,103,104,105,106,107,108,109,110,111,112,113,114,115,116,117,118,119,120,121,122,123,124,125,126,127,128,129,130,131,132,133,134,135,136,137,138,139,140,141,142,143,144,145,146,147,148,149,150,151,152,153,154,155,156,157,158,159,160]. Each group included several individual stressors. Substance use included alcohol (amount; 60 sampling events), cigarettes (38 sampling events), alcohol (binging; 19 sampling events), cocaine (18 sampling events), and other narcotics (12 sampling events). Socioeconomics included education (71 sampling events), income (29 sampling events), employment (15 sampling events), home location (13 sampling events), language (seven sampling events), resources (six sampling events), technology access (six sampling events), and home ownership (five sampling events). Maternal health included age (31 sampling events), mental health (24 sampling events), body mass index (BMI, 22 sampling events), thyroid health (20 sampling events), stress (20 sampling events), and cortisol (10 sampling events). When all maternal stressors were examined in the factor-level RMA, the OR was 1.04 (p ≤ 0.001; Table 1) which indicates that the factors are associated with a 4% greater likelihood of effect on childhood cognitive outcomes compared to when the factors are absent. The individual substance abuse stressors are discussed in the supplemental text.

Socioeconomics

When stressors within the maternal factor group were analyzed separately, the socioeconomic stressors were observed to have the greatest impact on childhood cognitive ability, with a 13% increase in likelihood of effect indicated (OR = 1.13, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1). The stressors included in the socioeconomic group had varying amounts of influence on childhood cognitive ability, but all were statistically significant (Figure 1). Maternal education (OR = 1.18, p ≤ 0.001), technology access (OR = 1.17, p ≤ 0.01), and income (OR = 1.13, p ≤ 0.001) were observed to have the greatest likelihood of effect on childhood cognition, ranging from 13–18% increased likelihood. Employment (OR = 1.06, p ≤ 0.001) and resources (OR = 1.06, p ≤ 0.01) were observed to have a 6% increased effect on childhood cognition. On the other hand, home ownership (OR = 0.99, p ≤ 0.05) and language spoken (OR = 0.96, p ≤ 0.001) were observed to have a 1% and 9% decreased likelihood of effect on childhood cognitive ability (Table 1).
The greater impact observed for education, technology access, income, employment, and resources were logical as these stressors are interrelated. Interestingly, home ownership was observed to have a decreased impact even though it is also generally interrelated with the preceding stressors. The decreased impact of language on childhood cognitive ability is the most interesting, as bilingualism suggests proficient cognitive ability [161]. However, a child coming to an English-only setting from a non-English speaking household may struggle to adapt and learn skills taught in English as well as lose proficiency in their native language [161]. Further investigation is needed to determine whether or not a new language is beneficial or detrimental to early childhood cognition [162].

Maternal Health

The maternal health stressor group collective analysis revealed that these stressors have a 4% increased likelihood to influence childhood cognitive ability (OR = 1.04, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1, Figure 1). However, when the stressors were analyzed separately, greater detail was revealed. Maternal age (OR = 1.08, p ≤ 0.001), thyroid health (OR = 1.05, p ≤ 0.001), BMI (OR = 1.01, p ≤ 0.001), and cortisol concentration related to physiological stress (OR = 1.08, p ≤ 0.01) influenced childhood cognitive ability with increases between 1% and 8%, while stressors including mental health (OR= 0.91, p ≤ 0.001), and perceived stress (OR = 0.97, p ≤ 0.001) were statistically significant but had a decreased likelihood of influence compared to no exposure (Table 1).
It is well recognized that maternal age is directly related to adverse neonatal outcomes, so it is not surprising that maternal age influenced childhood cognitive ability [163,164]. Recent evidence suggests that thyroid disease is also associated with adverse pregnancy outcomes, and we observed that general thyroid health had an increased impact on childhood cognitive ability [165,166]. Similarly, both low and high maternal BMI are known to affect pregnancy outcomes, and the statistically significant relationship between maternal BMI and childhood cognition observed here could be further explored to determine underlying mechanisms [167,168]. Maternal mental health has been shown to be related to the child’s mental health outcomes, but it appears to be less of an influence for childhood cognitive ability [169].
Interestingly, both cortisol-measured and perceived stress were statistically significant but had opposite likelihoods of impact on childhood cognitive ability compared to no exposure to stress (p ≤ 0.001 and p ≤ 0.01, respectively). There is not always a correlation between elevated cortisol and perceived stress. In this study, cortisol measurements were observed to have a greater impact on childhood cognitive ability than perceived stress (Table 1). Since cortisol level is a physiological indicator of stress it is logical that this variable would be more closely related to childhood cognition. However, the significant relationship observed for perceived stress suggests that cognitive development is not solely influenced by physiology, but also by the emotional and mental environment associated with child development [11,21,60].

3.1.2. Inherent Characteristics

There were 65 references that examined child-specific inherent characteristics. These references included: anthropometry (138 sampling events), birth outcomes (67 sampling events), and child health (40 sampling events) [29,36,79,81,82,83,84,88,89,97,99,100,101,102,103,104,106,107,108,113,115,116,119,120,122,123,126,127,128,133,136,137,138,139,142,143,146,147,148,149,150,152,153,155,156,157,159,170,171,172,173,174,175,176,177,178,179,180,181,182,183,184,185,186,187,188,189,190,191,192,193,194,195,196,197,198,199,200,201,202,203,204,205,206,207,208,209,210,211,212,213,214,215,216,217]. Stressors included in the anthropometry group were sex (63 sampling events), birth weight (47 sampling events), body length at birth (12 sampling events), head size at birth (10 sampling events), and fetal growth (6 sampling events). Birth outcomes included parity (24 sampling events), preterm birth (21 sampling events), gestational age (16 sampling events), delivery method (three sampling events), and if multiple births occurred (three sampling events). Child health included medical history (18 sampling events), genetic disorders (15 sampling events), and iron deficiency (seven sampling events). When all child-specific inherent characteristics were examined together in the factor-level RMA, the OR was 1.03 (p ≤ 0.001; Table 1) which indicated to us that the included characteristics were associated with a 3% greater effect on childhood cognitive outcomes than when the characteristics were absent. The individual child health stressors are discussed in the supplemental text.

Anthropometry and Birth Outcomes

All anthropometrics were observed to be associated with a 10% increase in effect on childhood cognitive ability when combined in the RMA (OR = 1.1, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1, Figure 2). When individual stressors were considered, all were statistically significant (Table 1). Head circumference at birth was observed to have the greatest likelihood of impact on childhood cognitive ability with a 22% increase (OR = 1.22, p ≤ 0.001). Body length at birth (OR = 1.21, p ≤ 0.001) and birth weight (OR = 1.14, p ≤ 0.001) were observed to have 21% and 14% increases in likelihood of effect, respectively. Fetal growth was also observed to be influential to cognitive ability with a 6% increase in effect (OR = 1.06, p ≤ 0.01). These four stressors are interrelated, as well as related to the birth outcomes stressors: gestational age (OR = 1.11, p ≤ 0.001), preterm birth (OR = 0.78, p ≤ 0.001), and parity (OR = 0.89, p ≤ 0.001). These stressors are all related to the length of a pregnancy, the mother’s diet during pregnancy, previous pregnancies, and a number of other maternal stressors including age and BMI [9,218]. Low birth weight and preterm birth are related and known to result from risky pregnancies, so it is logical that they are all statistically significant in relation to decreased cognitive ability [219]. The most surprising result we observed within these groups was the high statistical significance that sex had in the RMA analysis (OR = 1.05, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1). Further investigation revealed that cognitive development is linked to sex hormone production and maturation rate. Since the original studies examined males and females of the same age, the additional dimension of this stressor may not have been captured in those studies and may have led to the drastic difference between the sexes in this analysis [220,221,222].
When all birth outcome stressors were examined together, they were observed to decrease the likelihood of influencing childhood cognitive development (OR = 0.91, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1, Figure 2). Gestational age was the only stressor that was observed to increase the likelihood of influencing childhood cognitive development (OR = 1.11, p ≤ 0.001). Both preterm birth (OR = 0.78 p ≤ 0.001), and parity (OR = 0.89, p ≤ 0.001) were observed to decrease the likelihood of influencing cognition. The decreasing influence of preterm birth was unexpected, particularly since related birth characteristics were observed to be increasingly influential. This observation may be due to the gestational timing of neural connections in the developing brain. Beginning at 20 weeks into pregnancy the neural circuitry begins to form and by the end of 35 weeks the most pertinent pieces of the neural network are in place. The transition through week 40 may not be relevant to whether the child is still in gestation or has been born, as many of these connections are related to sensory driven activity [223]. Delivery method (OR = 0.98) and multiple births (OR = 0.86) were not observed to be statistically significant and these stressors may be of less concern to cognitive development.

3.2. Childhood Exposures

Stressors included in the childhood exposure analysis are those likely to occur and be measured/reported during the childhood life stages (i.e., birth, infancy, toddler), specifically activities and behaviors and social and chemical stressors [17]. The childhood stressor results are presented longitudinally and begin where the prenatal stressors end. First, chemical stressors that children can be exposed to throughout all life stages are examined. Then, activities and behaviors that begin during birth and infancy stages are examined. Lastly, social stressors that arise during the toddler stage, including childcare stressors, are presented.

3.2.1. Chemical Stressors

The chemical stressors examined in this analysis included 41 references with 110 sampling events [45,95,100,103,106,113,116,120,121,123,132,133,136,138,139,148,149,150,155,156,157,186,200,207,211,212,213,224,225,226,227,228,229,230,231,232,233,234,235,236,237,238]. The chemical stressor group included toxic elements (58 sampling events), endocrine-active compounds (27 sampling events), and toxic gases (25 sampling events). The toxic element stressors included lead ((Pb), 23 sampling events), mercury ((Hg), 16 sampling events), arsenic ((As), 11 sampling events), manganese ((Mn), five sampling events), and fluoride ((Fl), three sampling events). The toxic gases stressors were environmental cigarette/tobacco smoke ((ETS), 16 sampling events), polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons ((PAHs), six sampling events), and nitrogen dioxide ((NO2), three sampling events). The endocrine-active compounds included chlorinated chemicals ((polychlorinated biphenyls, PCBs; hexachlorobenzene, HCB; trichloropyridinol, TCPy); 11 sampling events), pesticides ((pyrethroids, Mirex, organophosphates); 10 sampling events), and others ((polybrominated diphenyl ethers, PBDEs; phthalates); six sampling events). Collectively, the chemical stressors were observed to have a 5% increase of influence on childhood cognitive ability (OR = 1.05, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1, Figure 3).

Toxic Elements

The toxic element stressors were observed to have a 4% increase in the likelihood of impacting childhood cognitive ability when examined together (OR = 1.04, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1). Heavy metal exposures (Hg, Pb, As, and cadmium (Cd)) are known risk factors for cognitive dysfunction as they interfere with neurotransmitter receptors in the brain [239]. When examined individually, Hg resulted in a 10% increase in impacting childhood cognition (OR = 1.10, p ≤ 0.001), which is not unexpected as Hg is a known neurotoxicant. Lead was also observed to have a slight (3%) increase in impact to cognition (OR = 1.03, p ≤ 0.001), and has been widely described to cause cognitive delays [240]. Fluoride was observed to have the greatest increase in impacting cognitive ability (OR = 1.40, p ≤ 0.05) and it is often reported to affect memory and cause cognitive deficits [241]. Both Mn (OR = 0.90, p ≤ 0.05), and As (OR = 0.95, p ≤ 0.01) were observed to decrease the likelihood of influencing childhood cognition (10% and 5%, respectively). While both Mn and As have been associated with cognitive impairment, the studies we examined in this analysis reported the Mn and As concentrations from water samples, while the other toxic elements were reported in biological samples [156,210,234,238,242]. These results highlight the discrepancies that can occur between different sampling methods and support the idea that the most direct method of sampling, in this case biological fluid samples as opposed to drinking water samples, will provide the most accurate measures for comparison.

Toxic Gases

When examined collectively in the RMA, the toxic gases were observed to have a 6% increase in likelihood of impacting childhood cognitive abilities (OR = 1.06, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1). When PAHs were examined, they had a 35% increase in their likelihood to influence childhood cognitive ability compared with when the toxic gases were not encountered (OR = 1.35, p ≤ 0.001). ETS was observed to have a 3% decrease in the likelihood of affecting childhood cognitive ability (OR = 0.97, p ≤ 0.001). The relationship between exposure to PAHs and neurodevelopment, cognitive development, and learning abilities has been described in a variety of epidemiological studies [8,243,244]. As PAHs comprise part of the chemical mixture of ETS, similar inverse relationships between exposure and cognition have been reported [245]. As both PAHs and ETS are widespread, more epidemiological data needs to be collected to elucidate causative relationships.

Endocrine-Active Stressors

The endocrine-active stressors resulted in a 1% decrease in their impact on childhood cognitive ability when analyzed together in the RMA (OR = 0.99, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1). When analyzed separately, only the pesticides had a significant increase in their effect on childhood cognition (OR = 1.05, p ≤ 0.001). Both the chlorinated compounds (OR = 0.97 p ≤ 0.01) and the PBDE/phthalate (OR = 0.92, p ≤ 0.05) groups resulted in decreases in the likelihood of an impact. The relationship between the endocrine system and cognitive ability has been well documented including the various modes of action through the endocrine system [246,247]. Interestingly, recent studies have reported that prenatal rather than childhood exposure to endocrine active compounds has a greater effect on childhood behavior [248,249,250]. The differences observed between the classes of endocrine-active chemicals in this work, as well as the differences reported between prenatal and childhood exposures, highlight the need for further examination of the specific relationships and modes of action that are relevant to childhood cognitive ability.

3.2.2. Activities and Behaviors

There were 41 references containing 128 sampling events included in the analysis of activities and behaviors [13,30,36,79,97,98,99,120,126,128,136,140,147,148,149,150,155,159,171,172,183,211,251,252,253,254,255,256,257,258,259,260,261,262,263,264,265,266,267,268,269,270]. The groups were breastfeeding behavior (63 sampling events), diet patterns (39 sampling events), and sleep characteristics (26 sampling events). The stressors included in breastfeeding behavior were duration of breastfeeding (44 sampling events), the presence of the long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LC-PUFAs) commonly associated with breastfeeding (13 sampling events), and if breastfeeding was conducted at all (yes/no; six sampling events). The dietary group included amount of fish consumed (16 sampling events), dietary pattern (10 sampling events), childhood BMI (eight sampling events), and folic acid supplementation (five sampling events). The sleep characteristics included were sleep disordered breathing (16 sampling events), sleep duration (six sampling events), and snoring behavior (four sampling events). When all activity and behavior stressors were analyzed collectively at the factor-level, we observed a 5% increase in the likelihood of effect on childhood cognitive ability (Table 1, Figure 4). The individual sleep stressors are discussed in the supplemental text.

Breastfeeding

When all breastfeeding stressors were examined together, there was a 16% increase in likelihood of effect on childhood cognitive ability (OR = 1.16, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1, Figure 4). When the stressors were examined individually, the LC-PUFAs were observed to have the greatest effect with a 20% increase (OR = 1.20, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1). If breastfeeding occurred at all was the next most impactful stressor with a 16% increase in effect on cognitive ability (OR = 1.16, p ≤ 0.001). Duration of breastfeeding was observed to have a 15% increase in effect on childhood cognitive ability (OR= 1.15, p ≤ 0.001). The relationship between these three stressors and childhood cognitive ability is in agreement with what has been reported in the literature as breastfeeding provides vitamins and nutrients necessary for healthy development [271,272]. Interestingly, when LC-PUFAs were supplemented into a formula-based diet, no improvement in general cognitive ability in infants was reported, but when specific cognitive behaviors were assessed, the supplemented children had improved visual attention and problem solving skills [273]. Children that received supplemented formula were also faster at decision-making by age 6 years than those that received non-supplemented formula [274,275]. Our results support the positive relationship between LC-PUFAs and general cognitive ability in childhood that has been previously documented in the peer-reviewed literature.

Diet

Dietary stressors included BMI, fish intake, folic acid, and diet pattern. When all dietary stressors were analyzed in the RMA, a 5% increase in the likelihood to effect childhood cognitive ability was observed (OR = 1.05, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1, Figure 4). When each stressor was examined separately, childhood BMI had a 14% increase in influencing cognitive ability (OR = 1.14, p ≤ 0.001). Dietary pattern (OR = 1.03, p ≤ 0.001), folic acid supplementation (OR = 1.04, p ≤ 0.05), and fish intake (OR = 1.01, p ≤ 0.01) all had significant increases in their likelihood to influence childhood cognitive ability. These stressors are components of a healthy diet, which influences BMI. Having a healthy diet that includes the proper amounts of vitamins and minerals has been shown to be important for childhood cognitive development [276]. The relationship between BMI and cognition is likely related to the dietary nutrients that are part of a healthy diet and missing from diets that include many processed foods, sodium, and fats [276]. However, the relationship between diet and BMI in early childhood is not clear, so future research could focus on this area to better understand childhood cognitive ability [277,278].

3.2.3. Social Factors

The analysis of social stressors was composed of 33 references that included 125 sampling events related to childhood cognitive ability [29,80,83,94,98,101,120,122,126,127,132,136,137,139,142,150,152,153,155,158,173,176,181,189,210,211,256,278,279,280,281,282,283]. The individual stressors examined within the social stressors factor group were social interactions (57 sampling events), home/family lifestyle (57 sampling events), and childcare (14 sampling events; Table 1). The social interaction subgroup included the stressors: parent/child interaction (24 sampling events), childhood trauma (21 sampling events), and maternal sensitivity (11 sampling events). The home/family lifestyle subgroup included maternal marital status (14 sampling events), home location (e.g., rural, urban, metro; 13 sampling events), family stability (11 sampling events), relationship of the child to their caregiver (nine sampling events), and number of siblings a child has (eight sampling events). The childcare subgroup included quality of care (five sampling events), child’s time in care (five sampling events), and attendance at care (four sampling events). When all social stressors were examined together, a 7% increase in impact to childhood cognitive ability was observed (OR = 1.07, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1, Figure 5). The individual home/family lifestyle stressors are discussed in the supplemental text.

Social Interactions

When the social interaction stressors were analyzed together in the RMA, a 14% increase in the influence on childhood cognitive ability was observed (OR = 1.14, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1, Figure 5). Upon individual analysis, maternal sensitivity (OR = 1.29, p ≤ 0.001) and parent/child interaction (OR = 1.25, p ≤ 0.001) were observed to significantly increase the influence on childhood cognitive ability. Maternal sensitivity resulted in a 29% increase and parental interaction resulted in a 25% increase (Table 1). The large increase in the percent impact highlights the importance of each stressor in influencing childhood cognitive ability. Childhood traumatic experiences were observed to have a significant decrease in influencing cognition (OR = 0.93, p ≤ 0.001). The low likelihood of a relationship between childhood traumatic experiences and cognition was unexpected as it has been previously observed to be highly correlated to childhood mental health outcomes [169]. The notion that social interaction is related to childhood cognitive development has been well explored, but specific causative measures have been challenging to identify [27,284,285]. Our reported dichotomy between the impacts of maternal emotionality/parental interaction and childhood trauma on childhood cognitive ability may further elucidate the complex interactions between social stressors and cognition.

Childcare

When analyzed collectively in the RMA, childcare stressors were observed to have a 15% increase in their likelihood of influence on childhood cognitive ability (OR = 1.15, p ≤ 0.001; Table 1, Figure 5). Individual stressors included in the childcare group were also observed to have statistically significant increases in their likelihood of impact on childhood cognitive ability, but to varying percentages. The quality of childcare had the greatest likelihood of influence at 25% (OR = 1.25, p ≤ 0.001), time in childcare was second at 13% (OR = 1.13, p ≤ 0.01), while attendance was observed to be 7% (OR = 1.07, p ≤ 0.05). Several studies have shown that childcare quality is related to cognitive ability, with significant implications related to the quality of the environment (e.g., mentally stimulating, conducive for learning, comfortable), social interactions the child has with others, and the relationship the child has with the teacher or caregiver [27,286,287,288]. Childcare has been previously reported to be protective of cognitive ability in children that are also exposed to negative stressors (e.g., domestic violence, abuse) [289]. Relationships present in the literature also provide support for the percentages of influence observed in this study; the most significant stressor is the quality of care, followed by the time a child spends in the care environment, then followed by their general attendance at the childcare facility (Table 1, Figure 5).

Stressors Influencing Childhood Cognitive Ability

Our analysis showed that many individual stressors had a small impact on childhood cognitive ability, but the collective impact of all chemical and non-chemical stressors experienced by children is not well understood, especially as it relates to cognitive ability. When all stressors were analyzed collectively, the result is a 4% increase in the likelihood of an influence on childhood cognition (OR = 1.04, p ≤ 0.001). When the five factor RMA results are compared, we observe a wide range of influence on childhood cognitive ability (Figure 6).
Our goal was to identify key stressors from the review conducted by Ruiz et al. [48]. To that end, this work identified 23 stressors that had a significant increase in their likelihood to influence childhood cognitive ability (by 10% or more) and 75 stressors that were statistically significant (Table 1).
The prenatal exposures observed to have the greatest impact of on childhood cognitive ability were maternal stressors related to the availability of information: education, income, and technology access. The inherent stressors with the greatest impact were those determined by the pregnancy environment and maternal behavior during pregnancy: head size, body length, and birth weight.
The childhood exposures observed to be most influential to cognitive ability were nutrition during infancy and throughout childhood, parental interaction, childcare, and toxic pollutant exposures (Table 1). Interestingly, each of these childhood stressors could also be attributed to maternal activities and behaviors (i.e., breastfeeding, childhood BMI, income), and are modifiable factors that can be changed by altered parental behavior. Our results highlight the importance of prenatal health, particularly how various stressors could impact a woman’s health during pregnancy, her activities and behaviors, and result in adversity for the child.

Limitations

A limitation of this work is that the meta-analysis included such diverse studies that specific “positive/negative” relationships could not be reported with certainty, only the “likelihood of impact”. While this provided the opportunity to include a wide array of primary research, additional research is required to elucidate the positive/negative effects of specific stressors on childhood cognitive ability.
The quality of a meta-analysis and the resulting conclusions are only as reliable as the included data. The results reported herein are the synthesis of high-quality observational and randomized control trial studies that met rigorous inclusion criteria. Specifically, that general cognitive outcome was measured using well established evidence-based assessments and expressed as a continuous variable or categorized as below average or significant cognitive delay (i.e., >1 or >2 standard deviations (SD) below the mean) [19]. Additionally, the majority of study participants were healthy children without any existing developmental disabilities, neonatal morbidities, or pathologies associated with cognitive deficits [48]. The most standardized and/or corrected data was extracted from the included references so that the data used in the meta-analysis would be as similar as possible. Variations in study context, goals, and inherent confounding factors were accounted for as much as possible using the random-effects meta-analysis model, and our language (i.e., likelihood of impact on childhood cognition) was chosen so as not to overstate the results of the analyses herein.
The challenge in pooling studies for meta-analyses lies in combining studies with differences in exposure assessment. Obtaining an agreed upon index of exposure to establish dose–response associations and a consensus in methodologies has proven difficult thus far. However, a consistent metric of exposure could lead to better understanding of the association between exposure and effect and increased certainty of effects on cognitive development. Establishing uniform metrics would enable the results of studies like those included here, and many others, to have a greater influence on risk management and policy making.
It should be noted that the studies included in this meta-analysis were primarily based in the United States (US), used data from primarily healthy children, and were from English speaking families. Due to these characteristics, extrapolating the results discussed herein to other areas of the world with chronic/ongoing stressors (i.e., chronic malnutrition, war, and other crises leading to the disruption of normal communities) may be challenging. The results discussing bilingualism and cognitive ability were also presented through an US-focused lens, where bilingualism is not as prevalent as other areas of the world. The global approach to multilingualism is quite different than the approach in the US; many countries have bilingualism as an inherent part of society, whereas the US does not, and this may have influenced our results. A greater understanding of the relationship between bilingualism and cognitive ability could be ascertained using global data.

4. Conclusions

Our goal was to determine if any key factors related to childhood cognitive ability could be identified from the stressors identified by Ruiz et al. [48]. Using the child health and well-being conceptual framework as a guide to life stage and stressor inter-relationships, this work identified key stressors in both the prenatal and early childhood life stages [17]. These stressors were maternal health and the mother’s ability to access information relevant to a healthy pregnancy (for the prenatal period), dietary nutrients (infancy), quality of social interaction (toddler), and exposure to toxic substances (throughout early childhood). In doing this analysis, we examined the relative impact of real-world exposures on cognitive development and have begun to understand the inter-relationships between exposures to chemical and non-chemical stressors and early childhood health outcomes.
Through this work we have elucidated the tremendous impact of maternal factors on childhood cognitive ability. The most significant maternal factors appear to be related to information accessibility and to maternal choices made during pregnancy. The child-specific inherent characteristics that we observed to be most impactful in influencing cognition are uncontrollable for the child but are at least partially controllable for the pregnant woman. Based on the data examined here, the more desirable behavioral, social, and chemical exposure factors may provide enhanced childhood cognitive abilities, and that quality may be related to maternal access to relevant information. As such, using available information regarding a healthy, full-term pregnancy, and making the recommended behavioral changes may improve cognitive outcomes related to exposures to prenatal stressors.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/17/15/5451/s1.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.D.C.R. and N.S.T.; methodology, J.D.C.R. and F.M.N.; software, F.M.N.; analysis, F.M.N.; resources, N.S.T.; data curation, J.D.C.R. and F.M.N.; writing—original draft preparation, F.M.N.; writing—review and editing, F.M.N. and N.S.T.; visualization, F.M.N.; supervision, N.S.T.; project administration, N.S.T.; funding acquisition, N.S.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project was supported by an appointment to the Internship/Research Participation Program at the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Office of Research and Development, National Exposure Research Laboratory, administered by the Oak Ridge Institute for Science and Education (ORISE) through an interagency agreement between the U.S. Department of Energy and EPA. It has been subjected to Agency administrative review and approved for publication. Mention of trade names or commercial products does not constitute endorsement or recommendation for use.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Plomin, R. Genetics and general cognitive ability. Nature 1999, 402, C25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Daly, M.; Egan, M.; O’Reilly, F. Childhood general cognitive ability predicts leadership role occupancy across life: Evidence from 17,000 cohort study participants. Leadersh. Q. 2015, 26, 323–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Judge, T.A.; Higgins, C.A.; Thoresen, C.J.; Barrick, M.R. The big five personality traits, general mental ability, and career success across the life span. Pers. Psychol. 1999, 52, 621–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Collins, W. Development during Middle Childhood: The Years from Six to Twelve; National Academy of Sciences Press: Washington, WA, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  5. Investigators, M.-E.N. Early childhood cognitive development is affected by interactions among illness, diet, enteropathogens and the home environment: Findings from the MAL-ED birth cohort study. BMJ Glob. Health 2018, 3, e000752. [Google Scholar]
  6. Zielinska, M.A.; Hamulka, J.; Grabowicz-Chądrzyńska, I.; Bryś, J.; Wesolowska, A. Association between Breastmilk LC PUFA, Carotenoids and Psychomotor Development of Exclusively Breastfed Infants. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Figlio, D.N.; Freese, J.; Karbownik, K.; Roth, J. Socioeconomic status and genetic influences on cognitive development. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, 13441–13446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Annavarapu, R.N.; Kathi, S. Cognitive disorders in children associated with urban vehicular emissions. Environ. Pollut 2016, 208, 74–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Jukic, A.M.; Baird, D.D.; Weinberg, C.R.; McConnaughey, D.R.; Wilcox, A.J. Length of human pregnancy and contributors to its natural variation. Hum. Reprod 2013, 28, 2848–2855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Eskenazi, B.; Chevrier, J.; Rauch, S.A.; Kogut, K.; Harley, K.G.; Johnson, C.; Trujillo, C.; Sjodin, A.; Bradman, A. In utero and childhood polybrominated diphenyl ether (PBDE) exposures and neurodevelopment in the CHAMACOS study. Environ. Health Perspect. 2013, 121, 257–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Tucker-Drob, E.M.; Harden, K.P. Early childhood cognitive development and parental cognitive stimulation: Evidence for reciprocal gene–environment transactions. Dev. Sci. 2012, 15, 250–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Yang, S.; Platt, R.W.; Kramer, M.S. Variation in child cognitive ability by week of gestation among healthy term births. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2010, 171, 399–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Bourke, R.; Anderson, V.; Yang, J.S.; Jackman, A.R.; Killedar, A.; Nixon, G.M.; Davey, M.J.; Walker, A.M.; Trinder, J.; Horne, R.S. Cognitive and academic functions are impaired in children with all severities of sleep-disordered breathing. Sleep Med. 2011, 12, 489–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Kennedy, J.D.; Blunden, S.; Hirte, C.; Parsons, D.W.; Martin, A.J.; Crowe, E.; Williams, D.; Pamula, Y.; Lushington, K. Reduced neurocognition in children who snore. Pediatr. Pulmonol. 2004, 37, 330–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Bellinger, D.C. A strategy for comparing the contributions of environmental chemicals and other risk factors to neurodevelopment of children. Environ. Health Perspect. 2011, 120, 501–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Sexton, K.; Hattis, D. Assessing cumulative health risks from exposure to environmental mixtures—Three fundamental questions. Environ. Health Perspect. 2007, 115, 825–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Tulve, N.; Ruiz, J.D.C.; Lichtveld, K.; Darney, S.; Quakenboss, J. Development of a Conceptual Framework Depicting a Childs Total (Built, Natural, Social) Environment in Order to Optimize Health and Well-Being. Ommega Int. 2016, 2, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Liu, J.; Lewis, G. Environmental toxicity and poor cognitive outcomes in children and adults. J. Environ. Health 2014, 76, 130. [Google Scholar]
  19. Ferguson, K.T.; Cassells, R.C.; MacAllister, J.W.; Evans, G.W. The physical environment and child development: An international review. Int. J. Psychol. 2013, 48, 437–468. [Google Scholar]
  20. Walker, S.P.; Wachs, T.D.; Grantham-McGregor, S.; Black, M.M.; Nelson, C.A.; Huffman, S.L.; Baker-Henningham, H.; Chang, S.M.; Hamadani, J.D.; Lozoff, B. Inequality in early childhood: Risk and protective factors for early child development. Lancet 2011, 378, 1325–1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Casey, B.; Galvan, A.; Hare, T.A. Changes in cerebral functional organization during cognitive development. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2005, 15, 239–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Aschner, M.; Aschner, J.L. Mercury neurotoxicity: Mechanisms of blood-brain barrier transport. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1990, 14, 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Julvez, J.; Debes, F.; Weihe, P.; Choi, A.L.; Grandjean, P. Thyroid dysfunction as a mediator of organochlorine neurotoxicity in preschool children. Environ. Health Perspect. 2011, 119, 1429–1435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Adams, J.; Barone, S., Jr.; LaMantia, A.; Philen, R.; Rice, D.; Spear, L.; Susser, E. Workshop to identify critical windows of exposure for children’s health: Neurobehavioral work group summary. Environ. Health Perspect. 2000, 108, 535–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Fudvoye, J.; Bourguignon, J.-P.; Parent, A.-S. Endocrine-disrupting chemicals and human growth and maturation: A focus on early critical windows of exposure. In Vitamins & Hormones; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014; Volume 94, pp. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  26. Choi, A.L.; Zhang, Y.; Sun, G.; Bellinger, D.C.; Wang, K.; Yang, X.J.; Li, J.S.; Zheng, Q.; Fu, Y.; Grandjean, P. Association of lifetime exposure to fluoride and cognitive functions in Chinese children: A pilot study. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2015, 47, 96–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Wegerif, R.; Mercer, N.; Dawes, L. From social interaction to individual reasoning: An empirical investigation of a possible socio-cultural model of cognitive development. Learn. Instr. 1999, 9, 493–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. St John, A.M.; Tarullo, A.R. Neighbourhood chaos moderates the association of socioeconomic status and child executive functioning. Infant Child Dev. 2019, e2153. [Google Scholar]
  29. Koutra, K.; Chatzi, L.; Roumeliotaki, T.; Vassilaki, M.; Giannakopoulou, E.; Batsos, C.; Koutis, A.; Kogevinas, M. Socio-demographic determinants of infant neurodevelopment at 18 months of age: Mother-Child Cohort (Rhea Study) in Crete, Greece. Infant Behav. Dev. 2012, 35, 48–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Buckhalt, J.A.; El-Sheikh, M.; Keller, P. Children’s sleep and cognitive functioning: Race and socioeconomic status as moderators of effects. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 213–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Tong, S.; Baghurst, P.; Vimpani, G.; McMichael, A. Socioeconomic position, maternal IQ, home environment, and cognitive development. J. Pediatr. 2007, 151, 284–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Benton, D. The influence of dietary status on the cognitive performance of children. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 2010, 54, 457–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Bellisle, F. Effects of diet on behaviour and cognition in children. Br. J. Nutr. 2004, 92, S227–S232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Ednick, M.; Cohen, A.P.; McPhail, G.L.; Beebe, D.; Simakajornboon, N.; Amin, R.S. A review of the effects of sleep during the first year of life on cognitive, psychomotor, and temperament development. Sleep 2009, 32, 1449–1458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Gertner, S.; Greenbaum, C.W.; Sadeh, A.; Dolfin, Z.; Sirota, L.; Ben-Nun, Y. Sleep–wake patterns in preterm infants and 6 month’s home environment: Implications for early cognitive development. Early Hum. Dev. 2002, 68, 93–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Gruber, R.; Laviolette, R.; Deluca, P.; Monson, E.; Cornish, K.; Carrier, J. Short sleep duration is associated with poor performance on IQ measures in healthy school-age children. Sleep Med. 2010, 11, 289–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Calhoun, S.L.; Fernandez-Mendoza, J.; Vgontzas, A.N.; Mayes, S.D.; Tsaoussoglou, M.; Rodriguez-Munoz, A.; Bixler, E.O. Learning, attention/hyperactivity, and conduct problems as sequelae of excessive daytime sleepiness in a general population study of young children. Sleep 2012, 35, 627–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Haworth, C.M.; Wright, M.J.; Luciano, M.; Martin, N.G.; de Geus, E.J.; van Beijsterveldt, C.E.; Bartels, M.; Posthuma, D.; Boomsma, D.; Davis, O. The heritability of general cognitive ability increases linearly from childhood to young adulthood. Mol. Psychiatry 2010, 15, 1112–1120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Yumoto, C.; Jacobson, S.W.; Jacobson, J.L. Fetal substance exposure and cumulative environmental risk in an African American cohort. Child Dev. 2008, 79, 1761–1776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Virgolini, M.; Rossi-George, A.; Lisek, R.; Weston, D.; Thiruchelvam, M.; Cory-Slechta, D. CNS effects of developmental Pb exposure are enhanced by combined maternal and offspring stress. Neurotoxicology 2008, 29, 812–827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Weiss, B.; Bellinger, D.C. Social ecology of children’s vulnerability to environmental pollutants. Environ. Health Perspect. 2006, 114, 1479–1485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Solon, O.; Riddell, T.J.; Quimbo, S.A.; Butrick, E.; Aylward, G.P.; Bacate, M.L.; Peabody, J.W. Associations between cognitive function, blood lead concentration, and nutrition among children in the central Philippines. J. Pediatr. 2008, 152, 237–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Reardon, S.F. The widening academic achievement gap between the rich and the poor: New evidence and possible explanations. Whither Oppor. 2011, 1, 91–116. [Google Scholar]
  44. Perera, F.P.; Wang, S.; Rauh, V.; Zhou, H.; Stigter, L.; Camann, D.; Jedrychowski, W.; Mroz, E.; Majewska, R. Prenatal exposure to air pollution, maternal psychological distress, and child behavior. Pediatrics 2013, 132, e1284–e1294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Horton, M.K.; Kahn, L.G.; Perera, F.; Barr, D.B.; Rauh, V. Does the home environment and the sex of the child modify the adverse effects of prenatal exposure to chlorpyrifos on child working memory? Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2012, 34, 534–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Huitt, W.; Hummel, J. Piaget’s theory of cognitive development. Educ. Psychol. Interact. 2003, 3, 1–5. [Google Scholar]
  47. Piaget, J. Part I: Cognitive development in children: Piaget development and learning. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1964, 2, 176–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Ruiz, J.D.C.; Quackenboss, J.J.; Tulve, N.S. Contributions of a child’s built, natural, and social environments to their general cognitive ability: A systematic scoping review. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0147741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Ard, K.; Colen, C.; Becerra, M.; Velez, T. Two mechanisms: The role of social capital and industrial pollution exposure in explaining racial disparities in self-rated health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Ferguson, A.; Solo-Gabriele, H. Children’s exposure to environmental contaminants: An editorial reflection of articles in the IJERPH special issue entitled,“children’s exposure to environmental contaminants”. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 1117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Rappazzo, K.; Coffman, E.; Hines, E. Exposure to perfluorinated alkyl substances and health outcomes in children: A systematic review of the epidemiologic literature. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Jensen, S.K.; Berens, A.E.; Nelson, C.A., 3rd. Effects of poverty on interacting biological systems underlying child development. Lancet Child. Adolesc. Health 2017, 1, 225–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Coxon, T.; Goldstein, L.; Odhiambo, B. Analysis of spatial distribution of trace metals, PCB, and PAH and their potential impact on human health in Virginian Counties and independent cities, USA. Environ. Geochem. Health 2019, 41, 783–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Matonda-Ma-Nzuzi, T.; Mampunza Ma Miezi, S.; Mpembi, M.N.; Mvumbi, D.M.; Aloni, M.N.; Malendakana, F.; Mpaka Mbeya, D.; Lelo, G.M.; Charlier-Mikolajczak, D. Factors associated with behavioral problems and cognitive impairment in children with epilepsy of Kinshasa, Democratic Republic of the Congo. Epilepsy Behav. 2018, 78, 78–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Buck, K.D.; Kevin Summers, J.; Smith, L.M.; Harwell, L.C. Application of the Human Well-Being Index to Sensitive Population Divisions: A Children’s Well-Being Index Development. Child Indic. Res. 2018, 11, 1249–1280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Stalgaitis, C.A.; Wagner, D.E.; Djakaria, M.; Jordan, J.W. Understanding Adversity and Peer Crowds to Prevent Youth Health Risks. Am. J. Health Behav. 2019, 43, 767–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Liu, Y.; Kaaya, S.; Chai, J.; McCoy, D.; Surkan, P.; Black, M.; Sutter-Dallay, A.-L.; Verdoux, H.; Smith-Fawzi, M. Maternal depressive symptoms and early childhood cognitive development: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 2017, 47, 680–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Britto, P.R.; Lye, S.J.; Proulx, K.; Yousafzai, A.K.; Matthews, S.G.; Vaivada, T.; Perez-Escamilla, R.; Rao, N.; Ip, P.; Fernald, L.C. Nurturing care: Promoting early childhood development. Lancet 2017, 389, 91–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Deoni, S.C.; O’Muircheartaigh, J.; Elison, J.T.; Walker, L.; Doernberg, E.; Waskiewicz, N.; Dirks, H.; Piryatinsky, I.; Dean, D.C.; Jumbe, N. White matter maturation profiles through early childhood predict general cognitive ability. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016, 221, 1189–1203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Klingberg, T. Childhood cognitive development as a skill. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014, 18, 573–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.V.; Higgins, J.P.; Rothstein, H.R. Introduction to Meta-Analysis; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  62. Wickham, H.; Wickham, M.H. Package Tidyverse. Available online: http://tidyverse.tidyverse.org (accessed on 20 July 2020).
  63. Viechtbauer, W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw. 2010, 36, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Wickham, H. Ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  65. Team, R. RStudio: Integrated Development for R; RStudio, Inc.: Boston, MA, USA, 2015; Volume 42, p. 14. Available online: http://www.rstudio.com (accessed on 29 April 2018).
  66. Xie, Y.; Allaire, J.J.; Grolemund, G. R Markdown: The Definitive Guide; Chapman and Hall/CRC: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  67. Rmarkdown: Dynamic Documents for R, R Package Version 1.14. 2019. Available online: https://github.com/rstudio/rmarkdown (accessed on 20 July 2020).
  68. Vasdekis, V.G.; Vlachonikolis, I.G. On the difference between ML and REML estimators in the modelling of multivariate longitudinal data. J. Stat. Plan. Inference 2005, 134, 194–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.; Rothstein, H. Meta-analysis: Fixed effect vs random effects. Meta-Anal. Com 2007, 16, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  70. Szumilas, M. Explaining odds ratios. J. Can. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2010, 19, 227. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  71. Thabane, L.; Mbuagbaw, L.; Zhang, S.; Samaan, Z.; Marcucci, M.; Ye, C.; Thabane, M.; Giangregorio, L.; Dennis, B.; Kosa, D.; et al. A tutorial on sensitivity analyses in clinical trials: The what, why, when and how. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2013, 13, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Sutton, A.J.; Duval, S.J.; Tweedie, R.L.; Abrams, K.R.; Jones, D.R. Empirical assessment of effect of publication bias on meta-analyses. BMJ 2000, 320, 1574–1577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Peters, J.L.; Sutton, A.J.; Jones, D.R.; Abrams, K.R.; Rushton, L. Comparison of two methods to detect publication bias in meta-analysis. JAMA 2006, 295, 676–680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Macaskill, P.; Walter, S.D.; Irwig, L. A comparison of methods to detect publication bias in meta-analysis. Stat. Med. 2001, 20, 641–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Lau, J.; Ioannidis, J.P.A.; Terrin, N.; Schmid, C.H.; Olkin, I. The case of the misleading funnel plot. BMJ 2006, 333, 597–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Egger, M.; Smith, G.D.; Schneider, M.; Minder, C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997, 315, 629–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Alati, R.; MacLeod, J.; Hickman, M.; Sayal, K.; May, M.; Smith, G.D.; Lawlor, D.A. Intrauterine Exposure to Alcohol and Tobacco Use and Childhood IQ: Findings from a Parental-Offspring Comparison within the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatr. Res. 2008, 64, 659–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Arendt, R.; Short, E.; Minnes, S.; Hewitt, J.; Flynn, S.; Carlson, L.; Min, M.; Klein, N.; Flannery, D. Children prenatally exposed to cocaine: Developmental outcomes and environmental risks at seven years of age. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2004, 25, 83–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Bahena-Medina, L.A.; Torres-Sanchez, L.; Schnaas, L.; Cebrian, M.E.; Chavez, C.H.; Osorio-Valencia, E.; Hernandez, R.M.G.; Lopez-Carrillo, L. Neonatal neurodevelopment and prenatal exposure to dichlorodiphenyldichloroethylene (DDE): A cohort study in Mexico. J. Expos. Sci. Environ. Epidemiol. 2011, 21, 609–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Banerjee, P.N.; Tamis-LeMonda, C.S. Infants’ persistence and mothers’ teaching as predictors of toddlers’ cognitive development. Infant Behav. Dev. 2007, 30, 479–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Bennett, D.S.; Bendersky, M.; Lewis, M. Children’s cognitive ability from 4 to 9 years old as a function of prenatal cocaine exposure, environmental risk, and maternal verbal intelligence. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 44, 919–928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  82. Bergman, K.; Sarkar, P.; Glover, V.; O’Connor, T.G. Maternal Prenatal Cortisol and Infant Cognitive Development: Moderation by Infant–Mother Attachment. Biol. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 1026–1032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  83. Busch, A.L.; Lieberman, A.F. Mothers’ Adult Attachment Interview ratings predict preschool children’s IQ following domestic violence exposure. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2010, 12, 505–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Canfield, R.L.; Henderson, C.R.J.; Cory-Slechta, D.A.; Cox, C.; Jusko, T.A.; Lanphear, B.P. Intellectual impairment in children with blood lead concentrations below 10 microg per deciliter. N. Engl. J. Med. 2003, 348, 1517–1526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Chiodo, L.M.; Jacobson, S.W.; Jacobson, J.L. Neurodevelopmental effects of postnatal lead exposure at very low levels. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2004, 26, 359–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Craig, W.Y.; Allan, W.C.; Kloza, E.M.; Pulkkinen, A.J.; Waisbren, S.; Spratt, D.I.; Palomaki, G.E.; Neveux, L.M.; Haddow, J.E. Mid-gestational maternal free thyroxine concentration and offspring neurocognitive development at age two years. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2012, 97, E22–E28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Davis, E.P.; Sandman, C.A. The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Dev. 2010, 81, 131–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Deroma, L.; Parpinel, M.; Tognin, V.; Channoufi, L.; Tratnik, J.; Horvat, M.; Valent, F.; Barbone, F. Neuropsychological assessment at school-age and prenatal low-level exposure to mercury through fish consumption in an Italian birth cohort living near a contaminated site. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 2013, 216, 486–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Emond, A.M.; Blair, P.S.; Emmett, P.M.; Drewett, R.F. Weight faltering in infancy and IQ levels at 8 years in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatrics 2007, 120, e1051–e1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Evans, J.; Melotti, R.; Heron, J.; Ramchandani, P.; Wiles, N.; Murray, L.; Stein, A. The timing of maternal depressive symptoms and child cognitive development: A longitudinal study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2012, 53, 632–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  91. Falgreen Eriksen, H.L.; Mortensen, E.L.; Kilburn, T.; Underbjerg, M.; Bertrand, J.; Stovring, H.; Wimberley, T.; Grove, J.; Kesmodel, U.S. The effects of low to moderate prenatal alcohol exposure in early pregnancy on IQ in 5-year-old children. BJOG Int. J. Obstet. Gynaecol. 2012, 119, 1191–1200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  92. Falgreen Eriksen, H.L.; Kesmodel, U.S.; Wimberley, T.; Underbjerg, M.; Kilburn, T.R.; Mortensen, E.L. Effects of tobacco smoking in pregnancy on offspring intelligence at the age of 5. J. Pregnancy 2012, 2012, 945196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Forns, J.; Julvez, J.; Garcia-Esteban, R.; Guxens, M.; Ferrer, M.; Grellier, J.; Vrijheid, M.; Sunyer, J. Maternal intelligence-mental health and child neuropsychological development at age 14 months. Gac. Sanit. 2012, 26, 397–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Frank, D.A.; Rose-Jacobs, R.; Beeghly, M.; Wilbur, M.; Bellinger, D.; Cabral, H. Level of prenatal cocaine exposure and 48-month IQ: Importance of preschool enrichment. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2005, 27, 15–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Freire, C.; Ramos, R.; Puertas, R.; Lopez-Espinosa, M.J.; Julvez, J.; Aguilera, I.; Cruz, F.; Fernandez, M.F.; Sunyer, J.; Olea, N. Association of traffic-related air pollution with cognitive development in children. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2010, 64, 223–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Galbally, M.; Lewis, A.J.; Buist, A. Developmental outcomes of children exposed to antidepressants in pregnancy. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2011, 45, 393–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Gomez-Sanchiz, M.; Canete, R.; Rodero, I.; Baeza, J.E.; Gonzalez, J.A. Influence of breast-feeding and parental intelligence on cognitive development in the 24-month-old child. Clin. Pediatr. 2004, 43, 753–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Gomez-Sanchiz, M.; Canete, R.; Rodero, I.; Baeza, J.E.; Avila, O. Influence of breast-feeding on mental and psychomotor development. Clin. Pediatr. 2003, 42, 35–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Gustafsson, P.A.; Duchen, K.; Birberg, U.; Karlsson, T. Breastfeeding, very long polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFA) and IQ at 6 1/2 years of age. Acta Paediatr. 2004, 93, 1280–1287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Hamadani, J.D.; Grantham-McGregor, S.M.; Tofail, F.; Nermell, B.; Fangstrom, B.; Huda, S.N.; Yesmin, S.; Rahman, M.; Vera-Hernandez, M.; Arifeen, S.E.; et al. Pre- and postnatal arsenic exposure and child development at 18 months of age: A cohort study in rural Bangladesh. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2010, 39, 1206–1216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  101. Han, W.J. Maternal nonstandard work schedules and child cognitive outcomes. Child Dev. 2005, 76, 137–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Hinkle, S.N.; Schieve, L.A.; Stein, A.D.; Swan, D.W.; Ramakrishnan, U.; Sharma, A.J. Associations between maternal prepregnancy body mass index and child neurodevelopment at 2 years of age. Int. J. Obes. 2012, 36, 1312–1319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  103. Hu, H.; Tellez-Rojo, M.M.; Bellinger, D.; Smith, D.; Ettinger, A.S.; Lamadrid-Figueroa, H.; Schwartz, J.; Schnaas, L.; Mercado-Garcia, A.; Hernandez-Avila, M. Fetal lead exposure at each stage of pregnancy as a predictor of infant mental development. Environ. Health Perspect. 2006, 114, 1730–1735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Huang, P.C.; Su, P.H.; Chen, H.Y.; Huang, H.B.; Tsai, J.L.; Huang, H.I.; Wang, S.L. Childhood blood lead levels and intellectual development after ban of leaded gasoline in Taiwan: A 9-year prospective study. Environ. Int. 2012, 40, 88–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Jacobson, S.W.; Jacobson, J.L.; Sokol, R.J.; Chiodo, L.M.; Corobana, R. Maternal age, alcohol abuse history, and quality of parenting as moderators of the effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on 7.5-year intellectual function. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2004, 28, 1732–1745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Jedrychowski, W.; Perera, F.P.; Jankowski, J.; Mrozek-Budzyn, D.; Mroz, E.; Flak, E.; Edwards, S.; Skarupa, A.; Lisowska-Miszczyk, I. Very low prenatal exposure to lead and mental development of children in infancy and early childhood: Krakow prospective cohort study. Neuroepidemiology 2009, 32, 270–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Jedrychowski, W.; Perera, F.; Jankowski, J.; Butscher, M.; Mroz, E.; Flak, E.; Kaim, I.; Lisowska-Miszczyk, I.; Skarupa, A.; Sowa, A. Effect of exclusive breastfeeding on the development of children’s cognitive function in the Krakow prospective birth cohort study. Eur. J. Pediatr. 2012, 171, 151–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  108. Jedrychowski, W.; Maugeri, U.; Perera, F.; Stigter, L.; Jankowski, J.; Butscher, M.; Mroz, E.; Flak, E.; Skarupa, A.; Sowa, A. Cognitive function of 6-year old children exposed to mold-contaminated homes in early postnatal period. Prospective birth cohort study in Poland. Physiol. Behav. 2011, 104, 989–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  109. Julvez, J.; Alvarez-Pedrerol, M.; Rebagliato, M.; Murcia, M.; Forns, J.; Garcia-Esteban, R.; Lertxundi, N.; Espada, M.; Tardon, A.; Riano Galan, I.; et al. Thyroxine levels during pregnancy in healthy women and early child neurodevelopment. Epidemiology 2013, 24, 150–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Julvez, J.; Ribas-Fito, N.; Torrent, M.; Forns, M.; Garcia-Esteban, R.; Sunyer, J. Maternal smoking habits and cognitive development of children at age 4 years in a population-based birth cohort. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2007, 36, 825–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  111. Kesmodel, U.S.; Eriksen, H.L.; Underbjerg, M.; Kilburn, T.R.; Stovring, H.; Wimberley, T.; Mortensen, E.L. The effect of alcohol binge drinking in early pregnancy on general intelligence in children. BJOG Int. J. Obstet. Gynaecol. 2012, 119, 1222–1231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Kesmodel, U.S.; Bertrand, J.; Stovring, H.; Skarpness, B.; Denny, C.H.; Mortensen, E.L. The effect of different alcohol drinking patterns in early to mid pregnancy on the child’s intelligence, attention, and executive function. BJOG Int. J. Obstet. Gynaecol. 2012, 119, 1180–1190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  113. Kim, Y.; Kim, B.N.; Hong, Y.C.; Shin, M.S.; Yoo, H.J.; Kim, J.W.; Bhang, S.Y.; Cho, S.C. Co-exposure to environmental lead and manganese affects the intelligence of school-aged children. Neurotoxicology 2009, 30, 564–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Kolobe, T.H. Childrearing practices and developmental expectations for Mexican-American mothers and the developmental status of their infants. Phys. Ther. 2004, 84, 439–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Lanphear, B.P.; Hornung, R.; Khoury, J.; Yolton, K.; Baghurst, P.; Bellinger, D.C.; Canfield, R.L.; Dietrich, K.N.; Bornschein, R.; Greene, T.; et al. Low-level environmental lead exposure and children’s intellectual function: An international pooled analysis. Environ. Health Perspect. 2005, 113, 894–899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Lee, B.E.; Hong, Y.C.; Park, H.; Ha, M.; Kim, J.H.; Chang, N.; Roh, Y.M.; Kim, B.N.; Kim, Y.; Oh, S.Y.; et al. Secondhand smoke exposure during pregnancy and infantile neurodevelopment. Environ. Res. 2011, 111, 539–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Lewis, M.W.; Misra, S.; Johnson, H.L.; Rosen, T.S. Neurological and developmental outcomes of prenatally cocaine-exposed offspring from 12 to 36 months. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 2004, 30, 299–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Li, Y.; Shan, Z.; Teng, W.; Yu, X.; Fan, C.; Teng, X.; Guo, R.; Wang, H.; Li, J.; Chen, Y.; et al. Abnormalities of maternal thyroid function during pregnancy affect neuropsychological development of their children at 25–30 months. Clin. Endocrinol. 2010, 72, 825–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Li, X.; Atkins, M.S. Early childhood computer experience and cognitive and motor development. Pediatrics 2004, 113, 1715–1722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  120. Llop, S.; Guxens, M.; Murcia, M.; Lertxundi, A.; Ramon, R.; Riano, I.; Rebagliato, M.; Ibarluzea, J.; Tardon, A.; Sunyer, J.; et al. Prenatal exposure to mercury and infant neurodevelopment in a multicenter cohort in Spain: Study of potential modifiers. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2012, 175, 451–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  121. Lovasi, G.S.; Quinn, J.W.; Rauh, V.A.; Perera, F.P.; Andrews, H.F.; Garfinkel, R.; Hoepner, L.; Whyatt, R.; Rundle, A. Chlorpyrifos exposure and urban residential environment characteristics as determinants of early childhood neurodevelopment. Am. J. Public Health 2011, 101, 63–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Lugo-Gil, J.; Tamis-LeMonda, C.S. Family resources and parenting quality: Links to children’s cognitive development across the first 3 years. Child Dev. 2008, 79, 1065–1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Menezes-Filho, J.A.; Novaes Cde, O.; Moreira, J.C.; Sarcinelli, P.N.; Mergler, D. Elevated manganese and cognitive performance in school-aged children and their mothers. Environ. Res. 2011, 111, 156–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  124. Messinger, D.S.; Bauer, C.R.; Das, A.; Seifer, R.; Lester, B.M.; Lagasse, L.L.; Wright, L.L.; Shankaran, S.; Bada, H.S.; Smeriglio, V.L.; et al. The maternal lifestyle study: Cognitive, motor, and behavioral outcomes of cocaine-exposed and opiate-exposed infants through three years of age. Pediatrics 2004, 113, 1677–1685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  125. Morrow, C.E.; Culbertson, J.L.; Accornero, V.H.; Xue, L.; Anthony, J.C.; Bandstra, E.S. Learning disabilities and intellectual functioning in school-aged children with prenatal cocaine exposure. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2006, 30, 905–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  126. Murcia, M.; Rebagliato, M.; Iniguez, C.; Lopez-Espinosa, M.J.; Estarlich, M.; Plaza, B.; Barona-Vilar, C.; Espada, M.; Vioque, J.; Ballester, F. Effect of iodine supplementation during pregnancy on infant neurodevelopment at 1 year of age. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2011, 173, 804–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  127. Nair, P.; Black, M.M.; Ackerman, J.P.; Schuler, M.E.; Keane, V.A. Children’s cognitive-behavioral functioning at age 6 and 7: Prenatal drug exposure and caregiving environment. Ambul. Pediatr. Off. J. Ambul. Pediatr. Assoc. 2008, 8, 154–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  128. Nakajima, S.; Saijo, Y.; Kato, S.; Sasaki, S.; Uno, A.; Kanagami, N.; Hirakawa, H.; Hori, T.; Tobiishi, K.; Todaka, T.; et al. Effects of prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and dioxins on mental and motor development in Japanese children at 6 months of age. Environ. Health Perspect. 2006, 114, 773–778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  129. Nulman, I.; Koren, G.; Rovet, J.; Barrera, M.; Pulver, A.; Streiner, D.; Feldman, B. Neurodevelopment of children following prenatal exposure to venlafaxine, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, or untreated maternal depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 2012, 169, 1165–1174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  130. Nulman, I.; Rovet, J.; Kennedy, D.; Wasson, C.; Gladstone, J.; Fried, S.; Koren, G. Binge alcohol consumption by non-alcohol-dependent women during pregnancy affects child behaviour, but not general intellectual functioning; a prospective controlled study. Arch. Women’s Ment. Health 2004, 7, 173–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. O’Callaghan, F.V.; O’Callaghan, M.; Najman, J.M.; Williams, G.M.; Bor, W. Prenatal alcohol exposure and attention, learning and intellectual ability at 14 years: A prospective longitudinal study. Early Hum. Dev. 2007, 83, 115–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  132. Perera, F.P.; Tang, D.; Rauh, V.; Tu, Y.H.; Tsai, W.Y.; Becker, M.; Stein, J.L.; King, J.; Del Priore, G.; Lederman, S.A. Relationship between polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbon-DNA adducts, environmental tobacco smoke, and child development in the World Trade Center cohort. Environ. Health Perspect. 2007, 115, 1497–1502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  133. Perera, F.P.; Li, Z.; Whyatt, R.; Hoepner, L.; Wang, S.; Camann, D.; Rauh, V. Prenatal airborne polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbon exposure and child IQ at age 5 years. Pediatrics 2009, 124, e195–e202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  134. Piteo, A.M.; Yelland, L.N.; Makrides, M. Does maternal depression predict developmental outcome in 18 month old infants? Early Hum. Dev. 2012, 88, 651–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  135. Pop, V.J.; Brouwers, E.P.; Vader, H.L.; Vulsma, T.; van Baar, A.L.; de Vijlder, J.J. Maternal hypothyroxinaemia during early pregnancy and subsequent child development: A 3-year follow-up study. Clin. Endocrinol. 2003, 59, 282–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  136. Puertas, R.; Lopez-Espinosa, M.J.; Cruz, F.; Ramos, R.; Freire, C.; Perez-Garcia, M.; Abril, A.; Julvez, J.; Salvatierra, M.; Campoy, C.; et al. Prenatal exposure to mirex impairs neurodevelopment at age of 4 years. Neurotoxicology 2010, 31, 154–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Raikes, H.; Pan, B.A.; Luze, G.; Tamis-LeMonda, C.S.; Brooks-Gunn, J.; Constantine, J.; Tarullo, L.B.; Raikes, H.A.; Rodriguez, E.T. Mother-child bookreading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Dev. 2006, 77, 924–953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  138. Rauh, V.; Arunajadai, S.; Horton, M.; Perera, F.; Hoepner, L.; Barr, D.B.; Whyatt, R. Seven-year neurodevelopmental scores and prenatal exposure to chlorpyrifos, a common agricultural pesticide. Environ. Health Perspect. 2011, 119, 1196–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Rauh, V.A.; Whyatt, R.M.; Garfinkel, R.; Andrews, H.; Hoepner, L.; Reyes, A.; Diaz, D.; Camann, D.; Perera, F.P. Developmental effects of exposure to environmental tobacco smoke and material hardship among inner-city children. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2004, 26, 373–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  140. Ribas-Fito, N.; Cardo, E.; Sala, M.; Eulalia de Muga, M.; Mazon, C.; Verdu, A.; Kogevinas, M.; Grimalt, J.O.; Sunyer, J. Breastfeeding, exposure to organochlorine compounds, and neurodevelopment in infants. Pediatrics 2003, 111, e580–e585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  141. Richardson, G.A.; Goldschmidt, L.; Willford, J. The effects of prenatal cocaine use on infant development. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2008, 30, 96–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  142. Silva Rde, C.; Assis, A.M.; Hasselmann, M.H.; dos Santos, L.M.; Pinto Ede, J.; Rodrigues, L.C. Influence of domestic violence on the association between malnutrition and low cognitive development. J. Pediatr. 2012, 88, 149–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  143. Singer, L.T.; Minnes, S.; Short, E.; Arendt, R.; Farkas, K.; Lewis, B.; Klein, N.; Russ, S.; Min, M.O.; Kirchner, H.L. Cognitive outcomes of preschool children with prenatal cocaine exposure. JAMA 2004, 291, 2448–2456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  144. Singer, L.T.; Moore, D.G.; Fulton, S.; Goodwin, J.; Turner, J.J.; Min, M.O.; Parrott, A.C. Neurobehavioral outcomes of infants exposed to MDMA (Ecstasy) and other recreational drugs during pregnancy. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2012, 34, 303–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  145. Slykerman, R.F.; Thompson, J.M.; Pryor, J.E.; Becroft, D.M.; Robinson, E.; Clark, P.M.; Wild, C.J.; Mitchell, E.A. Maternal stress, social support and preschool children’s intelligence. Early Hum. Dev. 2005, 81, 815–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. Smith, L.M.; LaGasse, L.L.; Derauf, C.; Newman, E.; Shah, R.; Haning, W.; Arria, A.; Huestis, M.; Strauss, A.; Della Grotta, S.; et al. Motor and cognitive outcomes through three years of age in children exposed to prenatal methamphetamine. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2011, 33, 176–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  147. Smithers, L.G.; Golley, R.K.; Mittinty, M.N.; Brazionis, L.; Northstone, K.; Emmett, P.; Lynch, J.W. Do dietary trajectories between infancy and toddlerhood influence IQ in childhood and adolescence? Results from a prospective birth cohort study. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e58904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  148. Stewart, P.W.; Reihman, J.; Lonky, E.I.; Darvill, T.J.; Pagano, J. Cognitive development in preschool children prenatally exposed to PCBs and MeHg. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2003, 25, 11–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Stewart, P.W.; Lonky, E.; Reihman, J.; Pagano, J.; Gump, B.B.; Darvill, T. The relationship between prenatal PCB exposure and intelligence (IQ) in 9-year-old children. Environ. Health Perspect. 2008, 116, 1416–1422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  150. Stewart, P.W.; Reihman, J.; Lonky, E.; Pagano, J. Issues in the interpretation of associations of PCBs and IQ. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2012, 34, 96–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Sutter-Dallay, A.L.; Murray, L.; Dequae-Merchadou, L.; Glatigny-Dallay, E.; Bourgeois, M.L.; Verdoux, H. A prospective longitudinal study of the impact of early postnatal vs. chronic maternal depressive symptoms on child development. Eur. Psychiatry J. Assoc. Eur. Psychiatr. 2011, 26, 484–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. Sylva, K.; Stein, A.; Leach, P.; Barnes, J.; Malmberg, L.E. Effects of early child-care on cognition, language, and task-related behaviours at 18 months: An English study. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 29, 18–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  153. Tamis-LeMonda, C.S.; Shannon, J.D.; Cabrera, N.J.; Lamb, M.E. Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year-olds: Contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Dev. 2004, 75, 1806–1820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  154. Tofail, F.; Hamadani, J.D.; Ahmed, A.Z.; Mehrin, F.; Hakim, M.; Huda, S.N. The mental development and behavior of low-birth-weight Bangladeshi infants from an urban low-income community. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2012, 66, 237–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  155. Vrijheid, M.; Martinez, D.; Aguilera, I.; Bustamante, M.; Ballester, F.; Estarlich, M.; Fernandez-Somoano, A.; Guxens, M.; Lertxundi, N.; Martinez, M.D.; et al. Indoor air pollution from gas cooking and infant neurodevelopment. Epidemiology 2012, 23, 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  156. Wasserman, G.A.; Liu, X.; Parvez, F.; Ahsan, H.; Levy, D.; Factor-Litvak, P.; Kline, J.; van Geen, A.; Slavkovich, V.; LoIacono, N.J.; et al. Water manganese exposure and children’s intellectual function in Araihazar, Bangladesh. Environ. Health Perspect. 2006, 114, 124–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  157. Wasserman, G.A.; Liu, X.; Parvez, F.; Ahsan, H.; Factor-Litvak, P.; van Geen, A.; Slavkovich, V.; LoIacono, N.J.; Cheng, Z.; Hussain, I.; et al. Water arsenic exposure and children’s intellectual function in Araihazar, Bangladesh. Environ. Health Perspect. 2004, 112, 1329–1333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  158. Williamson, D.L.; Salkie, F.J.; Letourneau, N. Welfare reforms and the cognitive development of young children. Can. J. Public Health 2005, 96, 13–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  159. Zhou, S.J.; Baghurst, P.; Gibson, R.A.; Makrides, M. Home environment, not duration of breast-feeding, predicts intelligence quotient of children at four years. Nutrition 2007, 23, 236–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  160. Huizink, A.C.; Robles de Medina, P.G.; Mulder, E.J.; Visser, G.H.; Buitelaar, J.K. Stress during pregnancy is associated with developmental outcome in infancy. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2003, 44, 810–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  161. Lee, P. Cognitive development in bilingual children: A case for bilingual instruction in early childhood education. Biling. Res. J. 1996, 20, 499–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  162. Bialystok, E. Consequences of bilingualism for cognitive development. Handb. Biling. Psycholinguist. Approaches 2005, 417–432. [Google Scholar]
  163. Kenny, L.C.; Lavender, T.; McNamee, R.; O’Neill, S.M.; Mills, T.; Khashan, A.S. Advanced maternal age and adverse pregnancy outcome: Evidence from a large contemporary cohort. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e56583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  164. Fraser, A.M.; Brockert, J.E.; Ward, R.H. Association of young maternal age with adverse reproductive outcomes. N. Engl. J.Med. 1995, 332, 1113–1118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  165. Negro, R.; Schwartz, A.; Gismondi, R.; Tinelli, A.; Mangieri, T.; Stagnaro-Green, A. Thyroid antibody positivity in the first trimester of pregnancy is associated with negative pregnancy outcomes. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2011, 96, E920–E924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  166. Männistö, T.; Mendola, P.; Grewal, J.; Xie, Y.; Chen, Z.; Laughon, S.K. Thyroid diseases and adverse pregnancy outcomes in a contemporary US cohort. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2013, 98, 2725–2733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  167. Khashan, A.S.; Kenny, L.C. The effects of maternal body mass index on pregnancy outcome. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2009, 24, 697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  168. Sebire, N.J.; Jolly, M.; Harris, J.; Wadsworth, J.; Joffe, M.; Beard, R.; Regan, L.; Robinson, S. Maternal obesity and pregnancy outcome: A study of 287 213 pregnancies in London. Int. J. Obes. 2001, 25, 1175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  169. Nilsen, F.M.; Tulve, N.S. A systematic review and meta-analysis examining the interrelationships between chemical and non-chemical stressors and inherent characteristics in children with ADHD. Environ. Res. 2020, 180, 108884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  170. Anderson, P.; Doyle, L.W.; Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. NEurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children born extremely low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s. JAMA 2003, 289, 3264–3272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  171. Barrigas, C.; Fragoso, I. Obesity, academic performance and reasoning ability in Portuguese students between 6 and 12 years old. J. Biosoc. Sci. 2012, 44, 165–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  172. Beyerlein, A.; Ness, A.R.; Streuling, I.; Hadders-Algra, M.; von Kries, R. Early rapid growth: No association with later cognitive functions in children born not small for gestational age. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2010, 92, 585–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  173. Cabrera, N.J.; Fagan, J.; Wight, V.; Schadler, C. Influence of mother, father, and child risk on parenting and children’s cognitive and social behaviors. Child Dev. 2011, 82, 1985–2005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  174. Carlo, W.A.; Goudar, S.S.; Pasha, O.; Chomba, E.; McClure, E.M.; Biasini, F.J.; Wallander, J.L.; Thorsten, V.; Chakraborty, H.; Wright, L.L. Neurodevelopmental outcomes in infants requiring resuscitation in developing countries. J. Pediatr. 2012, 160, 781–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  175. Chaudhari, S.; Otiv, M.; Chitale, A.; Pandit, A.; Hoge, M. Pune low birth weight study--cognitive abilities and educational performance at twelve years. Indian Pediatr. 2004, 41, 121–128. [Google Scholar]
  176. Cho, S.C.; Bhang, S.Y.; Hong, Y.C.; Shin, M.S.; Kim, B.N.; Kim, J.W.; Yoo, H.J.; Cho, I.H.; Kim, H.W. Relationship between environmental phthalate exposure and the intelligence of school-age children. Environ. Health Perspect. 2010, 118, 1027–1032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  177. Claus Henn, B.; Ettinger, A.S.; Schwartz, J.; Tellez-Rojo, M.M.; Lamadrid-Figueroa, H.; Hernandez-Avila, M.; Schnaas, L.; Amarasiriwardena, C.; Bellinger, D.C.; Hu, H.; et al. Early postnatal blood manganese levels and children’s neurodevelopment. Epidemiology 2010, 21, 433–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  178. Cooke, R.W.; Foulder-Hughes, L. Growth impairment in the very preterm and cognitive and motor performance at 7 years. Arch. Dis. Child. 2003, 88, 482–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  179. Cserjesi, R.; Van Braeckel, K.N.; Butcher, P.R.; Kerstjens, J.M.; Reijneveld, S.A.; Bouma, A.; Geuze, R.H.; Bos, A.F. Functioning of 7-year-old children born at 32 to 35 weeks’ gestational age. Pediatrics 2012, 130, e838–e846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  180. El-khayat, H.; Shaaban, S.; Emam, E.K.; Elwakkad, A. Cognitive functions in protein-energy malnutrition: In relation to long chain-polyunsaturated fatty acids. Pak. J. Biol. Sci. 2007, 10, 1773–1781. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  181. Feldman, R.; Eidelman, A.I. Does a triplet birth pose a special risk for infant development? Assessing cognitive development in relation to intrauterine growth and mother-infant interaction across the first 2 years. Pediatrics 2005, 115, 443–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  182. Freire, C.; Ramos, R.; Amaya, E.; Fernandez, M.F.; Santiago-Fernandez, P.; Lopez-Espinosa, M.J.; Arrebola, J.P.; Olea, N. Newborn TSH concentration and its association with cognitive development in healthy boys. Eur. J. Endocrinol. 2010, 163, 901–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  183. Gale, C.R.; Martyn, C.N.; Marriott, L.D.; Limond, J.; Crozier, S.; Inskip, H.M.; Godfrey, K.M.; Law, C.M.; Cooper, C.; Robinson, S.M. Dietary patterns in infancy and cognitive and neuropsychological function in childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2009, 50, 816–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  184. Gale, C.R.; O’Callaghan, F.J.; Bredow, M.; Martyn, C.N. The influence of head growth in fetal life, infancy, and childhood on intelligence at the ages of 4 and 8 years. Pediatrics 2006, 118, 1486–1492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  185. Gunnell, D.; Miller, L.L.; Rogers, I.; Holly, J.M. Association of insulin-like growth factor I and insulin-like growth factor-binding protein-3 with intelligence quotient among 8- to 9-year-old children in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatrics 2005, 116, e681–e686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  186. Horton, M.K.; Rundle, A.; Camann, D.E.; Boyd Barr, D.; Rauh, V.A.; Whyatt, R.M. Impact of prenatal exposure to piperonyl butoxide and permethrin on 36-month neurodevelopment. Pediatrics 2011, 127, e699–e706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  187. Kilbride, H.W.; Thorstad, K.; Daily, D.K. Preschool outcome of less than 801-gram preterm infants compared with full-term siblings. Pediatrics 2004, 113, 742–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  188. Larroque, B.; Ancel, P.Y.; Marret, S.; Marchand, L.; Andre, M.; Arnaud, C.; Pierrat, V.; Roze, J.C.; Messer, J.; Thiriez, G.; et al. Neurodevelopmental disabilities and special care of 5-year-old children born before 33 weeks of gestation (the EPIPAGE study): A longitudinal cohort study. Lancet 2008, 371, 813–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  189. Lowe, J.; Erickson, S.J.; MacLean, P. Cognitive correlates in toddlers born very low birth weight and full-term. Infant Behav. Dev. 2010, 33, 629–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  190. Marlow, N.; Wolke, D.; Bracewell, M.A.; Samara, M. Neurologic and developmental disability at six years of age after extremely preterm birth. N. Engl. J. Med. 2005, 352, 9–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  191. Morales, E.; Sunyer, J.; Julvez, J.; Castro-Giner, F.; Estivill, X.; Torrent, M.; De Cid, R. GSTM1 polymorphisms modify the effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy on cognitive functioning in preschoolers. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2009, 38, 690–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  192. Morales, E.; Bustamante, M.; Gonzalez, J.R.; Guxens, M.; Torrent, M.; Mendez, M.; Garcia-Esteban, R.; Julvez, J.; Forns, J.; Vrijheid, M.; et al. Genetic variants of the FADS gene cluster and ELOVL gene family, colostrums LC-PUFA levels, breastfeeding, and child cognition. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e17181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  193. Munck, P.; Niemi, P.; Lapinleimu, H.; Lehtonen, L.; Haataja, L. Stability of cognitive outcome from 2 to 5 years of age in very low birth weight children. Pediatrics 2012, 129, 503–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  194. Nelson, S.; Lerner, E.; Needlman, R.; Salvator, A.; Singer, L.T. Cocaine, anemia, and neurodevelopmental outcomes in children: A longitudinal study. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2004, 25, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  195. Nepomnyaschy, L.; Hegyi, T.; Ostfeld, B.M.; Reichman, N.E. Developmental outcomes of late-preterm infants at 2 and 4 years. Matern. Child Health J. 2012, 16, 1612–1624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  196. Odd, D.E.; Lewis, G.; Whitelaw, A.; Gunnell, D. Resuscitation at birth and cognition at 8 years of age: A cohort study. Lancet 2009, 373, 1615–1622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  197. Orchinik, L.J.; Taylor, H.G.; Espy, K.A.; Minich, N.; Klein, N.; Sheffield, T.; Hack, M. Cognitive outcomes for extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children in kindergarten. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 1067–1079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  198. Park, H.Y.; Hertz-Picciotto, I.; Sovcikova, E.; Kocan, A.; Drobna, B.; Trnovec, T. Neurodevelopmental toxicity of prenatal polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) by chemical structure and activity: A birth cohort study. Environ. Health Glob. Access Sci. Sour. 2010, 9, 51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  199. Park, H.Y.; Park, J.S.; Sovcikova, E.; Kocan, A.; Linderholm, L.; Bergman, A.; Trnovec, T.; Hertz-Picciotto, I. Exposure to hydroxylated polychlorinated biphenyls (OH-PCBs) in the prenatal period and subsequent neurodevelopment in eastern Slovakia. Environ. Health Perspect. 2009, 117, 1600–1606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  200. Rauh, V.A.; Garfinkel, R.; Perera, F.P.; Andrews, H.F.; Hoepner, L.; Barr, D.B.; Whitehead, R.; Tang, D.; Whyatt, R.W. Impact of prenatal chlorpyrifos exposure on neurodevelopment in the first 3 years of life among inner-city children. Pediatrics 2006, 118, e1845–e1859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  201. Riojas-Rodriguez, H.; Solis-Vivanco, R.; Schilmann, A.; Montes, S.; Rodriguez, S.; Rios, C.; Rodriguez-Agudelo, Y. Intellectual function in Mexican children living in a mining area and environmentally exposed to manganese. Environ. Health Perspect. 2010, 118, 1465–1470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  202. Schermann, L.; Sedin, G. Cognitive function at 10 years of age in children who have required neonatal intensive care. Acta Paediatr. 2004, 93, 1619–1629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  203. Schnaas, L.; Rothenberg, S.J.; Flores, M.F.; Martinez, S.; Hernandez, C.; Osorio, E.; Velasco, S.R.; Perroni, E. Reduced intellectual development in children with prenatal lead exposure. Environ. Health Perspect. 2006, 114, 791–797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  204. Short, E.J.; Klein, N.K.; Lewis, B.A.; Fulton, S.; Eisengart, S.; Kercsmar, C.; Baley, J.; Singer, L.T. Cognitive and academic consequences of bronchopulmonary dysplasia and very low birth weight: 8-year-old outcomes. Pediatrics 2003, 112, e359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  205. Serenius, F.; Kallen, K.; Blennow, M.; Ewald, U.; Fellman, V.; Holmstrom, G.; Lindberg, E.; Lundqvist, P.; Marsal, K.; Norman, M.; et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome in extremely preterm infants at 2.5 years after active perinatal care in Sweden. JAMA 2013, 309, 1810–1820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  206. Soria-Pastor, S.; Padilla, N.; Zubiaurre-Elorza, L.; Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N.; Botet, F.; Costas-Moragas, C.; Falcon, C.; Bargallo, N.; Mercader, J.M.; Junque, C. Decreased regional brain volume and cognitive impairment in preterm children at low risk. Pediatrics 2009, 124, e1161–e1170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  207. Surkan, P.J.; Zhang, A.; Trachtenberg, F.; Daniel, D.B.; McKinlay, S.; Bellinger, D.C. Neuropsychological function in children with blood lead levels <10 microg/dL. Neurotoxicology 2007, 28, 1170–1177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  208. Tellez-Rojo, M.M.; Bellinger, D.C.; Arroyo-Quiroz, C.; Lamadrid-Figueroa, H.; Mercado-Garcia, A.; Schnaas-Arrieta, L.; Wright, R.O.; Hernandez-Avila, M.; Hu, H. Longitudinal associations between blood lead concentrations lower than 10 microg/dL and neurobehavioral development in environmentally exposed children in Mexico City. Pediatrics 2006, 118, e323–e330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  209. von Ehrenstein, O.S.; Mikolajczyk, R.T.; Zhang, J. Timing and trajectories of fetal growth related to cognitive development in childhood. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2009, 170, 1388–1395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  210. Wasserman, G.A.; Liu, X.; Parvez, F.; Ahsan, H.; Factor-Litvak, P.; Kline, J.; van Geen, A.; Slavkovich, V.; Loiacono, N.J.; Levy, D.; et al. Water arsenic exposure and intellectual function in 6-year-old children in Araihazar, Bangladesh. Environ. Health Perspect. 2007, 115, 285–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  211. Watson, G.E.; Evans, K.; Thurston, S.W.; van Wijngaarden, E.; Wallace, J.M.; McSorley, E.M.; Bonham, M.P.; Mulhern, M.S.; McAfee, A.J.; Davidson, P.W.; et al. Prenatal exposure to dental amalgam in the Seychelles Child Development Nutrition Study: Associations with neurodevelopmental outcomes at 9 and 30 months. Neurotoxicology 2012, 33, 1511–1517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  212. Watson, G.E.; Lynch, M.; Myers, G.J.; Shamlaye, C.F.; Thurston, S.W.; Zareba, G.; Clarkson, T.W.; Davidson, P.W. Prenatal exposure to dental amalgam: Evidence from the Seychelles Child Development Study main cohort. J. Am. Dent. Assoc. 2011, 142, 1283–1294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  213. Whyatt, R.M.; Liu, X.; Rauh, V.A.; Calafat, A.M.; Just, A.C.; Hoepner, L.; Diaz, D.; Quinn, J.; Adibi, J.; Perera, F.P.; et al. Maternal prenatal urinary phthalate metabolite concentrations and child mental, psychomotor, and behavioral development at 3 years of age. Environ. Health Perspect. 2012, 120, 290–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  214. Yang, S.; Tilling, K.; Martin, R.; Davies, N.; Ben-Shlomo, Y.; Kramer, M.S. Pre-natal and post-natal growth trajectories and childhood cognitive ability and mental health. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2011, 40, 1215–1226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  215. Delahunty, C.; Falconer, S.; Hume, R.; Jackson, L.; Midgley, P.; Mirfield, M.; Ogston, S.; Perra, O.; Simpson, J.; Watson, J. Levels of neonatal thyroid hormone in preterm infants and neurodevelopmental outcome at 5½ years: Millennium cohort study. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2010, 95, 4898–4908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  216. Grunau, R.E.; Whitfield, M.F.; Petrie-Thomas, J.; Synnes, A.R.; Cepeda, I.L.; Keidar, A.; Rogers, M.; MacKay, M.; Hubber-Richard, P.; Johannesen, D. Neonatal pain, parenting stress and interaction, in relation to cognitive and motor development at 8 and 18 months in preterm infants. Pain 2009, 143, 138–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  217. Lewis, S.J.; Zuccolo, L.; Smith, G.D.; Macleod, J.; Rodriguez, S.; Draper, E.S.; Barrow, M.; Alati, R.; Sayal, K.; Ring, S. Fetal alcohol exposure and IQ at age 8: Evidence from a population-based birth-cohort study. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e49407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  218. McDonald, S.D.; Han, Z.; Mulla, S.; Beyene, J. Overweight and obesity in mothers and risk of preterm birth and low birth weight infants: Systematic review and meta-analyses. BMJ 2010, 341, c3428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  219. Kelleher, K.J.; Casey, P.H.; Bradley, R.H.; Pope, S.K.; Whiteside, L.; Barrett, K.W.; Swanson, M.E.; Kirby, R.S. Risk factors and outcomes for failure to thrive in low birth weight preterm infants. Pediatrics 1993, 91, 941–948. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  220. Kimura, D.; Hampson, E. Cognitive pattern in men and women is influenced by fluctuations in sex hormones. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 1994, 3, 57–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  221. Berenbaum, S.A.; Korman, K.; Leveroni, C. Early hormones and sex differences in cognitive abilities. Learn. Individ. Differ. 1995, 7, 303–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  222. Waber, D.P. Sex differences in cognition: A function of maturation rate? Science 1976, 192, 572–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  223. Kostović, I.; Jovanov-Milošević, N. The development of cerebral connections during the first 20–45 weeks’ gestation. Semin. Fetal Neonatal Med. 2006, 11, 415–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  224. Chiodo, L.M.; Janisse, J.; Delaney-Black, V.; Sokol, R.J.; Hannigan, J.H. A metric of maternal prenatal risk drinking predicts neurobehavioral outcomes in preschool children. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2009, 33, 634–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  225. Davidson, P.W.; Strain, J.J.; Myers, G.J.; Thurston, S.W.; Bonham, M.P.; Shamlaye, C.F.; Stokes-Riner, A.; Wallace, J.M.; Robson, P.J.; Duffy, E.M.; et al. Neurodevelopmental effects of maternal nutritional status and exposure to methylmercury from eating fish during pregnancy. Neurotoxicology 2008, 29, 767–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  226. Edwards, S.C.; Jedrychowski, W.; Butscher, M.; Camann, D.; Kieltyka, A.; Mroz, E.; Flak, E.; Li, Z.; Wang, S.; Rauh, V.; et al. Prenatal exposure to airborne polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons and children’s intelligence at 5 years of age in a prospective cohort study in Poland. Environ. Health Perspect. 2010, 118, 1326–1331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  227. Hamadani, J.D.; Tofail, F.; Nermell, B.; Gardner, R.; Shiraji, S.; Bottai, M.; Arifeen, S.E.; Huda, S.N.; Vahter, M. Critical windows of exposure for arsenic-associated impairment of cognitive function in pre-school girls and boys: A population-based cohort study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2011, 40, 1593–1604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  228. Harari, R.; Julvez, J.; Murata, K.; Barr, D.; Bellinger, D.C.; Debes, F.; Grandjean, P. Neurobehavioral deficits and increased blood pressure in school-age children prenatally exposed to pesticides. Environ. Health Perspect. 2010, 118, 890–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  229. Jedrychowski, W.; Perera, F.; Jankowski, J.; Mrozek-Budzyn, D.; Mroz, E.; Flak, E.; Edwards, S.; Skarupa, A.; Lisowska-Miszczyk, I. Gender specific differences in neurodevelopmental effects of prenatal exposure to very low-lead levels: The prospective cohort study in three-year olds. Early Hum. Dev. 2009, 85, 503–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  230. Jedrychowski, W.; Jankowski, J.; Flak, E.; Skarupa, A.; Mroz, E.; Sochacka-Tatara, E.; Lisowska-Miszczyk, I.; Szpanowska-Wohn, A.; Rauh, V.; Skolicki, Z.; et al. Effects of prenatal exposure to mercury on cognitive and psychomotor function in one-year-old infants: Epidemiologic cohort study in Poland. Ann. Epidemiol. 2006, 16, 439–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  231. Laslo-Baker, D.; Barrera, M.; Knittel-Keren, D.; Kozer, E.; Wolpin, J.; Khattak, S.; Hackman, R.; Rovet, J.; Koren, G. Child neurodevelopmental outcome and maternal occupational exposure to solvents. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2004, 158, 956–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  232. Min, M.O.; Singer, L.T.; Kirchner, H.L.; Minnes, S.; Short, E.; Hussain, Z.; Nelson, S. Cognitive development and low-level lead exposure in poly-drug exposed children. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2009, 31, 225–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  233. Perera, F.P.; Rauh, V.; Whyatt, R.M.; Tsai, W.Y.; Tang, D.; Diaz, D.; Hoepner, L.; Barr, D.; Tu, Y.H.; Camann, D.; et al. Effect of prenatal exposure to airborne polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons on neurodevelopment in the first 3 years of life among inner-city children. Environ. Health Perspect. 2006, 114, 1287–1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  234. Seraj, B.; Shahrabi, M.; Shadfar, M.; Ahmadi, R.; Fallahzadeh, M.; Eslamlu, H.F.; Kharazifard, M.J. Effect of high water fluoride concentration on the intellectual development of children in makoo/iran. J. Dent. 2012, 9, 221–229. [Google Scholar]
  235. Shivaprakash, P.K.; Ohri, K.; Noorani, H. Relation between dental fluorosis and intelligence quotient in school children of Bagalkot district. J. Indian Soc. Pedod. Prev. Dent. 2011, 29, 117–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  236. Shy, C.G.; Huang, H.L.; Chang-Chien, G.P.; Chao, H.R.; Tsou, T.C. Neurodevelopment of infants with prenatal exposure to polybrominated diphenyl ethers. Bull. Environ. Contam. Toxicol. 2011, 87, 643–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  237. Thompson, W.W.; Price, C.; Goodson, B.; Shay, D.K.; Benson, P.; Hinrichsen, V.L.; Lewis, E.; Eriksen, E.; Ray, P.; Marcy, S.M.; et al. Early thimerosal exposure and neuropsychological outcomes at 7 to 10 years. N. Engl. J. Med. 2007, 357, 1281–1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  238. Wang, S.X.; Wang, Z.H.; Cheng, X.T.; Li, J.; Sang, Z.P.; Zhang, X.D.; Han, L.L.; Qiao, X.Y.; Wu, Z.M.; Wang, Z.Q. Arsenic and fluoride exposure in drinking water: Children’s IQ and growth in Shanyin county, Shanxi province, China. Environ. Health Perspect. 2007, 115, 643–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  239. Karri, V.; Schuhmacher, M.; Kumar, V. Heavy metals (Pb, Cd, As and MeHg) as risk factors for cognitive dysfunction: A general review of metal mixture mechanism in brain. Environ. Toxicol. Pharmacol. 2016, 48, 203–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  240. Dietrich, K.N.; Succop, P.A.; Berger, O.G.; Hammond, P.B.; Bornschein, R.L. Lead exposure and the cognitive development of urban preschool children: The Cincinnati Lead Study cohort at age 4 years. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 1991, 13, 203–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  241. Smith, P.W. Memory Maintenance and Cognitive Decline. In Anti-Aging Therapeutics Volume XVI; Klatz, R., Goldman, R., Eds.; A4M Publications: Chicago, IL, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  242. Braun, J.M.; Kalloo, G.; Chen, A.; Dietrich, K.N.; Liddy-Hicks, S.; Morgan, S.; Xu, Y.; Yolton, K.; Lanphear, B.P. Cohort profile: The Health Outcomes and Measures of the Environment (HOME) study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2017, 46, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  243. Tillett, T. PAHs and Cognitive Impairment: Prenatal Exposure Catches Up with Toddlers. Environ. Health Perspect. 2006, 114, A487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  244. Jurewicz, J.; Polańska, K.; Hanke, W. Exposure to widespread environmental toxicants and children’s cognitive development and behavioral problems. Int. J. Occup. Med. Environ. Health 2013, 26, 185–204. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  245. Yolton, K.; Dietrich, K.; Auinger, P.; Lanphear, B.P.; Hornung, R. Exposure to environmental tobacco smoke and cognitive abilities among US children and adolescents. Environ. Health Perspect. 2005, 113, 98–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  246. Tabb, M.M.; Blumberg, B. New modes of action for endocrine-disrupting chemicals. Mol. Endocrinol. 2006, 20, 475–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  247. Galea, L.A.; Uban, K.A.; Epp, J.R.; Brummelte, S.; Barha, C.K.; Wilson, W.L.; Lieblich, S.E.; Pawluski, J.L. Endocrine regulation of cognition and neuroplasticity: Our pursuit to unveil the complex interaction between hormones, the brain, and behaviour. Can. J. Exp. Psychol./Revue Can. Psychol. Exp. 2008, 62, 247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  248. Braun, J.M.; Froehlich, T.E.; Daniels, J.L.; Dietrich, K.N.; Hornung, R.; Auinger, P.; Lanphear, B.P. Association of environmental toxicants and conduct disorder in U.S. children: NHANES 2001–2004. Environ. Health Perspect. 2008, 116, 956–962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  249. Braun, J.M.; Kalkbrenner, A.E.; Calafat, A.M.; Yolton, K.; Ye, X.; Dietrich, K.N.; Lanphear, B.P. Impact of early-life bisphenol A exposure on behavior and executive function in children. Pediatrics 2011, 128, 873–882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  250. Nilsen, F.; Frank, J.; Tulve, N. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis Investigating the Relationship between Exposures to Chemical and Non-Chemical Stressors during Prenatal Development and Childhood Externalizing Behaviors. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 2361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  251. Bakker, E.C.; Ghys, A.J.A.; Kester, A.D.M.; Vles, J.S.H.; Dubas, J.S.; Blanco, C.E.; Hornstra, G. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids at birth and cognitive function at 7[emsp14]y of age. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2003, 57, 89–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  252. Calhoun, S.L.; Mayes, S.D.; Vgontzas, A.N.; Tsaoussoglou, M.; Shifflett, L.J.; Bixler, E.O. No relationship between neurocognitive functioning and mild sleep disordered breathing in a community sample of children. J. Clin. Sleep Med. Off. Publ. Am. Acad. Sleep Med. 2009, 5, 228–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  253. Chatzi, L.; Papadopoulou, E.; Koutra, K.; Roumeliotaki, T.; Georgiou, V.; Stratakis, N.; Lebentakou, V.; Karachaliou, M.; Vassilaki, M.; Kogevinas, M. Effect of high doses of folic acid supplementation in early pregnancy on child neurodevelopment at 18 months of age: The mother-child cohort ‘Rhea’ study in Crete, Greece. Public Health Nutr. 2012, 15, 1728–1736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  254. del Rio Garcia, C.; Torres-Sanchez, L.; Chen, J.; Schnaas, L.; Hernandez, C.; Osorio, E.; Portillo, M.G.; Lopez-Carrillo, L. Maternal MTHFR 677C>T genotype and dietary intake of folate and vitamin B(12): Their impact on child neurodevelopment. Nutr. Neurosci. 2009, 12, 13–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  255. Emancipator, J.L.; Storfer-Isser, A.; Taylor, H.G.; Rosen, C.L.; Kirchner, H.L.; Johnson, N.L.; Zambito, A.M.; Redline, S. Variation of cognition and achievement with sleep-disordered breathing in full-term and preterm children. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2006, 160, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  256. Freire, C.; Ramos, R.; Lopez-Espinosa, M.J.; Diez, S.; Vioque, J.; Ballester, F.; Fernandez, M.F. Hair mercury levels, fish consumption, and cognitive development in preschool children from Granada, Spain. Environ. Res. 2010, 110, 96–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  257. Gale, C.R.; Robinson, S.M.; Godfrey, K.M.; Law, C.M.; Schlotz, W.; O’Callaghan, F.J. Oily fish intake during pregnancy--association with lower hyperactivity but not with higher full-scale IQ in offspring. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2008, 49, 1061–1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  258. Gottlieb, D.J.; Chase, C.; Vezina, R.M.; Heeren, T.C.; Corwin, M.J.; Auerbach, S.H.; Weese-Mayer, D.E.; Lesko, S.M. Sleep-disordered breathing symptoms are associated with poorer cognitive function in 5-year-old children. J. Pediatr. 2004, 145, 458–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  259. Guxens, M.; Mendez, M.A.; Molto-Puigmarti, C.; Julvez, J.; Garcia-Esteban, R.; Forns, J.; Ferrer, M.; Vrijheid, M.; Lopez-Sabater, M.C.; Sunyer, J. Breastfeeding, long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in colostrum, and infant mental development. Pediatrics 2011, 128, e880–e889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  260. Jackman, A.R.; Biggs, S.N.; Walter, L.M.; Embuldeniya, U.S.; Davey, M.J.; Nixon, G.M.; Anderson, V.; Trinder, J.; Horne, R.S. Sleep-disordered breathing in preschool children is associated with behavioral, but not cognitive, impairments. Sleep Med. 2012, 13, 621–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  261. Jacobson, J.L.; Jacobson, S.W.; Muckle, G.; Kaplan-Estrin, M.; Ayotte, P.; Dewailly, E. Beneficial effects of a polyunsaturated fatty acid on infant development: Evidence from the inuit of arctic Quebec. J. Pediatr. 2008, 152, 356–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  262. Julvez, J.; Fortuny, J.; Mendez, M.; Torrent, M.; Ribas-Fito, N.; Sunyer, J. Maternal use of folic acid supplements during pregnancy and four-year-old neurodevelopment in a population-based birth cohort. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2009, 23, 199–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  263. Kaemingk, K.L.; Pasvogel, A.E.; Goodwin, J.L.; Mulvaney, S.A.; Martinez, F.; Enright, P.L.; Rosen, G.M.; Morgan, W.J.; Fregosi, R.F.; Quan, S.F. Learning in children and sleep disordered breathing: Findings of the Tucson Children’s Assessment of Sleep Apnea (tuCASA) prospective cohort study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2003, 9, 1016–1026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  264. Kohler, M.J.; Lushington, K.; van den Heuvel, C.J.; Martin, J.; Pamula, Y.; Kennedy, D. Adenotonsillectomy and neurocognitive deficits in children with Sleep Disordered Breathing. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e7343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  265. Mendez, M.A.; Torrent, M.; Julvez, J.; Ribas-Fito, N.; Kogevinas, M.; Sunyer, J. Maternal fish and other seafood intakes during pregnancy and child neurodevelopment at age 4 years. Public Health Nutr. 2009, 12, 1702–1710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  266. Miano, S.; Paolino, M.C.; Urbano, A.; Parisi, P.; Massolo, A.C.; Castaldo, R.; Villa, M.P. Neurocognitive assessment and sleep analysis in children with sleep-disordered breathing. Clin. Neurophysiol. Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2011, 122, 311–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  267. Pilsner, J.R.; Hu, H.; Wright, R.O.; Kordas, K.; Ettinger, A.S.; Sanchez, B.N.; Cantonwine, D.; Lazarus, A.L.; Cantoral, A.; Schnaas, L.; et al. Maternal MTHFR genotype and haplotype predict deficits in early cognitive development in a lead-exposed birth cohort in Mexico City. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2010, 92, 226–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  268. Ribas-Fito, N.; Julvez, J.; Torrent, M.; Grimalt, J.O.; Sunyer, J. Beneficial effects of breastfeeding on cognition regardless of DDT concentrations at birth. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2007, 166, 1198–1202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  269. Torres-Sanchez, L.; Rothenberg, S.J.; Schnaas, L.; Cebrian, M.E.; Osorio, E.; Del Carmen Hernandez, M.; Garcia-Hernandez, R.M.; Del Rio-Garcia, C.; Wolff, M.S.; Lopez-Carrillo, L. In utero p,p’-DDE exposure and infant neurodevelopment: A perinatal cohort in Mexico. Environ. Health Perspect. 2007, 115, 435–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  270. Veldwijk, J.; Scholtens, S.; Hornstra, G.; Bemelmans, W.J. Body mass index and cognitive ability of young children. Obes. Facts 2011, 4, 264–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  271. Quigley, M.A.; Hockley, C.; Carson, C.; Kelly, Y.; Renfrew, M.J.; Sacker, A. Breastfeeding is associated with improved child cognitive development: A population-based cohort study. J. Pediatr. 2012, 160, 25–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  272. Karr, J.E.; Alexander, J.E.; Winningham, R.G. Omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids and cognition throughout the lifespan: A review. Nutr. Neurosci. 2011, 14, 216–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  273. Willatts, P.; Forsyth, J. The role of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in infant cognitive development. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fat. Acids 2000, 63, 95–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  274. Qawasmi, A.; Landeros-Weisenberger, A.; Leckman, J.F.; Bloch, M.H. Meta-analysis of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation of formula and infant cognition. Pediatrics 2012, 129, 1141–1149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  275. Willatts, P.; Forsyth, S.; Agostoni, C.; Casaer, P.; Riva, E.; Boehm, G. Effects of long-chain PUFA supplementation in infant formula on cognitive function in later childhood. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2013, 98, 536S–542S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  276. Khodaee, G.H.; Emami Moghadam, Z.; Khademi, G.; Saeidi, M. Healthy Diet in Children: Facts and Keys. Int. J. Pediatr. 2015, 3, 1183–1194. [Google Scholar]
  277. Jago, R.; Baranowski, T.; Baranowski, J.C.; Thompson, D.; Greaves, K. BMI from 3–6 y of age is predicted by TV viewing and physical activity, not diet. Int. J. Obes. 2005, 29, 557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  278. Brown, K.A.; Ogden, J.; Vögele, C.; Gibson, E.L. The role of parental control practices in explaining children’s diet and BMI. Appetite 2008, 50, 252–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  279. Albers, E.M.; Riksen-Walraven, J.M.; de Weerth, C. Developmental stimulation in child care centers contributes to young infants’ cognitive development. Infant Behav. Dev. 2010, 33, 401–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  280. DePrince, A.P.; Weinzierl, K.M.; Combs, M.D. Executive function performance and trauma exposure in a community sample of children. Child Abuse Negl. 2009, 33, 353–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  281. Fishbein, D.; Warner, T.; Krebs, C.; Trevarthen, N.; Flannery, B.; Hammond, J. Differential relationships between personal and community stressors and children’s neurocognitive functioning. Child Maltreatment 2009, 14, 299–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  282. Koenen, K.C.; Moffitt, T.E.; Caspi, A.; Taylor, A.; Purcell, S. Domestic violence is associated with environmental suppression of IQ in young children. Dev. Psychopathol. 2003, 15, 297–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  283. Pears, K.; Fisher, P.A. Developmental, cognitive, and neuropsychological functioning in preschool-aged foster children: Associations with prior maltreatment and placement history. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2005, 26, 112–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  284. Yang, S.; Kramer, M.S. Paternal alcohol consumption, family transition and child development in a former Soviet country. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2012, 41, 1086–1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  285. Perret-Clermont, A.-N. Social Interaction and Cognitive Development in Children; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
  286. Peisner-Feinberg, E.S.; Burchinal, M.R.; Clifford, R.M.; Culkin, M.L.; Howes, C.; Kagan, S.L.; Yazejian, N. The relation of preschool child-care quality to children’s cognitive and social developmental trajectories through second grade. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 1534–1553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  287. Moore, G.T. The physical environment and cognitive development in child-care centers. In Spaces for Children; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1987; pp. 41–72. [Google Scholar]
  288. Duncan, G.J.; National Institute of Child Health; Human Development Early Child Care Research Network. Modeling the impacts of child care quality on children’s preschool cognitive development. Child Dev. 2003, 74, 1454–1475. [Google Scholar]
  289. Caughy, M.O.B.; DiPietro, J.A.; Strobino, D.M. Day-care participation as a protective factor in the cognitive development of low-income children. Child Dev. 1994, 65, 457–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Violin plot of the maternal stressors that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean standard error (SE). BMI denotes body mass index.
Figure 1. Violin plot of the maternal stressors that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean standard error (SE). BMI denotes body mass index.
Ijerph 17 05451 g001
Figure 2. Violin plot of the inherent characteristics that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean SE.
Figure 2. Violin plot of the inherent characteristics that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean SE.
Ijerph 17 05451 g002
Figure 3. Violin plot of the chemical stressors that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean SE.
Figure 3. Violin plot of the chemical stressors that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean SE.
Ijerph 17 05451 g003
Figure 4. Violin plot of the activities/behaviors that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean SE.
Figure 4. Violin plot of the activities/behaviors that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean SE.
Ijerph 17 05451 g004
Figure 5. Violin plot of the social stressors that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean SE.
Figure 5. Violin plot of the social stressors that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean SE.
Ijerph 17 05451 g005
Figure 6. Violin plot of all factors that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean SE.
Figure 6. Violin plot of all factors that may influence cognitive development. Violin width represents the distribution of the Odds Ratios (OR), the height represents density of the ORs; dots represent mean OR, error bars represent mean SE.
Ijerph 17 05451 g006
Table 1. The odds ratio (OR) from the random-effects meta-analyses in this analysis are presented alongside the factor and subgroupings.
Table 1. The odds ratio (OR) from the random-effects meta-analyses in this analysis are presented alongside the factor and subgroupings.
Prenatal ExposuresChildhood Exposures
Primary FactorExposure + VariablesStudiesSampling EventsIndividualsSecondary FactorPrimary FactorExposure + VariablesStudiesSampling EventsIndividualsSecondary Factor
Maternal (1.04) ***Socioeconomics (1.13) ***55139113,976SocialActivities & Behaviors (1.05) ***Breastfeeding (1.16) ***246328,895Maternal/Social
Employment (1.06) ***6156818Yes/No (1.16) **663349
Education (1.18) ***457155,020Duration (1.15) ***164723,789
Income (1.13) ***192928,235LC-PUFAs (1.20) ***3131757
Language (0.96) *572580Diet (1.05) ***173946,179
Resources (1.06) **563149BMI (1.14) ***381802
Home Ownership (0.99) *3517,418Diet pattern (1.03) **51039,284
Technology Access (1.17) **36602Folic Acid (1.04) *451499
Maternal Health (1.02) ***43127125,369Inherent/SocialFish Intake (1.01) ***5163594
Age (1.08) ***253125,100Sleep (0.82) ***11263963
BMI (1.01) ***102252,271Duration (1.06) **33657
Thyroid Health (1.05) ***6206121Snoring (0.66)44356
Mental Health (0.91) ***112436,909Disordered Breathing (0.77) **8162950
Stress (perceived) (0.97) ***7204917Social (1.07) ***Social Interaction (1.14) ***185643,081Inherent/Maternal
Stress (cortisol) (1.08) **3101496Maternal Sensitivity (1.29) ***7112092
Substance Use (0.97) ***39147159,067SocialParent/Child Interact (1.25) ***72426,804
Alcohol (amount) (0.99) ***196064,292Childhood Trauma (0.93) ***62114,185
Alcohol (binging) (0.96) ***41936,817Home/Family (0.98) ***2755119,554
Cigarettes (0.96) ***253851,251Number of Siblings (0.94) **6817,393
Cocaine (0.98) ***9184218Caregiver Relationship (0.92) **594579
Other Narcotics (0.96) **5122489Family Stability (0.98) ***71141,287
Inherent (1.03) ***Anthropometry (1.1) ***84138111,310MaternalMarital Status (1.01) ***111453,001
Head Size (1.22) ***10104111Home Location (0.99) ***4133294
Body Length (1.21) ***91217,112Childcare (1.15) ***4144144
Birth Weight (1.14) ***374743,129Quality of Care (1.25) **351349
Fetal Growth (1.06) **362315Time in Care (1.13) **352251
Sex (1.05) ***596344,643Attendance (1.07) *444144
Birth Outcomes (0.91) ***336753,490Chemical (1.05) ***Toxic Gases (1.06) ***162510,640Social/Activities & Behaviors
Delivery Method (0.98)332532PAHs (1.35) ***561514
Preterm (0.78) ***112116,558Cigarette Smoke (0.97) ***13167017
Parity (0.89) ***142420,195Toxic Elements (1.04) ***255825,754
Multiple Births (0.86)338387Fluorine (1.40) *33902
Gestational Age (1.11) ***13165818Arsenic (0.95) **51112,249
Child Health (1.0) ***184063,969Manganese (0.90) *35520
Medical History (1.03) ***61847,923Lead (1.03) ***11236314
Iron Deficiency (0.89) *671302Mercury (1.10) ***8164769
Genetics (0.99) ***41514,744Endocrine Active (0.99) ***12274621
Pesticides (1.05) ***8101498
Chlorinated (0.92) **3111835
PBDE/phthalates (0.92) *361288
*** p ≤ 0.001 ** p ≤ 0.01 * p ≤ 0.05; The number of studies indicates the number of references used in this analysis; The number of sampling events indicates the data points that were extracted from the included references; The number of individuals is the collective sample size for all included studies in each group/subgroup. PAH-polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons; PBDE-polybrominated diphenyl ethers; BMI-body mass index; LC-PUFA-long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids
Table 2. The results of the sensitivity and publication bias analyses for the data analyzed in this publication. The Odds Ratio (OR) for each of the seven estimation methods are presented. The estimation methods were the restricted maximum likelihood (REML), the maximum likelihood (ML), the fixed-effects (FE), the Hedges (HE), the DerSimonian–Laird (DL), the empirical Bayesian (EB), and the Paul–Mandel (PM) estimators. Standard error is denoted by SE, and the inverse of the sample size is denoted by 1/n.
Table 2. The results of the sensitivity and publication bias analyses for the data analyzed in this publication. The Odds Ratio (OR) for each of the seven estimation methods are presented. The estimation methods were the restricted maximum likelihood (REML), the maximum likelihood (ML), the fixed-effects (FE), the Hedges (HE), the DerSimonian–Laird (DL), the empirical Bayesian (EB), and the Paul–Mandel (PM) estimators. Standard error is denoted by SE, and the inverse of the sample size is denoted by 1/n.
FactorSensitivity AnalysisPublication Bias
Meta-Analysis Estimation ModelData RobustnessMethodp-Value
REML (This Study)FEHEDLPMEBML(% ORs within 10%)
Maternal1.041.041.041.041.041.041.04100%SE0.86
OR Difference0000001/n0.88
% Difference0%0%0%0%0%0%
Inherent1.031.031.031.031.031.031.03100%SE0.76
OR Difference0000001/n1.0
% Difference0%0%0%0%0%0%
Chemical1.051.051.051.051.051.051.05100%SE0.67
OR Difference0.00.00.00.00.00.01/n0.72
% Difference0%0%0%0%0%0%
Behavioral1.061.061.061.061.061.061.06100%SE0.87
OR Difference0000001/n0.89
% Difference0%0%0%0%0%0%
Social1.101.101.101.101.101.101.10100%SE0.63
OR Difference0000001/n0.59
% Difference0%0%0%0%0%0%

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Nilsen, F.M.; Ruiz, J.D.C.; Tulve, N.S. A Meta-Analysis of Stressors from the Total Environment Associated with Children’s General Cognitive Ability. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5451. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155451

AMA Style

Nilsen FM, Ruiz JDC, Tulve NS. A Meta-Analysis of Stressors from the Total Environment Associated with Children’s General Cognitive Ability. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(15):5451. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155451

Chicago/Turabian Style

Nilsen, Frances M., Jazmin D.C. Ruiz, and Nicolle S. Tulve. 2020. "A Meta-Analysis of Stressors from the Total Environment Associated with Children’s General Cognitive Ability" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 15: 5451. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155451

APA Style

Nilsen, F. M., Ruiz, J. D. C., & Tulve, N. S. (2020). A Meta-Analysis of Stressors from the Total Environment Associated with Children’s General Cognitive Ability. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(15), 5451. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155451

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop