A Contagious Other? Exploring the Public’s Appraisals of Contact with ‘Mental Illness’
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Stigma
1.2. Public Understandings of ‘Madness’ and Mental Illness
1.3. Embodied Cognition
1.4. Summary
Research Highlights
- Mental illness is Othered and engaged fears of contamination.
- Constructions of mental illness elicited appraisals of disgust, fear, pity, and compassion.
- Appraisals of mental illness are differentiated by disorder label between schizophrenia and depression.
- Public health anti-stigma campaigns need to tackle implicit prohibitions around intergroup contact.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials and Procedure
2.2.1. Abbreviations
DS | Disgust Sensitivity |
PCMI | Prior Contact with Mental Illness |
RIBS | Reported and Intended Behaviour Scale |
GEW | Geneva Emotion Wheel |
HCNV | High-Control Negative-Valence |
LCPV | Low-Control Positive-Valence |
2.2.2. Questionnaires
2.2.3. Vignette Presentation
2.2.4. Interviews
2.2.5. Triangulation
2.2.6. Quantitative Analysis
2.2.7. Qualitative Analysis
2.2.8. Analytic Strategy
“initially, when I read the one where you just used the other persons mug, I thought okay, not too big a deal, a bit gross, but you know, okay. But then I started thinking about it, wondering what I hadn’t been told about them, that’s when I started to worry” (p. 17).
3. Results
3.1. Prior and Projected Contact with Mental Illness
“I would say anything that has connections, or some sort of links with the head, … pillow, crash helmet … I think it is a feeling of how dangerous can it be to pass on to me… how dangerous can it be if I talk to this person too much for a long time, how can I be somehow contaminated by his ideas or somehow influenced by something that I don’t know”. (P. 31)
Similarly, P.12 expresses concerns over: “their computer, I mean that is kinda a reflection of the kind of person they are, and like probably their condition influences what they do.” By avoiding sharing a computer, she maintains her distance from the perceived personal characteristics and behaviours of someone with a mental health problem.
3.2. Experiential Aspects of Contact with Mental Illness
“If you’ve seen somebody with a cold sneeze onto something, … then obviously there would be germs on there” (p. 3)
“For the cold, I think I would have answered very differently if I had to use a spoon that someone else had used. Because with the mug, you would have had to place your lips exactly on the spot where the other people put it.” (p. 23)
“You don’t know what to do now, in that situation, it’s basically like ‘ahhh’. I feel like I could feel the difference in kinda that feeling. It was different categories. It was really something that is like like ‘oh’, they are more different, like psychologically” (P. 19)
“Is there emotions ‘I just want to escape’? I just want to get away, I just want to stop thinking this. I feel sickness in my stomach. This is strong, I feel I want to eugh [pretends to vomit]. This is a strong emotion … disgust, yeah. Extremely disgust!” (P. 9)
“If they are in depression, I’m afraid they will do something aggressive, especially in private room. Just speak to someone in depression, she just start crying or screaming or lots of negative complaints” (P. 9)
“People with depression, or being prone to be sad or over-upset by small things, their emotions are quite volatile …. Something like this could easily upset them.” (P. 27)
“People with depression, or being in prone to be sad or have sad attitudes, or negative attitudes, I feel they transfer part of that to myself, and I don’t feel comfortable. I don’t despise them, but it makes me feel a bit upset or angry, because I feel how all that negative feeling is taking, transferring on to me” (P. 15)
3.3. Triangulating the Data
“They would attach a degree of contagiousness to it. It’s the fear of losing control… or not knowing what you are doing. It’s dangerous, so better to seal it off” (P. 31)
“Mental like, the people not like us, abnormals… normal is like, we can talk really … we think like human things, don’t imagine things, … they crazy, they abnormal, they take drugs, maybe like have HIV, or hepatitis A B C D. I do care, because I can be addicted to it” (P. 36)
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A: Vignettes:
Appendix A.1. Depression
Appendix A.1.1. Side 1:
Appendix A.1.2. Side 2:
Appendix A.2. Schizophrenia
Appendix A.2.1. Side 1:
Appendix A.2.2. Side 2:
Appendix A.3. Common Cold
Appendix A.3.1. Side 1:
Appendix A.3.2. Side 2
Appendix A.4. No Added Medical Description
Appendix A.4.1. Side 1:
Appendix A.4.2. Side 2:
Appendix B: Interview Guide
- Could you tell me a bit about how you felt imagining using the same mug as the classmates described.
- -
- Probe: what was the sensation of having the water in your mouth? How comfortable did it feel? Which sort of emotions were you feeling?
- Did it feel the same each time?
- -
- Probe: Where there any differences? Was the intensity of your feelings the same each time? Was there one test that was particularly uncomfortable for you?
- Was there anything you were worried about?
- -
- Probe: What sort of ideas might you have about why that was?
- Do you think there any sort of things people might be uncomfortable doing if they knew someone with mental health problems had already used the same object?
- -
- Probe: Which sort of actions? Which sort of objects? Around which sort of people? Why do you think that might be?
- Is there anything else more you want to say?
- -
- Probe: Are there any questions you think I should have asked you?
References
- Jorm, A. Mental Health Literacy: Empowering the Community to Take Action for Better Mental Health; APA: Washington, DC, USA, 2012; Volume 67, pp. 231–243. [Google Scholar]
- Kutcher, S.; Bagnell, A.; Wei, Y. Mental Health Literacy in Secondary Schools. In Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America; Elsevier: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2015; Volume 24, pp. 233–244. [Google Scholar]
- Choudhry, F.; Mani, V.; Ming, L.; Kahn, T. Beliefs and perception about mental health issues: A meta-synthesis. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2016, 12, 2807–2818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Foster, J. Mental Health Campaigns and Social Representations Theory: A Consideration. Pap. Soc. Represent. 2017, 26, 1–21. [Google Scholar]
- Larkings, J.; Brown, P. Do biogenetic causal beliefs reduce mental illness stigma in people with mental illness and in mental health professionals? A systematic review. Int. J. Ment. Health Nurs. 2017, 27, 928–941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Arthi. Representing mental illness: A case of cognitive polyphasias. Pap. Soc. Represent. 2012, 21, 167–186. [Google Scholar]
- Bilic, B.; Georgaca, E. Representations of Mental Illness in Serbian Newspapers: A Critical Discourse Analysis. Qual. Res. Psychol. 2007, 4, 167–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jodelet, D. Madness and Social Representations; Harvester Wheatsheaf: New York, NY, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Wagner, W.; Duveen, G.; Verma, J.; Themel, M. I have some faith and at the same time I don’t believe’—Cognitive polyphasia and cultural change in India. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 10, 301–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Markova, I. Social Identities and Social Representations. In Social Representations and Identity; Palgrave Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Markova, I. The Dialogical Mind: Common Sense and Ethics; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan, P. Lessons learned from unintended consequences about erasing the stigma of mental illness. World Psychiatry 2016, 15, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Henderson, C.; Gronholm, P. Mental Health Related Stigma as a Wicked Problem: The Need to Address Stigma and Consider the Consequences. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2018, 15, 1158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kalampalikis, N.; Haas, V. More than a Theory: A New Map of Social Thought. J. Theory Soc. Behav. 2008, 38, 449–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moscovici, S. Pensée Stigmatique et Pensée Symbolique: Deux Formes élémentaires de la Pensée Sociale. In Les Formes de la Pensee Sociale; PUF: Paris, France, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Goffman, E. Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity; Prentice-Hall Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1963. [Google Scholar]
- Foster, J. Unification and differentiation: A study of the social representations of mental illness. Pap. Soc. Represent. 2001, 10, 1–18. [Google Scholar]
- Foster, J. Media presentation of the mental health bill and representations of mental health problems. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 16, 285–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rose, D. Television, madness and community care. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 8, 213–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lund, E.; Boggero, I. Sick in the Head? Pathogen Concerns Bias Implicit Perceptions of Mental Illness. Evol. Psychol. 2014, 12, 706–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gilman, S. Disease and Representation; Cornell University Press: Ithaca, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Hartnell, J. Medieval Bodies: Life, Death and Art in the Middle Ages; Gomer Press: London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Yanni, C. The Architecture of Madness; University of Minnesota Press: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Schomerus, G.; Schwahn, C.; Holzinger, A.; Corrigan, P.; Grabe, H.; Carta, M.M. Angermeyer, Evolution of public attitudes about mental illness: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2012, 125, 440–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pescosolido, B.; Martin, J. The Stigma Complex; NCBI: Bethesda, MD, USA, 2015; Volume 41, pp. 87–116. [Google Scholar]
- Marsh, J.; Shanks, L. Thinking you can catch mental illness: How beliefs about membership attainment and category structure influence interactions with mental health category members. Mem. Cogn. 2015, 42, 1011–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rozin, P.; Markwith, M.; Nemeroff, C. Magical Contagion Beliefs and Fear of AIDS1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 22, 1081–1092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stavrova, O.; Newman, G.; Kuleman, A.D. Fetchenhauer, Contamination without contact: An examination of intention-based contagion. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2016, 11, 554–571. [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor, C. Embodiment and the Construction of Social Knowledge: Towards an Integration of Embodiment and Social Representations Theory. J. Theory Soc. Behav. 2016, 47, 2–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Finnell, D. A call to action: Managing the neural pathway of disgust, bias, prejudice, and discrimination that fuels stigma. Subst. Abus. 2018, 39, 399–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deacon, B.; Olatunji, B. Specificity of disgust sensitivity in the prediction of behavioral avoidance in contamination fear. Behav. Res. Ther. 2007, 45, 2100–2120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitton, A.; Henry, J.; Rendell, P.; Grisham, J. Disgust, but not anger provocation, enhances levator labii superioris activity during exposure to moral transgressions. Biol. Psychol. 2014, 96, 48–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haidt, J.; McCauley, C.; Rozin, P. Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1994, 16, 701–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hart, B.; Struiksma, M.; van Boxtel, A.; van Berkum, J. Emotion in Stories: Facial EMG Evidence for Both Mental Simulation and Moral Evaluation. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greenland, K.; Xenias, D.; Maio, G. Intergroup anxiety from the self and other: Evidence from self-report, physiological effects, and real interactions. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 42, 150–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saunders, B.; Kingstone, T.; Baker, S.; Waterfeild, J.; Bartlam, B.; Burroughs, H. Saturation in qualitative research: Exploring its conceptualization and operationalization. Qual. Quant. 2018, 52, 1893–1907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fridlund, A.; Cacioppo, J. Guidelines for Human Electromyographic Research. Psychophysiology 1986, 23, 567–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans-Lacko, S.; Rose, D.; Little, K.; Flach, C.; Rhydderch, D.; Henderson, C.; Hornicroft, G. Development and psychometric properties of the Reported and Intended Behaviour Scale (RIBS): A stigma-related behaviour measure. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 2011, 20, 263–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robinson, E.; Henderson, C. Public knowledge, attitudes, social distance and reporting contact with people with mental illness 2009–2017. Psychol. Med. 2018, 49, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Geoghegan, L.; Kwasnicki, R.; Kanabar, S.P.D.; Nduka, C. A systematic recurrent theme analysis of the reported limitations of facial electromyography. Ann. Med. Surg. 2018, 33, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fontaine, J.; Shuman, V.; Scherer, J.; Soriano, C. The GRID meets Wheel: Assessing emotional feeling via self-report. In Components of Emotional Meaning: A Sourcebook; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Dan-Glauser, E.; Scherer, K. The Geneva affective picture database (GAPED): A new 730-picture database focusing on valence and normative significance. Behav. Methods 2011, 43, 468–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Boxtel, A. Facial EMG as a Tool for Inferring Affective States. In Proceedings of the Measuring Behaviour, Eindhoven, The Netherlands, 24–27 August 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Flick, U. Designing Qualitative Research; Sage Publications: London, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Flick, U.; Foster, J. Caillaud Researching Social Representations in Cambridge Handbook of Social Representations; Cambridge University Press: St Ives, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Joffe, H. The Power of Visual Material: Persuasion, Emotion and Identification. Diogenes 2008, 55, 84–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Joffe, H.; Staerkle, C. The Centrality of the Self-Control Ethos in Western Aspersions Regarding Outgroups: A Social Representational Approach to Stereotype Content. Cult. Psychol. 2007, 13, 395–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jackson-Best, F.; Edwards, N. Stigma and intersectionality: A systematic review of systematic reviews across HIV/AIDS, mental illness, and physical disability. BMC Public Health 2018, 18, 919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Foster, J. Perpetuating stigma? Differences between advertisements for psychiatric and non-psychiatric medication in two professional journals. J. Ment. Health 2010, 19, 26–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Repper, J.; Perkins, R. Social Inclusion and Recovery; Baillere Tindall: Edingburgh, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
Gender | Male | (13) |
Female | (23) | |
Age | Range | (18–44) |
Mean | (24) | |
Standard deviation | (5) | |
Subject | Arts and Humanities | (11) |
Biological and Physical Sciences | (5) | |
Mathematics and Engineering | (7) | |
Psychological and Health Sciences | (5) | |
Business and Management | (8) | |
Nationality | Western Europe (17/36) | Britain (11) Italy (2) France (2) Spain (2) |
Eastern Europe (7/36) | Poland (1) Czech Republic (1) Croatia (1) Slovakia (1) Bulgaria (1) Romania (1) Russia (1) | |
Asia (10/36) | China (4) Thailand (3) Malaysia (2) Taiwan (1) Singapore (1) | |
Americas (2/36) | Canada (1) Brazil (1) |
Frequency of Prior Contact with Mental Illness | N (%) |
---|---|
No prior contact with mental illness | 8 (22.2) |
One or less forms of prior contact with mental illness | 17 (47.2) |
Two or less forms of prior contact with mental illness | 29 (80.6) |
Three or less forms of prior contact with mental illness | 34 (94.4) |
Four or less forms of prior contact with mental illness | 36 (100) |
Reported and Intended Behaviour Scale (RIBS) Question | N (%) |
---|---|
Are you currently living, or have ever lived with, someone with a mental health problem? | 14 (39) |
Are you currently working, or have ever worked with, someone with a mental health problem? | 21 (42) |
Do you currently have, or have you ever had, a neighbour with a mental health problem? | 10 (36) |
Do you currently have, or have you ever had, a close friend with a mental health problem? | 17 (47) |
Dependent Variable | Mediator | df(test) | df(error) | F | p | ηp2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HCPV | 1.00 | 35.00 | 0.574 | 0.712 | 0.004 | |
LCPV | 1.00 | 35.00 | 17.513 | <0.001 ** | 0.361 | |
DSQ | 1.00 | 34.00 | 1.121 | 0.298 | 0.035 | |
PCMI | 1.00 | 34.00 | 1.096 | 0.303 | 0.034 | |
LCNV | 1.00 | 35.00 | <0.001 | 0.992 | <0.001 | |
HCNV | 1.00 | 35.00 | 5.264 | 0.028 * | 0.141 | |
DSQ | 1.00 | 34.00 | 3.620 | 0.066 | 0.102 | |
PCMI | 1.00 | 34.00 | 0.031 | 0.862 | 0.001 | |
Levator Labii Activation | 1.00 | 35.00 | 5.037 | 0.032 * | 0.140 | |
DSQ | 1.00 | 34.00 | 0.421 | 0.521 | 0.013 | |
PCMI | 1.00 | 34.00 | 0.029 | 0.521 | 0.001 |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Walsh, D.; Foster, J. A Contagious Other? Exploring the Public’s Appraisals of Contact with ‘Mental Illness’. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 2005. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17062005
Walsh D, Foster J. A Contagious Other? Exploring the Public’s Appraisals of Contact with ‘Mental Illness’. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(6):2005. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17062005
Chicago/Turabian StyleWalsh, Daniel, and Juliet Foster. 2020. "A Contagious Other? Exploring the Public’s Appraisals of Contact with ‘Mental Illness’" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 6: 2005. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17062005
APA StyleWalsh, D., & Foster, J. (2020). A Contagious Other? Exploring the Public’s Appraisals of Contact with ‘Mental Illness’. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(6), 2005. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17062005