COVID-19 Scientific Facts vs. Conspiracy Theories: Is Science Failing to Pass Its Message?
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedure
2.3. Measures
3. Results
3.1. Conspiracy Theory Belief and Adherence Behavior
3.2. Conspiracy Theory Beliefs and Psychological State
3.3. Conspiracy Theory Beliefs and Trust in Science
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Van Prooijen, J.-W.; Douglas, K.M. Belief in conspiracy theories: Basic principles of an emerging research domain. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 48, 897–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Douglas, K.M. COVID-19 conspiracy theories. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 2021, 24, 270–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- World Health Organization. Countering Misinformation About COVID-19: A Joint Campaign with the Government of the United Kingdom. 2020. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/feature-stories/detail/countering-misinformation-about-covid-19 (accessed on 31 December 2020).
- De Coninck, D.; Frissen, T.; Matthijs, K.; D’Haenens, L.; Lits, G.; Champagne-Poirier, O.; Carignan, M.-E.; David, M.D.; Pignard-Cheynel, N.; Salerno, S.; et al. Beliefs in Conspiracy Theories and Misinformation About COVID-19: Comparative Perspectives on the Role of Anxiety, Depression and Exposure to and Trust in Information Sources. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 646394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fornaro, M.; Clementi, N.; Fornaro, P. Medicine and psychiatry in Western culture: Ancient Greek myths and modern prejudices. Ann. Gen. Psychiatry 2009, 8, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cartwright, M. Greek Mythology. World History Encyclopedia. 2012. Available online: https://www.worldhistory.org/Greek_Mythology/ (accessed on 31 May 2020).
- Masse, W.B.; Barber, E.W.; Piccardi, L.; Barber, P.T. Exploring the nature of myth and its role in science. Geol. Soc. Lond. Spec. Publ. 2007, 273, 9–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ünal, Ö.; Cesur-Atintaş, M.; Kiremitçi-Canıöz, E.; Kaya, H.; Yağmurcu, Y. The predictors of COVID-19 anxiety and helping behaviour during the pandemic: An investigation within the framework of individual and national level resources. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Franks, B.; Bangerter, A.; Bauer, M.W. Conspiracy theories as quasi-religious mentality: An integrated account from cognitive science, social representations theory, and frame theory. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Swami, V. Social Psychological Origins of Conspiracy Theories: The Case of the Jewish Conspiracy Theory in Malaysia. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dentith, M.R. Conspiracy Theories and Philosophy—Bringing the Epistemology of a Freighted Term into the Social Sciences. In Conspiracy Theories & the People Who Believe Them; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018; pp. 94–108. [Google Scholar]
- Grzesiak-Feldman, M. The Effect of High-Anxiety Situations on Conspiracy Thinking. Curr. Psychol. 2013, 32, 100–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taher, A.M.; Mannan, A.; Dulal, S.; Gida, I.H. Contribution of Greek mythology and civilization on medical science—A brief analysis. Int. J. Adv. Res. 2018, 6, 1095–1102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ullah, I.; Khan, K.S.; Tahir, M.J.; Ahmed, A.; Harapan, H. Myths and conspiracy theories on vaccines and COVID-19: Potential effect on global vaccine refusals. Vacunas 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Naslund, J.A.; Bondre, A.; Torous, J.; Aschbrenner, K.A. Social Media and Mental Health: Benefits, Risks, and Opportunities for Research and Practice. J. Technol. Behav. Sci. 2020, 5, 245–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Naslund, J.A.; Aschbrenner, K.A.; Marsch, L.A.; Bartels, S.J. The future of mental health care: Peer-to-peer support and social media. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 2016, 25, 113–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kareklas, I.; Muehling, D.D.; Weber, T. Reexamining Health Messages in the Digital Age: A Fresh Look at Source Credibility Effects. J. Advert. 2015, 44, 88–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vlasceanu, M.; Coman, A. Mnemonic accessibility affects statement believability: The effect of listening to others selectively practicing beliefs. Cognition 2018, 180, 238–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swami, V.; Weis, L.; Lay, A.; Barron, D.; Furnham, A. Associations between belief in conspiracy theories and the maladaptive personality traits of the personality inventory for DSM-5. Psychiatry Res. 2015, 236, 86–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Klein, C.; Clutton, P.; Dunn, A.G. Pathways to conspiracy: The social and linguistic precursors of involvement in Reddit’s conspiracy theory forum. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0225098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Swami, V.; Coles, R.; Stieger, S.; Pietschnig, J.; Furnham, A.; Rehim, S.; Voracek, M. Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories. Br. J. Psychol. 2011, 102, 443–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swami, V.; Voracek, M.; Stieger, S.; Tran, U.S.; Furnham, A. Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition 2014, 133, 572–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cherry, K. Why People Believe in Conspiracy Theories, Verywellmind. 2019. Available online: https://www.verywellmind.com/why-people-believe-in-conspiracy-theories-4690335 (accessed on 31 May 2020).
- Douglas, K.M.; Sutton, R.M.; Cichocka, A. The Psychology of Conspiracy Theories. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 26, 538–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oliver, J.E.; Wood, T. Medical Conspiracy Theories and Health Behaviors in the United States. JAMA Intern. Med. 2014, 174, 817–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Islam, S.; Kamal, A.-H.M.; Kabir, A.; Southern, D.L.; Khan, S.H.; Hasan, S.M.M.; Sarkar, T.; Sharmin, S.; Das, S.; Roy, T.; et al. COVID-19 vaccine rumors and conspiracy theories: The need for cognitive inoculation against misinformation to improve vaccine adherence. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0251605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hale, T.; Angrist, N.; Goldszmidt, R.; Kira, B.; Petherick, A.; Phillips, T.; Webster, S.; Cameron-Blake, E.; Hallas, L.; Majumdar, S.; et al. A global panel database of pandemic policies (Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker). Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021, 5, 529–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turon, H.; Carey, M.; Boyes, A.; Hobden, B.; Dilworth, S.; Sanson-Fisher, R. Agreement between a single-item measure of anxiety and depression and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale: A cross-sectional study. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0210111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Mackenzie, L.J.; Carey, M.L.; Sanson-Fisher, R.W.; D’Este, C.A.; Paul, C.L.; Yoong, S.L. Agreement between HADS classifications and single-item screening questions for anxiety and depression: A cross-sectional survey of cancer patients. Ann. Oncol. 2014, 25, 889–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Young, Q.-R.; Nguyen, M.; Roth, S.; Broadberry, A.; Mackay, M.H. Single-item measures for depression and anxiety: Validation of the Screening Tool for Psychological Distress in an inpatient cardiology setting. Eur. J. Cardiovasc. Nurs. 2015, 14, 544–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stevens, J. Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences, 4th ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- The Guardian. Survey Uncovers Widespread Belief in ‘Dangerous’ Covid Conspiracy Theories: False Claims That Pandemic Is a Hoax or Was Started Deliberately are Attracting Adherents Around World. 2021. Available online: https://www.theguardian.com/world/2020/oct/26/survey-uncovers-widespread-belief-dangerous-covid-conspiracy-theories (accessed on 31 May 2021).
- Roozenbeek, J.; Schneider, C.R.; Dryhurst, S.; Kerr, J.; Freeman, A.L.J.; Recchia, G.; van der Bles, A.M.; van der Linden, S. Susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19 around the world. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2020, 7, 201199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kuhn, S.A.K.; Lieb, R.; Freeman, D.; Andreou, C.; Zander-Schellenberg, T. Coronavirus conspiracy beliefs in the German-speaking general population: Endorsement rates and links to reasoning biases and paranoia. Psychol. Med. 2021, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allington, D. Why Conspiracy Theories Are Not Just a Harmless Joke, New Statesman, Observations. 2018. Available online: https://www.newstatesman.com/politics/uk/2018/11/why-conspiracy-theories-are-not-just-harmless-joke (accessed on 31 May 2020).
- Allington, D. COVID-19 Conspiracy Theories Present ‘Substantial’ Health Risk, New Research Finds. Kings College London News Center Spotlight on Research. 2020. Available online: https://www.kcl.ac.uk/news/covid-19-conspiracy-theorists-present-substantial-health-risk-new-research-finds (accessed on 30 December 2020).
- Douglas, K.M.; Uscinski, J.E.; Sutton, R.M.; Cichocka, A.; Nefes, T.; Ang, C.S.; Deravi, F. Understanding Conspiracy Theories. Political Psychol. 2019, 40, 3–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nattrass, N. Understanding the origins and prevalence of AIDS conspiracy beliefs in the United States and South Africa. Sociol. Health Illn. 2012, 35, 113–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oleksy, T.; Wnuk, A.; Maison, D.; Łyś, A. Content matters. Different predictors and social consequences of general and government-related conspiracy theories on COVID-19. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2021, 168, 110289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Romer, D.; Jamieson, K.H. Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the U.S. Soc. Sci. Med. 2020, 263, 113356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eurostat Education Attainment Statistics, Tertiary Education Attainment. 2020. Available online: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/statistics-explained/index.php?title=Educational_attainment_statistics (accessed on 31 December 2020).
- Grimes, D.R. Medical disinformation and the unviable nature of COVID-19 conspiracy theories. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0245900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- World Health Organization. Ambient (Outdoor) Air Quality and Health. 2021. Available online: http://www.who.int (accessed on 31 January 2021).
How Strongly Do You Believe Each of the Following Statements: | Avg (SD) | SEM | Factor CB Loading | % No-to-Weak Belief | % Moderate Belief | % Strong Belief |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. COVID-19 is not real | 1.99 (1.75) | 0.06 | 0.620 | 63.70 | 12.70 | 23.60 |
2. There is already a vaccine for COVID-19 and will be released when millions are infected | 4.27 (2.83) | 0.90 | 0.815 | 35.80 | 16.90 | 47.30 |
3. Deaths from COVID-19 in Italy, Spain, and USA are not as many as reported | 3.48 (2.73) | 0.09 | 0.611 | 36.20 | 40.50 | 23.30 |
4. Nobody really died from COVID-19 | 2.24 (2.53) | 0.08 | 0.382 | 72.00 | 16.70 | 11.30 |
5. People dying from COVID-19 would have died very soon, anyway | 3.05 (2.51) | 0.08 | 0.539 | 45.80 | 36.00 | 18.20 |
6. I am generally a believer of conspiracy theories | 3.55 (2.70) | 0.09 | 0.794 | 33.70 | 43.60 | 22.70 |
7. With COVID-19 vaccinations we will be microchipped unwillingly | 3.78 (2.97) | 0.09 | 0.786 | 34.50 | 40.90 | 23.60 |
8. COVID-19 was created for population control | 4.37 (3.01) | 0.10 | 0.856 | 26.30 | 54.90 | 18.80 |
9. COVID-19 was created on purpose in a laboratory by scientists | 5.20 (3.04) | 0.10 | 0.781 | 26.6 | 29.90 | 43.50 |
CB | AB | PS | TS | Sex | Age | Education | Living Area | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AB | −0.33 ** | |||||||
PS | 0.13 ** | 0.05 | ||||||
TS | −0.47 ** | 0.33 ** | 0.01 | |||||
Sex | −0.10 ** | −0.14 ** | −0.13 ** | 0.06 | ||||
Age | −0.14 ** | 0.14 ** | −0.03 | 0.10 ** | 0.08 * | |||
Education | −0.19 ** | 0.10 * | −0.06 * | 0.06 | −0.05 | –0.04 | ||
Living Area | −0.17 ** | 0.15 ** | −0.04 | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.07 * | 0.04 | |
Income | −0.18 ** | 0.10 * | −0.01 | 0.09 * | 0.14 ** | 0.39 ** | 0.17 ** | 0.05 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Constantinou, M.; Kagialis, A.; Karekla, M. COVID-19 Scientific Facts vs. Conspiracy Theories: Is Science Failing to Pass Its Message? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 6343. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18126343
Constantinou M, Kagialis A, Karekla M. COVID-19 Scientific Facts vs. Conspiracy Theories: Is Science Failing to Pass Its Message? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(12):6343. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18126343
Chicago/Turabian StyleConstantinou, Marios, Antonios Kagialis, and Maria Karekla. 2021. "COVID-19 Scientific Facts vs. Conspiracy Theories: Is Science Failing to Pass Its Message?" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 12: 6343. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18126343
APA StyleConstantinou, M., Kagialis, A., & Karekla, M. (2021). COVID-19 Scientific Facts vs. Conspiracy Theories: Is Science Failing to Pass Its Message? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(12), 6343. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18126343