The Relationship between Social Environmental Factors and Motor Performance in 3- to 12-Year-Old Typically Developing Children: A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Search Strategy
2.2. Selection Criteria
2.3. Data Extraction
2.4. Quality Assessment
3. Results
3.1. Included Studies
Study | Sample Size (♂/♀) | Mean Age ± SD, range | Type of Motor Skill Performance Instrument Used to Measure | Type of Social Environmental Factor Instrument Used to Measure | Result |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Barnett et al. 2013 [41] | 76 (34/42) | 4.1 years ± 0.68, 3.0–6.0 years | Locomotor performance, object control performance Test of Gross Motor Development-2 | Parental interaction in child’s PA, parental moderate- and vigorous-intensity PA, parental confidence in their own skills to support child’s activity Parental questionnaire | Parents’ confidence in their own skills was associated with object control performance (ß = 0.23, p = 0.038) |
Barnett et al. 2019 [31] | 178 (sex unspecified) | 5 years a | Locomotor performance, object control performance Test of Gross Motor Development-2 | Time spent being physically active with mum, time spent with children of a similar age, time spent with older children, parental behaviors (parental facilitation of PA, maternal PA), maternal beliefs (PA optimism, PA self-efficacy) all measured at 4 months, 9 months, 19 months, 3.5 years Maternal beliefs (PA knowledge and PA views measured at 4 months, 19 months, 3.5 years, floor concerns measured at 4 months) Parental questionnaire | Spending time with older children at 3.5 years (ß = 3.00, p < 0.05) and maternal optimism at 4 months (ß = 2.43, p < 0.05) were positively associated with locomotor performance. Time spent being physically active with mum at 3.5 years (ß = −3.73, p < 0.05) and maternal PA at 9 months (ß = −0.01, p < 0.05) were negatively associated with locomotor performance. Spending more time with older children at 4 months (ß = 2.27, p < 0.05) and 19 months (ß = 2.97, p < 0.05) was positively associated with object control performance. Maternal PA knowledge at 3.5 years (ß = −3.05, p < 0.05) was negatively associated with children’s object control performance |
Bindman et al. 2014 [42] | 135 (63/72) | 4.56 years ± 0.55, 3.58–5.81 years | Fine motor performance Early Screening Inventory-Revised | Parental graphophonemic support, Parental print support, Parental demand for precision Observation during a writing task with mother | High levels of graphophonemic support were positively associated with fine motor performance (ß = 0.20, p = 0.014) |
Cao et al. 2014 [33] | 89 (42/47) | 5.5 years a | Balance, bilateral coordination Bruininks–Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency Visual-motor integration The Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration | Maternal anxiety measured at 6 months and 5.5 years General Health Questionnaire-28 | No significant relationships |
Chaves et al. 2015 [37] | 390 (186/204) | 8.50 years ± 1.27, 6.0–9.99 years | Gross motor performance Körperkoordinationtest für Kinder | School size (number of students) Obtained from school | School size is negatively related to gross motor performance (ß = −0.39, p = 0.005) |
Comuk-Balci et al. 2016 [43] | 437 (sex unspecified) | 208 children between 41–56 months, 229 children between 57–80 months | Fine motor performance Denver II developmental screening test | Number of children at home Parental questionnaire | Number of children at home was negatively correlated with 1 out of the 5 fine motor tasks in 41–56 months (r = −0.152, p < 0.05) |
Cools et al. 2011 [44] | 846 (471/375) | 5.1 years± 0.6, 4.0–7.0 years | Fundamental motor performance Motor Proficiency Test for 4–6-Year-Old Children | Parental work status (full-time, part-time), family situation (single- or two-parent families), Parental behaviors: parent’s PA, involvement in the children’s play activities, transport habits of the family, and school involvement, Parental beliefs: parental importance rating on developmental and rearing aspects, PA characteristics, and PA equipment characteristics Parental questionnaire | Paternal PA (r = 0.13, p < 0.01), father’s involvement in active play (r = 0.11, p < 0.05), and parental beliefs on the importance of the child’s PA (r = 0.12, p < 0.01), supporting motor development (r = 0.12, p < 0.01) and sport specificity of PA (r = 0.19, p < 0.01) were related to boys’ fundamental motor performance. Maternal (r = −0.12, p < 0.05) and paternal (r = −0.14, p < 0.01) school involvement, father’s involvement in creative play (r = −13, p < 0.01) and dance activities (r = −0.12, p < 0.05), mother’s involvement in gaming (r = −0.14, p < 0.05), and parental beliefs on the importance of winning (r = −0.16, p < 0.05) were related to girls’ fundamental motor performance |
de Oliveira & Jackson 2017 [45] | 47 (27/20) | 4.67 years ± 0.93, preschoolers a | Fine motor performance Teacher-rated 3-point Likert scale on nine fine motor items taken from normative developmental charts | Rates of maternal verbal support, mother’s encouragement of the child’s autonomy, maternal emotional support, maternal physical support Observation during a building task with mother | In the Somewhat Difficult task, maternal cognitive (r = −0.33, p < 0.05) and emotional (r = −0.31, p = 0.05) support were negatively correlated to fine motor performance. In the More Difficult task, maternal autonomy support (r = −30, p = 0.05) was negatively related to fine motor performance |
Fabes et al. 2003 [46] | 98 (50/48) | 54.77 months ± 10.50, 35–72 months | Perceptual-motor competence Teacher rated five-item scale that measured children’s locomotor, perceptual, and physical skills | Interacting with same-sex peers or in mixed-sex groups Observation during free play | No significant relationships |
Giagazoglou et al. 2011 [47] | 412 (208/204) | 61 months ± 7.7, 4.0–6.9 years | Manual dexterity, object control performance, balance Movement Assessment Battery for Children—2nd Edition | Birth order position Unspecified | No significant relationships |
Herry et al. 2007 [48] | 821 (406/415) | 59.4 months, 48–60 months | Psychomotor performance Early Development Instrument | Number of children per class, family structure (Two- or single-parent households) Questionnaire for teachers and parents | The number of children per class were significantly associated with motor performance |
Hua et al. 2016 [49] | 4001 (2067/1934) | 3.0–6.0 years a | Manual dexterity, object control performance, balance Movement Assessment Battery for Children—2nd Edition | Family structure (single families, nuclear families, extended families) Parental questionnaire Parental rearing behaviors (encouragement of children’s activities/games, teaching verbs, related activities, developing children’s habits, and others) Family Environment checklist on Motor Development for Urban Pre-school Children Class interaction (including amount of supervision, discipline, interaction between teacher and child and interaction between children) Early Childhood Environment Rating Scale–Revised | Parental rearing behaviors were positively related to total motor performance (ß = 0.119, p < 0.001), manual dexterity (ß = 0.034, p < 0.01), object control performance (ß = 0.062, p < 0.05), and balance (ß = 0.024, p < 0.05). Class interaction was positively related to total motor performance (ß = 0.139, p < 0.01) and balance (ß = 0.184, p < 0.001) |
Jensen et al. 2019 [50] | 130 (sex unspecified) | 36 months a | Gross and fine motor performance Adapted version of Mullen Scales of Early Leaning | Maternal distress (stress and depressive symptoms) Bangla version of the Edinburg Postnatal Depression Scale, Perceived Stress Scale Cognitive stimulation Family Care Indicators | Cognitive stimulation correlated to gross (r = 0.216, p < 0.05) and fine motor performance (r = 0.186, p < 0.05). |
Krombholz 2006 [51] | 1194 (638/556) | 43–84 months | Gross motor performance Forward balancing, hopping on one foot, and 2 items of the Körperkoordinationtest für Kinder (backward balancing, lateral jump) Manual dexterity Paper-and-pencil test | Birth order position Unspecified | Children with older sibling outperformed only or firstborn children on balancing, lateral jump, and hopping on the right foot (no test results) |
Kumar et al. 2016 [36] | 321 | 3.0–9.9 years a | Total motor performance Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale | Joint or nuclear family types, occupation of mother (housewife, working) Parental questionnaire | No significant relationships |
Lejarraga et al. 2002 [52] | Heel-to-toe walking: 1182; Copy cross: 996; Draw a person in six parts: 1455 | Heel-to-toe walking: 2.83–5.30 years; Copy cross: 3.12–5.20 years; Draw a person in six parts: 3.48–5.93 years | Gross and fine motor performance Score on the developmental items “Copy cross”, “Draw person six parts”, and “Heel-to-toe walk” | Family size, father living at home, birth order position Interview with parents | Birth order position was significant for the fine motor task “Copy cross” (OR = 1.47, 95% CI [1.08, 2.02], p < 0.05) and the gross motor task “Heel-to-toe walk” (OR = 0.68, 95% CI [0.50, 0.92], p < 0.05) |
Lin & Li 2019 [53] | 163 (87/76) | 38.73 months ± 4.91, 24–47 months | Fine and gross motor performance China Development Scale for Children | Mothers’ play beliefs Chinese Parent Play Beliefs Scale | No significant relationships |
Lin et al. 2020 [54] | 163 (87/76) | 38.73 months ± 4.91, 36–47 months | Fine and gross motor performance China Developmental Scale for Children | Parental play beliefs Chinese Parent Play Beliefs Scale Single child Demographic questionnaire | Children of fathers, who placed a higher value on early academics than on free play, showed poorer gross motor performance than children of fathers who rated free play as more important (F(1, 146) = 3.63, p = 0.05) |
Livesey et al. 2011 [39] | 192 (80/112) | 129 months ± 11.1, 105–147 months | Total motor performance Movement Assessment Battery for Children—2nd Edition | Sociometric preference during play and schoolwork Peer Rating Scale Teacher ratings of peer exclusion Peer Exclusion subscale of the Child Behavior Scale | For boys, sociometric preference during play (r = −0.228, p < 0.05) and schoolwork (r = −0.245, p < 0.05) was related to total motor performance. Peer exclusion was related to total motor performance for both boys (r = 0.447, p < 0.01) and girls (r = 0.348, p < 0.01). |
Lung et al. 2011 [55] | 1412 (sex unspecified) | 36 months a | Gross and fine motor performance The Taiwan Birth Cohort Study instrument | Maternal self-perceived health status 36-Item Short Form Health Survey Family support, pressure from childcare, number of children in the family, parental marital status Reported by mother | Marriage was a predictor for gross motor performance (β = 0.66, p = 0.026). Pressure from childcare (β = −0.11, p = 0.011), number of children at home (β = 0.14, p = 0.021), family support (β = 0.14, p = 0.042), and maternal mental health status (β = −0.02, p = 0.047) were predictors of fine motor performance |
Luz et al. 2018 [38] | 173 (89/84) | 8.57 years ± 0.60, 7.00–9.90 years | Gross motor performance Körperkoordinationtest für Kinder | Maternal PA International Physical Activity Questionnaire | Maternal PA was a predictor of motor coordination of girls (OR = 0.183; 95% CI [0.052, 0.642]) |
Moller, Forbes-Jones, & Hightower 2008 [35] | 770 (411/395) | 4.15 years ± 0.50, preschoolers a | Total motor performance development over half a year Teacher-rated Child Observation Record | Number of children in a class, range of chronological age in a class Unspecified Range in developmental age in a class Teacher-rated Child Observation Record | Chronological age range (ß = −0.60, p < 0.05) and developmental age range (ß = −0.46, p < 0.05) were negative predictors of total motor performance development |
Moller, Forbes-Jones, Hightower, et al. 2008 [34] | 770 (411/395) | 4.15 years ± 0.50, preschoolers a | Total motor performance development over half a year Teacher-rated Child Observation Record | Number of children in a class, classroom sex composition Unspecified | No significant relationships |
Peyre et al. 2019 [32] | 1144 (611/533) | 67.8 months ± 1.8, 60–72 months | Motor performance development between 3 and 5 years. A mean score consisting of gross and fine motor performance and visual–motor integration Ages and Stage 2 Questionnaire for gross and fine motor skills; Copy Design task for visual–motor integration | Single-parent household after birth, main caretaker at 2 years, presence of younger and older siblings at 5 years Parental questionnaire Maternal cognitive stimulation measured at 2, 3 and 5–6 years Home Observation for the Measurement of the Environment | Maternal cognitive stimulation at 5 years was associated with motor performance development (β = 0.05, p = 0.021) |
Sartori et al. 2017 [40] | 82 (sex unspecified) | 8.5 years ± 0.7, 8.0–9.0 years | Manual dexterity, object control performance, balance Movement Assessment Battery for Children—2nd Edition | Maltreatment and abuse Children were recruited from foster homes after separation from parents due to parental neglect and domestic violence | Children who were maltreated and abused performed worse on balance (F(1.80) = 9.340, p = 0.003) |
Simcock et al. 2018 [56] | 113 (59/54) | 48.65 months ± 0.91, 45–51 months | Fine and gross motor performance Ages and Stages 3 Questionnaire | Maternal composite subjective stress Calculated from the Impact of Event Scale-Revised, peritraumatic distress inventory, and peritraumatic dissociative experiences questionnaire Concurrent anxiety, concurrent depression Depression, anxiety and stress scale Marital status (married or de facto vs. single/separated/divorced) Unspecified | No significant relationships |
Taverna et al. 2011 [57] | 77 (43/34) | 53.31 months ± 9.67, 3.0–5.0 years | Gross and fine motor performance Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales | Culture-sensitive socialization processes (mother social support, father involvement with the family, child autonomy, family connectedness, family involvement in mealtimes) Ecocultural Family Interview | Family connectedness (r = 0.25, p < 0.05) and family involvement in mealtimes (r = 0.23, p < 0.05) were positively associated with fine motor performance |
True et al. 2017 [58] | 229 (118/111) | 4.2 years ± 0.7, 3.0–5.0 years | Locomotor performance, object control performance, total gross motor performance The Children’s Activity and Movement in Preschool Study (CHAMPS) Motor Skills Protocol | Teacher education Reported by director from preschool | Teacher education was a predictor of total motor score (ß = 0.22, p < 0.01) and locomotor performance (ß = 0.14, p < 0.001) |
Wolf & McCoy 2019 [59] | 2137 (1064/1073) | 5.16 years ± 1.34, preschoolers a | Fine motor performance International Development and Early Learning Assessment | Caregivers’ cognitive stimulation Six adapted questions from the Multiple Indicators Cluster SurveyCaregiver school-based involvementSelf-reported by caregiver | Caregiver school involvement was a predictor of fine motor performance (ß = 0.08, p < 0.05). |
Wu et al. 2012 [60] | 19,499 (10,237/9262) | 3 years a | Fine motor performance The Taiwan Birth Cohort Study instrument | Home environment (cognitive stimulation and emotional support) Adaptation from the Home Observation for the Measurement of Environment Inventory—Short Form | Home environment was associated with fine motor performance (β = 0.05, p < 0.001) |
Zeng et al. 2019 [61] | 100/128 | 56.08 months ± 4.09 | Balance, locomotor performance, object control performance Bruininks–Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, 2nd edition | Number of children in family, parent work status Parental questionnaire | No significant relationships |
3.2. Quality Assessment
3.3. Home Environment
3.3.1. Parental Characteristics
3.3.2. Parental Beliefs
3.3.3. Parental Behaviors
3.3.4. Siblings
3.4. School Environment
4. Discussion
4.1. Home Environment
4.1.1. Parents
4.1.2. Siblings
4.2. School Environment
4.3. Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Adolph, K.E.; Hoch, J.E. Motor Development: Embodied, Embedded, Enculturated, and Enabling. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2019, 70, 141–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lubans, D.R.; Morgan, P.J.; Cliff, D.P.; Barnett, L.M.; Okely, A.D. Fundamental movement skills in children and adolescents: Review of associated health benefits. Sports Med. 2010, 40, 1019–1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lloyd, M.; Saunders, T.J.; Bremer, E.; Tremblay, M.S. Long-term importance of fundamental motor skills: A 20-year follow-up study. Adapt. Phys. Activ. Q. 2014, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kantomaa, M.T.; Stamatakis, E.; Kankaanpää, A.; Kaakinen, M.; Rodriguez, A.; Taanila, A.; Ahonen, T.; Järvelin, M.R.; Tammelin, T. Physical activity and obesity mediate the association between childhood motor function and adolescents’ academic achievement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 1917–1922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Runhaar, J.; Collard, D.C.M.; Singh, A.S.; Kemper, H.C.G.; van Mechelen, W.; Chinapaw, M. Motor fitness in Dutch youth: Differences over a 26-year period (1980–2006). J. Sci. Med. Sport 2010, 13, 323–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tester, G.; Ackland, T.R.; Houghton, L. A 30-Year Journey of Monitoring Fitness and Skill Outcomes in Physical Education: Lessons Learned and a Focus on the Future. Adv. Phys. Educ. 2014, 4, 127–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Timmermans, A.; Hartman, E.; Smits, I.; Hemker, B.; Spithoff, M.; Rekers-Mombarg, L.; Kannekens, R.; Moolenaar, B. Peiling Bewegingsonderwijs 2017. Technische Rapportage. [Survey Movement Education 2017. Technical Report]; GION Onderwijs/Onderzoek: Groningen, The Netherlands, 2017; pp. 155–163. [Google Scholar]
- Cummins, A.; Piek, J.P.; Dyck, M.J. Motor coordination, empathy, and social behaviour in school-aged children. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2005, 47, 437–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Piek, J.P.; Bradbury, G.S.; Elsley, S.C.; Tate, L. Motor coordination and social-emotional behaviour in preschool-aged children. Int. J. Disabil. Dev. Educ. 2008, 55, 143–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rogers, A.T. Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 2nd ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 171–173. [Google Scholar]
- Motor Skills. Encyclopedia of Child Behavior and Development; Goldstein, S., Naglieri, J.A., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Piek, J.P.; Hands, B.; Licari, M.K. Assessment of motor functioning in the preschool period. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2012, 22, 402–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mah, V.K.; Ford-Jones, E.L. Spotlight on middle childhood: Rejuvenating the “forgotten years”. Paediatr. Child Health 2012, 17, 81–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thomas, K.; Thomas, J.R. Principles of motor development for elementary school physical education. Elem. Sch. J. 2008, 108, 181–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Venetsanou, F.; Kambas, A. Environmental factors affecting preschoolers’ motor development. Early Child. Educ. J. 2010, 37, 319–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newell, K.M. Constraints on the Development of Coordination. In Motor Development in Children: Aspects of Coordination and Control; Martinus Nijhoff: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1986; pp. 341–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bredland, E.L.; Söderström, S.; Vik, K. Challenges and motivators to physical activity faced by retired men when ageing: A qualitative study. BMC Public Health 2018, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Barnett, L.M.; Lai, S.K.; Veldman, S.L.C.; Hardy, L.L.; Cliff, D.P.; Morgan, P.J.; Zask, A.; Lubans, D.R.; Shultz, S.P.; Ridgers, N.D.; et al. Correlates of Gross Motor Competence in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Sport. Med. 2016, 46, 1663–1688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Flôres, F.S.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Copetti, F.; Lopes, F.; Cordovil, R. Affordances for Motor Skill Development in Home, School, and Sport Environments: A Narrative Review. Percept. Mot. Skills 2019, 126, 366–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Golding, J.; Emmett, P.; Iles-Caven, Y.; Steer, C.; Lingam, R. A review of environmental contributions to childhood motor skills. J. Child Neurol. 2014, 29, 1531–1547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Furman, W.; Buhrmester, D. The Contribution of Siblings and Peers to the Parenting Process. In Child Nurturance: Patterns of Supplementary Parenting; Kostelnik, M.J., Rabin, A.I., Phenice, L.A., Soderman, A.K., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1982; pp. 69–100. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura, A.; McClelland, D.C. Social Learning Theory; Prentice-hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1977; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
- Goldenberg, G.; Karnath, H.O. The neural basis of imitation is body part specific. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 6282–6287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Barnett, E.; Casper, M. A definition of “social environment”. Am. J. Public Health 2001, 91, 465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Caspersen, C.; Powell, K.; Christenson, G. Physical activity, exercise, and physical fitness: Definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health Rep 1985, 100, 126–131. [Google Scholar]
- Haga, M. The relationship between physical fitness and motor competence in children. Child. Care. Health Dev. 2008, 34, 329–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bramer, W.M.; Giustini, D.; De Jong, G.B.; Holland, L.; Bekhuis, T. De-duplication of database search results for systematic reviews in endnote. J. Med. Libr. Assoc. 2016, 104, 240–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayden, J.A.; Côté, P.; Bombardier, C. Evaluation of the quality of prognosis studies in systematic reviews. Ann. Intern. Med. 2006, 144, 427–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayden, J.A.; van der Windt, D.A.; Cartwright, J.L.; Côté, P.; Bombardier, C. Assessing bias in studies of prognostic factors. Ann. Intern. Med. 2013, 158, 280–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barnett, L.M.; Hnatiuk, J.A.; Salmon, J.; Hesketh, K.D. Modifiable factors which predict children’s gross motor competence: A prospective cohort study. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2019, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Peyre, H.; Albaret, J.M.; Bernard, J.Y.; Hoertel, N.; Melchior, M.; Forhan, A.; Taine, M.; Heude, B.; De Agostini, M.; Galéra, C.; et al. Developmental trajectories of motor skills during the preschool period. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2019, 28, 1461–1474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cao, X.; Laplante, D.P.; Brunet, A.; Ciampi, A.; King, S. Prenatal maternal stress affects motor function in 51/2-year-old children: Project Ice Storm. Dev. Psychobiol. 2014, 56, 117–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moller, A.C.; Forbes-Jones, E.; Hightower, A.D.; Friedman, R. The developmental influence of sex composition in preschool classrooms: Boys fare worse in preschool classrooms with more boys. Early Child. Res. Q. 2008, 23, 409–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moller, A.C.; Forbes-Jones, E.; Hightower, A.D. Classroom Age Composition and Developmental Change in 70 Urban Preschool Classrooms. J. Educ. Psychol. 2008, 100, 741–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kumar, R.; Shankar, K.; Kush, V.; Kumar, C.; Bhave, A.; Agarwal, V. Adaptation: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale for 3-9 year-old Indian children. Int. J. Disabil. Hum. Dev. 2016, 15, 49–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chaves, R.; Baxter-Jones, A.; Gomes, T.; Souza, M.; Pereira, S.; Maia, J. Effects of individual and school-level characteristics on a child’s gross motor coordination development. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 8883–8896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luz, L.G.O.; Valente-dos-Santos, J.; Luz, T.D.D.; Sousa-e-Silva, P.; Duarte, J.P.; Machado-Rodrigues, A.; Seabra, A.; Santos, R.; Cumming, S.P.; Coelho-e-Silva, M.J. Biocultural Predictors of Motor Coordination Among Prepubertal Boys and Girls. Percept. Mot. Skills 2018, 125, 21–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Livesey, D.; Lum Mow, M.; Toshack, T.; Zheng, Y. The relationship between motor performance and peer relations in 9- to 12-year-old children. Child. Care. Health Dev. 2011, 37, 581–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sartori, R.F.; Ribeiro Bandeira, P.F.; Nobre, G.C.; da Silva Ramalho, M.H.; Valentini, N.C. Associations between motor proficiency in children with history of maltreatment and living in social economically vulnerability. Child Abus. Negl. 2017, 70, 75–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barnett, L.M.; Hinkley, T.; Okely, A.D.; Salmon, J. Child, family and environmental correlates of children’s motor skill proficiency. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2013, 16, 332–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bindman, S.W.; Skibbe, L.E.; Hindman, A.H.; Aram, D.; Morrison, F.J. Parental writing support and preschoolers’ early literacy, language, and fine motor skills. Early Child. Res. Q. 2014, 29, 614–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Comuk-Balci, N.; Bayoglu, B.; Tekindal, A.; Kerem-Gunel, M.; Anlar, B. Screening preschool children for fine motor skills: Environmental influence. J. Phys. Ther. Sci. 2016, 28, 1026–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cools, W.; de Kristine, M.; Samaey, C.; Andries, C. Fundamental movement skill performance of preschool children in relation to family context. J. Sports Sci. 2011, 29, 649–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Oliveira, E.A.; Jackson, E.A. The Moderation Role of Self-perceived Maternal Empathy in Observed Mother-Child Collaborative Problem Solving. Infant Child Dev. 2017, 26, e1993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fabes, R.A.; Martin, C.L.; Hanish, L.D.; Anders, M.C.; Madden-Derdich, D.A. Early School Competence: The Roles of Sex-Segregated Play and Effortful Control. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 39, 848–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giagazoglou, P.; Kabitsis, N.; Kokaridas, D.; Zaragas, C.; Katartzi, E.; Kabitsis, C. The movement assessment battery in Greek preschoolers: The impact of age, gender, birth order, and physical activity on motor outcome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 2577–2582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Herry, Y.; Maltais, C.; Thompson, K. Effects of a Full-Day Preschool Program on 4-Year Old Children. Early Child. Res. Pract. 2007, 9, n2. [Google Scholar]
- Hua, J.; Duan, T.; Gu, G.; Wo, D.; Zhu, Q.; Liu, J.Q.; Liu, M.; Wu, Z.; Meng, W. Effects of home and education environments on children’s motor performance in China. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2016, 58, 868–876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jensen, S.K.G.; Tofail, F.; Haque, R.; Petri, W.A.; Nelson, C.A. Child development in the context of biological and psychosocial hazards among poor families in Bangladesh. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0215304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krombholz, H. Physical performance in relation to age, sex, birth order, social class, and sports activities of preschool children. Percept. Mot. Skills 2006, 102, 477–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lejarraga, H.; Pascucci, M.C.; Krupitzky, S.; Kelmansky, D.; Bianco, A.; Martínez, E.; Tibaldi, F.; Cameron, N. Psychomotor development in Argentinean children aged 0-5 years. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2002, 16, 47–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lin, X.; Li, H. Chinese mothers’ profile which values both play and academics predicts better developmental outcome in young children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2019, 43, 61–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lin, X.; Li, H.; Yang, W. You Reap What You Sow: Profiles of Chinese Fathers’ Play Beliefs and Their Relation to Young Children’s Developmental Outcomes. Early Educ. Dev. 2020, 31, 426–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lung, F.W.; Shu, B.C.; Chiang, T.L.; Lin, S.J. Maternal mental health and childrearing context in the development of children at 6, 18 and 36 months: A Taiwan birth cohort pilot study. Child. Care. Health Dev. 2011, 37, 211–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simcock, G.; Laplante, D.P.; Elgbeili, G.; Kildea, S.; King, S. A trajectory analysis of childhood motor development following stress in pregnancy: The QF2011 flood study. Dev. Psychobiol. 2018, 60, 836–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Taverna, L.; Bornstein, M.H.; Putnick, D.L.; Axia, G. Adaptive behaviors in young children: A unique cultural comparison in Italy. J. Cross. Cult. Psychol. 2011, 42, 445–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- True, L.; Pfeiffer, K.A.; Dowda, M.; Williams, H.G.; Brown, W.H.; O’Neill, J.R.; Pate, R.R. Motor competence and characteristics within the preschool environment. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2017, 20, 751–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wolf, S.; McCoy, D.C. Household Socioeconomic Status and Parental Investments: Direct and Indirect Relations with School Readiness in Ghana. Child Dev. 2019, 90, 260–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, J.C.L.; Bradley, R.H.; Chiang, T.L. Cross-border marriage and disparities in early childhood development in a population-based birth cohort study: The mediation of the home environment. Child. Care. Health Dev. 2012, 38, 595–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zeng, N.; Johnson, S.L.; Boles, R.E.; Bellows, L.L. Social-ecological correlates of fundamental movement skills in young children. J. Sport Health Sci. 2019, 8, 122–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- National Academies of Sciences Engineering and Medicine Parenting Knowledge, Attitudes, and Practices. In Parenting Matters: Supporting Parents of Children Ages 0–8; Breiner, H.; Ford, M.; Gadsden, V.L. (Eds.) National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2016; pp. 45–100. [Google Scholar]
- Vygotsky, L.S. Mind in Society: Development of Higher Psychological Processes; Cole, M., Jolm-Steiner, V., Scribner, S., Souberman, E., Eds.; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1978; p. 86. [Google Scholar]
- Kluczniok, K.; Lehrl, S.; Kuger, S.; Rossbach, H.-G. Quality of the home learning environment during preschool age—Domains and contextual conditions. Eur. Early Child. Educ. Res. J. 2013, 21, 420–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Son, S.H.; Morrison, F.J. The nature and impact of changes in home learning environment on development of language and academic skills in preschool children. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, 1103–1118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sylva, K.; Melhuish, E.; Sammons, P.; Siraj-Blatchford, I.; Taggart, B. The Effective Provision of Pre-school Education (EPPE) Project Technical Paper 12: The Final Report: Effective Pre-school Education; Institute of Education: London, UK, 2004; pp. 4–5. [Google Scholar]
- Saakslahti, A.; Numminen, P.; Niinikoski, H.; Rask-Nissila, L.; Viikari, J.; Tuominen, J.; Valimaki, I. Is physical activity related to body size, fundamental motor skills, and CHD risk factors in early childhood? Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 1999, 11, 327–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dinkel, D.; Snyder, K. Exploring gender differences in infant motor development related to parent’s promotion of play. Infant Behav. Dev. 2020, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thomas, J.R.; Thomas, K.T. Development of gender differences in physical activity. Quest 1988, 40, 219–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amado, D.; Sánchez-Oliva, D.; González-Ponce, I.; Pulido-González, J.J.; Sánchez-Miguel, P.A. Incidence of parental support and pressure on their children’s motivational processes towards sport practice regarding gender. PLoS ONE 2015, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bailey, R.; Cope, E.J.; Pearce, G. Why do children take part in, and remain involved in sport? A literature review and discussion of implications for sports coaches. Int. J. Coach. Sci. 2013, 7, 56–75. [Google Scholar]
- Fuemmeler, B.F.; Anderson, C.B.; Mâsse, L.C. Parent-child relationship of directly measured physical activity. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2011, 8, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Berger, S.E.; Nuzzo, K. Older siblings influence younger siblings’ motor development. Infant Child Dev. 2008, 17, 607–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McHale, S.M.; Updegraff, K.A.; Whiteman, S.D. Sibling Relationships and Influences in Childhood and Adolescence. J. Marriage Fam. 2012, 74, 913–930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rolan, E.P.; Schmitt, S.A.; Purpura, D.J.; Nichols, D.L. Sibling presence, executive function, and the role of parenting. Infant Child Dev. 2018, 27, e2091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindsey, E.W. Girl’s and Boy’s Play Form Preferences and Gender Segregation in Early Childhood. In Psychology of Gender Differences; McGeown, S.P., Ed.; Nova Science Publishers, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 9–24. [Google Scholar]
- Holloway, J.M.; Long, T.M. The Interdependence of Motor and Social Skill Development: Influence on Participation. Phys. Ther. 2019, 99, 761–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wagner, M.O.; Bös, K.; Jascenoka, J.; Jekauc, D.; Petermann, F. Peer problems mediate the relationship between developmental coordination disorder and behavioral problems in school-aged children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 33, 2072–2079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Smyth, M.M.; Anderson, H.I. Coping with clumsiness in the school playground: Social and physical play in children with coordination impairments. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2000, 18, 389–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Filges, T.; Sonne-Schmidt, C.S.; Nielsen, B.C.V. Small class sizes for improving student achievement in primary and secondary schools: A systematic review. Campbell Syst. Rev. 2018, 14, 1–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Coupé, T.; Olefir, A.; Alonso, J.D. Class size, school size and the size of the school network. Educ. Econ. 2016, 24, 329–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chillón, P.; Ortega, F.B.; Ferrando, J.A.; Casajus, J.A. Physical fitness in rural and urban children and adolescents from spain. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2011, 14, 417–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Manning, M.; Wong, G.T.W.; Fleming, C.M.; Garvis, S. Is Teacher Qualification Associated With the Quality of the Early Childhood Education and Care Environment? A Meta-Analytic Review. Rev. Educ. Res. 2019, 89, 370–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bronfenbrenner, U. Toward an experimental ecology of human development. Am. Psychol. 1977, 32, 513–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Derikx, D.F.A.A.; Houwen, S.; Meijers, V.; Schoemaker, M.M.; Hartman, E. The Relationship between Social Environmental Factors and Motor Performance in 3- to 12-Year-Old Typically Developing Children: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 7516. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18147516
Derikx DFAA, Houwen S, Meijers V, Schoemaker MM, Hartman E. The Relationship between Social Environmental Factors and Motor Performance in 3- to 12-Year-Old Typically Developing Children: A Systematic Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(14):7516. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18147516
Chicago/Turabian StyleDerikx, Dagmar F. A. A., Suzanne Houwen, Vivian Meijers, Marina M. Schoemaker, and Esther Hartman. 2021. "The Relationship between Social Environmental Factors and Motor Performance in 3- to 12-Year-Old Typically Developing Children: A Systematic Review" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 14: 7516. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18147516
APA StyleDerikx, D. F. A. A., Houwen, S., Meijers, V., Schoemaker, M. M., & Hartman, E. (2021). The Relationship between Social Environmental Factors and Motor Performance in 3- to 12-Year-Old Typically Developing Children: A Systematic Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(14), 7516. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18147516