2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Ethical Considerations
The University of Melbourne Ethics Committee approved the project (ID:1443502.1). Permissions to access and analyse data were provided by the Department of Education and Early Childhood Development. In line with local protocols, an application for research partnership was submitted to VACCHO and approved.
At project commencement, Terms of Reference were devised and agreed to by all coauthors to guide the partnership and co-analysis. These terms outlined expectations relating to ethical conduct, community protocols, roles and responsibilities, reporting and knowledge translation, authorship, intellectual property, and student involvement.
2.2. Engagement, Collaboration, and Co-analysis Process
Data analysis was led by an Aboriginal (Alywarre/Stolen Generation) Ph.D. Student (J.L.), who alongside Gunai, Yorta Yorta, and Gunditjmara researchers (A.T.) engaged staff at three Aboriginal Community Controlled Health Organizations (Victorian Aboriginal Child Care Agency (VACCA), Victorian Aboriginal Community Controlled Health Organisation (VACCHO) and Bubup Wilam Early Learning Centre to co-analyse precollected cross-sectional survey data relating to Aboriginal adolescent health in Victoria. Representatives from Aboriginal organizations were engaged to allow for local understandings of sociocultural realities and lived experiences in the Victorian community to guide this research.
Co-analysis was conducted between 2015–2021 and involved three broad meetings with the Aboriginal researchers and staff from Aboriginal organizations. In these meetings representatives from Aboriginal organizations–1. identified organizational priorities and data needs; 2. designed and framed the analysis by selecting variables and outcomes that aligned best with local sociocultural understandings of health and wellbeing; and 3. interpreted data, contextualized findings and decided on processes for dissemination of findings.
Both the priorities of Aboriginal researchers and staff from Aboriginal-community-controlled organizations are reflected in this work.
2.3. Study Populaiton
In total 88 (0.8% of adolescents sampled) self-identified as Aboriginal and completed the Aboriginal module and have been included in our co-analysis. In recognizing the small sample size, our analysis is compromised by limited statistical power. However, we emphasize here the collaborative process for data-analysis rather than findings alone.
2.4. Demographic Variables
Age was measured as age at last birthday (in years). Gender was collected as a self-reported binary (male/female). A binary location variable was created using postcode coded to the Australian Bureau of Statistics 2011 Australian Statistical Geography Standard (ASGS) in regard to remoteness. Because of small sample size, inner regional and outer region classifications were merged, and a dichotomous variable, “major city” or “regional”, was created.
2.5. Explanatory Variables
The collaborative team identified which cultural and structural determinants fit with the work they did and with their understanding of social determinants. Because of the novel nature of these measures, we include how these were collected and measured as appendices. Measures were dichotomized to present the descriptive characteristics of the population (see
Appendix A Table A1 and
Table A2), but for principal component analysis, categorical variables with all responses were used where possible.
2.6. Outcome Measure
An inverted version of the Kessler-10 psychological distress questionnaire (K-10) was used [
37]. Kessler-10 scores were dichotomized into “likely to be well”, corresponding to a score of 10–19, and “likely to have psychological distress”, corresponding to a score of 20–50. “Likely being well” was the outcome of interest and was selected to focus on youth doing well, a salutogenic approach to disrupt narratives of problemed adolescents. This approach has been used for Aboriginal populations elsewhere to describe subsets of Aboriginal populations doing well [
38,
39,
40].
2.7. Statistical Analysis
Age has been summarized in terms of means and standard deviation. All categorical data were expressed as frequencies (percentages) and 95% CI. For categorical data, the χ2 test was used to assess difference between the groups “likely being well” and “likely to have psychological distress”. Rate ratios (RR) were calculated as “likely being well”/”likely to have psychological distress”, and confidence intervals for RR were calculated using MedCalc (MedCalc software, Ostend, Belgium). All other analyses were completed using SPSS Statistics version 27 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).
PCA was used to group highly correlated variables into a smaller number of “components”. Correlations between variables were determined using a correlation matrix. PCA was used to expose latent clustering of variables in the population. We ran two PCA models, one for cultural determinants and one for structural determinants, because of a small sample size (
n = 88), as model stability would be compromised by the inclusion of too many variables [
41,
42].
Because of constraints on number of variables able to be included, we used a correlation matrix to assess the strengths of intercorrelations among the cultural and structural determinants. Where variables had poor correlation (no correlations > 0.3), they were excluded from PCA; these included neighbourhood attachment, high housing transition, access to basic services, health access, sees members of extended family, having Aboriginal friends, speaking language, participation in Aboriginal ceremonies, going to Aboriginal funerals or sorry business, and going to Aboriginal organizations, which were used only in univariate analysis. The suitability of data for PCA was assessed by testing appropriateness of sample size using Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) value and the Bartlett test of sphericity [
43].
The PCA used an Oblimin rotation given that correlation matrices revealed that variables were highly correlated. Components were extracted from PCA on the basis of an eigenvalue > 1. To interpret PCA models, loadings (correlations) greater than 0.4 were considered meaningful. In addition, components arising from PCA modelling were culturally validated through the co-analysis process where the collaborative members identified whether the components produced made sense and captured the social experiences of the communities they worked with. Components produced using PCA were used as continuous variables. Linear regression was used to assess associations between the cultural and structural PCA components, with scatter plots uses to assess suitability for linear regression.
Components produced by the structural and cultural PCA models were included in logistic regression models with the outcomes variable “likely to be well”, with adjustment for gender only. Age was excluded, as the survey included only a small age range.
3. Results
Characteristics of the population are presented in
Table 1. The mean age of adolescents was 14 years (SD 1.7; range 11–17 years), with similar proportions of adolescents across school levels 7, 9 and 11. There were more females than males and similar numbers of participants from the major city and other locations.
Adolescents reported strong family connectedness, with most talking to or seeing extended family or feeling close to their mum or dad. In terms of community, one in three participants reported growing up in an Aboriginal community, and just over half had contact with the Aboriginal community. However, only a section of the population spoke an Aboriginal language (22%) or recognized an area as their homeland or traditional country (29%). Although participation in individual cultural activities was not high in the 12 months prior, 62.5% of adolescents had participated in at least one contemporary cultural activity—either NAIDOC, ceremonies, sporting carnivals, festivals, sorry business, or a traditional activity such as hunting, fishing, or gathering bush foods.
For structural determinants, the historical and political contexts of ongoing government intervention were revealed in the proportions reporting being Stolen Generations (17.6%); having a parent forced from their homeland (12.0%); having a parent taken by the government, a mission, or welfare (10.5%); harassment or negative contact with police (9.3%); and removal from family by community services or/and ACCA (6.8%). Social contexts of negative school environments were revealed with just over half reporting experiences of racism at school (52.0%). Not all schools gave help for adolescents to feel comfortable, identified goals with students, or set up plans with students so they could achieve their goals. The acknowledgment and inclusion of Aboriginal culture in the curriculum and school involvement in activities such as NAIDOC was low (28.4%). For the socioeconomic determinants, just under half of adolescents reported high housing transition (40.9%) and just over half having medium or low family affluence score (52.3%). Almost all youth reported geographic access to basic services, and the vast majority felt they could access a GP if needed.
Health Outcomes
Characteristics of adolescents and bivariate associations with “likely being well” are reported in
Table 2. Half (51.2%) of adolescents were coded as “likely being well”, while 14.3% were coded as likely having “mild”, 17.9% “moderate”, and 16.7% as “severe” psychological distress disorder. For demographic variables there were no statistically significant associations with “likely being well”. For cultural determinants bivariate analysis revealed statistically significant associations between “likely being well” and feeling close to dad (
p < 0.01). Bivariate associations for structural determinants revealed statistically significant associations between “likely being well” and not having parents forced from their country (
p = 0.03) and feeling they could access healthcare (
p = 0.04). Associations were revealed between “likely being well” and schools giving help for adolescents to feel comfortable, adolescents not experiencing racism at school, and either parent not being taken by the government, a mission, or welfare, but these associations were not statistically significant.
The cultural PCA produced 4 components from the 11 cultural variables modelled (
Table 3). These explained 64.3% of the variance in the population. These were labelled based on latent clustering of variables: (i)
cultural component 1—grew up in Aboriginal family/community and connected (grew up in in an Aboriginal community, contact with Aboriginal community, grew up in an Aboriginal family, sees members of Aboriginal community, has talked to relatives/Elders about history, has attended Aboriginal early years program, recognizes traditional country, and has gone to Aboriginal sports carnivals); (ii)
cultural component 2—traditional cultural activities, sport, and early years (traditional cultural activities, attended Aboriginal early years, sees members of Aboriginal community, has gone to any Aboriginal Sports carnivals); and (iii)
cultural component 3—disconnected from community, country, and culture (has participated in NAIDOC week (inverse), has participated in Aboriginal festivals (inverse), has participated in Aboriginal sports carnivals (inverse), recognizes country (inverse), grew up in an Aboriginal community (inverse), and talks with/sees members of Aboriginal community (inverse)). Cultural component 1 explained a large proportion of variance (41.2%).
The structural PCA produced 4 components from the 11 structural variables modelled (
Table 4). These explained 61.1% of the variance in the population. These were (iv)
structural component 1—institutionally imposed family displacement (either parent taken by government/mission/welfare, either parent forced from homelands, Stolen Generations); (v)
structural component 2—Negative school environment (school discusses goals with Aboriginal adolescent (inverse), school sets plans to achieve goals (inverse), experienced racism at school, Stolen Generations); (vi)
structural component 3—negative police contact and poverty (family affluence scale (inverse), harassed or negative contact with police); and (vii)
structural component 4—removed from parent with positive school environment (under care of community services/ACCA, school gives help to feel comfortable, school includes Aboriginal activities, Stolen Generations). Variance was explained equally across variables, with structural component 1 explaining most (19.4%) and structural component 4 the least (11.4%).
In looking at associations between each of the three cultural components and the four structural components, “growing up in an Aboriginal family/community and connected” associated with higher “institutionally imposed family displacement” (p-value = 0.011) and higher “removed from parent with positive school environment” (p-value = 0.012), while “disconnected from community, country, and culture” associated with lower “institutionally imposed family displacement” (p-value < 0.001), indicating that those disconnected still had family together.
Logistic regression modelling of PCA components revealed that for adolescents, “institutionally imposed family displacement” associated with significantly lower odds of “likely being well” (
p-value
< 0.05)
(Table 5). A similar association was also revealed for “negative contact with police and poverty” and “likely being well”, but this was not statistically significant (
p = 0.07). Conversely, adolescents who “grew up in Aboriginal family/community and connected” had statistically significant higher odds of “likely being well”.
4. Discussion
This collaborative co-analysis approach to social-epidemiology identified structural and cultural determinants that are “upon the people” from the insider perspective of Aboriginal people and organizations in Victoria. Modelling social survey items identified as important and unique to Aboriginal populations revealed that the relational cultural determinants of growing up in an Aboriginal family and community associated with “likely being well”, as did closeness to dad in bivariate analysis. Structural environments characterized by generational child removal and family displacement were associated negatively with “likely being well”. Bivariate analysis revealed an association with health access and “likely being well”.
In terms of structural determinants, the trauma caused by generational child removal and experiences with the justice system are well established and also acknowledged by the Australian government with the seminal “Bringing them home report” and the report of the Royal Commission into Aboriginal Deaths in Custody [
44,
45]. Here our data demonstrate that these structural issues continue to have ongoing impacts on the lives of school-aged adolescents as young as 11 years in Victoria in 2009, some 10–20 years after these reports were published.
The Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (2018) reported on the multiple adverse health, cultural and socioeconomic outcomes that associate with being a member of the Stolen Generations (removed before 1972) or a descendant of the Stolen Generation [
46]. In Australia, official policies from 1869 to the early 1970s saw over 100,000 Aboriginal children removed from their families and placed in government and church institutions or with white families. In present times, Noongar academic Jacynta Krakouer has described ongoing high Aboriginal child removal rates as a continuum of the Stolen Generation [
47]. In our study, 17.6% of adolescents connected to the identity of being Stolen Generations, and 7% reported being in the care of community services or an Aboriginal Child Care Agency. These high rates of family displacement are understood against Victorian state-wide child protection data that showed 16% of Aboriginal youth under the monitoring of child protection services, eight times the 2% of non-Aboriginal people [
46]. However, despite historic and ongoing disruptions to family units, a vast majority of Aboriginal adolescents reported strong and close relationships with family, including seeing extended family and reporting closeness with either mum or dad. The maintenance of these relationships is revealing of the resilience of Aboriginal family units. The key finding that growing up in and being connected to Aboriginal family and community was associated with far greater odds (126% more likely) of “likely being well” speaks to the important role Aboriginal family has in contributing to health, especially to adolescents.
Shockingly, half of all Aboriginal adolescents in Victorian schools reported experiencing racism at school. This proportion is higher than the 34.6% of 12–17 year olds reporting racism in any setting 20 years earlier in a Victorian Aboriginal Health Service survey [
48]. The interpersonal accounts of racism reported in schools, alongside a low proportion of schools including Aboriginal culture or activities in the curriculum, highlighted the problematic nature of the Victorian school system. Although our findings were inconclusive regarding the association between self-reported racism and “likely being well”, they were broadly consistent with the influence of racism in schools. Bangerang/Wiradjuri Elder and cochair of the First People Assembly of Victoria Aunty Geraldine Atkinson framed changes needed in schools as “Aboriginal staff, Aboriginal culture and Aboriginal languages taught in these schools so we can make them acceptable to all our children” [
49], while research elsewhere in Australia highlighted the importance of a social justice perspective, culturally inclusive curricula, culturally differentiated quality teaching, and a primary focus on students’ wellbeing for school environments to support social and academic outcomes for Aboriginal adolescents [
50]. Success in these areas is likely achieved when Victorian schools work in partnership with Aboriginal organizations that can provide leadership on culturally safe learning environments for all students. We recognize that a 2021 announcement by the Victorian government to adapt the Australian curriculum and incorporate First Nations histories, knowledges, and experiences of colonization is a step in the right direction. Further, recent work led by Distinguished Professor Marcia Langton under the Indigenous Knowledge Resources for Australian School Curricula Project is leading the way in providing resources for teachers so they are empowered to teach to Aboriginal histories.
Absolute or relative socioeconomic disadvantage in terms of employment, education, and housing are widely promoted and accepted as social determinants in Australian and global settings [
51]. These determinants largely relate to everyone and have been an emphasized focus of social policy relating to Aboriginal people for decades. Here we saw 52% of Aboriginal participants as having a high family affluence score compared to 70% of participants from the wider HOWRU survey [
32]. For Aboriginal populations, responding to poverty is not straightforward. It is important to understand that pathways to affluence and socioeconomic opportunities are not equal for Aboriginal populations, owing to the processes of colonization and the racialized structuring embedded within Australian society. These processes see Aboriginal people more likely to be excluded from social privilege and employment, marginalized from resources (including own lands) and opportunities, and inheriting socioeconomic deprivation [
52]. Although family affluence is a measure of material poverty for both dominant and Aboriginal populations, it is important to recognize that the concept of “high affluence”, though it at some level measures the ability to meet basic human needs, it also is reflective of the aspirations of the dominant capitalist culture [
53]. Particularly in the HOWRU survey, where the components of family affluence included the familial ownership of multiple cars and computers, frequent holidays, and house size, this metric speaks less to the ability to meet basic needs than social measures of adequate housing and access to foods or services would. These are imposed aspirations of Western capitalism, and there is a need for more appropriate indicators to be used in surveys relating to Aboriginal populations. We suggest future surveys consider material poverty in terms of human need rather than dominant aspirations; such a metric would have greater utility across all populations.
As we report here on the quantifiable associations between psychological distress and child removal, negative school environments, police harassment, and material poverty, we are reminded by Johnson et al. (2013) and Doyle et al. (2016) and their work with Aboriginal communities in the Goulburn–Murray Rivers region that structural determinants are real and legitimate points for health intervention [
54,
55]. Interventions and policy need to move beyond just responding to Aboriginal disadvantage through setting targets for individuals and communities to examining and measuring change in dominant institutions including schools, police, and social services.
For cultural determinants, expressions of Aboriginal culture are well and alive in Victoria. Here we saw that despite the continuing impacts of colonization, more than half of Aboriginal adolescents engaged in an Aboriginal cultural activity. We also saw Aboriginal adolescents as connected to friends, community Elders, and culture. Many Aboriginal adolescents in the study also participated in traditional cultural activities of hunting, fishing, or gathering bush foods, while others participated in more contemporary expressions of Aboriginal social life including sports carnivals and NAIDOC. These contemporary activities are all revealing of Aboriginal culture as dynamic and evolving. These local understandings of culture in Victoria are contemporary constructs that move beyond the limiting stereotypes of the “traditional”, the “dysfunctional”, and the “pathogenic” that have plagued epidemiology [
56].
In terms of culture, our data spoke to the importance of the relational aspect of belonging to and participating in a living social system. We found that growing up in and being connected to Aboriginal family/community was associated with greater odds of “likely being well”. The high loading of Aboriginal family (0.828) for this component revealed its contribution to the association with “likely being well”. These findings highlighted the resilience of Aboriginal families and communities that have maintained identity and connection to country in a region that has a brutal colonial history. They also evidence the important work of Aboriginal organizations in Victoria, which are known to connect Aboriginal people to community and place [
57]. These descriptions of culture are also consistent with those generated by Victorian Aboriginal researcher Dr Cammi Murrup-Stewart (2020), whose yarning work with young people spoke to the relational nature of culture for Aboriginal young people [
58].
We found those “disconnected from community, country, and culture” had higher odds of “likely being well”, but this association was not statistically significant. We also found that “Disconnected from community, country, and culture” associated with lower “institutionally imposed family displacement” (
p-value < 0.001), so low rates of generational child removal may reflect in “likely being well” for those “disconnected from community, country, and culture”. Although this group stated that they were not connected to Aboriginal community, they may have misinterpreted the question due to a lack of explanation. In all likelihood, many of these participants would be connected to an Aboriginal family (we know that most Aboriginal children are raised by an Aboriginal parent or kin), and this may be why they are “likely to be well”. However, we are also mindful that the small sample size makes it difficult to determine if this is true beyond the study. The interpretation of Aboriginal organizations was integral to understanding why “disconnected from community, country, and culture” associated (although not significantly) with “likely being well”. The coauthors from Aboriginal organizations spoke of the process of colonization in Victoria and how it worked to disperse, dislocate, and make less visible Aboriginal people. Individuals who are disconnected from community are less visible, and with that can come greater access to opportunities (school, employment) and less exposure to racism. For example, participation in community activities such as marches (i.e., NAIDOC) or in Aboriginal sporting teams, although acknowledged as important from the relational aspect of connecting people and building Aboriginal identity, made individuals more visible as Aboriginal and exposed to interpersonal racism [
59].
For cultural determinants, speaking language and knowing homeland were not widely reported by adolescents, reflective of the aggressive nature of colonization in Victoria a history of forced assimilation and mass displacement. However, this will be an important indicator to monitor given the proliferation of Aboriginal language revitalization programs in Victoria and the development of other cultural heritage programs and activities since this survey was administered.
Our analysis was only possible because the HOWRU Survey was designed to include an Aboriginal module. We emphasize the important need for social surveys to have measures that capture the social realities of Aboriginal people as identified by them. We also propose that an ecosocial theory and a multilevel framework that focuses on “who” and “what” drives social inequities, rather than focusing on individuals or communities, is a useful approach for social epidemiology involving Aboriginal populations [
60]. Work by the Victorian Aboriginal Health Service two decades ago spoke to the importance of ecological frameworks as alternatives to the linear frameworks traditionally used in non-communicable-disease epidemiology. Thompson et al. (2000) called for ecological approaches that recognized structural issues when they wrote, “The advantage of applying this [ecological] approach to epidemiological models is that it enables a more sophisticated and comprehensive understanding of risk and allows for the identification of factors that reflect people’s own constructions of their social worlds through naturalistic observation” [
21] (p. 1461).
A maintained focus on dominant social determinants in social-epidemiology has come from a scientific methodology that has centred and normalized Western epistemologies (knowledges) and axiologies (ideologies) [
61]. By not engaging with Aboriginal-defined social determinants, the discipline of epidemiology applies a casualness to the construction of Aboriginal social worlds that is at odds with a scientific discipline that focuses on statistic precision and rigor [
62]. Further, by overlooking cofounding—the basic problem of compatibility between Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal groups—questions are raised about the internally and externally validity of these analyses. As highlighted by Larkins (2006), research relating to Aboriginal people, in general, is methodologically strengthened through the inclusion of Aboriginal people and their knowledges and the centring of these voices [
61]. Rigney describes a fundament of Indigenist research is that it privileges Aboriginal voice [
63]. Here, Aboriginal voice is reflected not only in the authorship by Aboriginal academics, but also in participation by Aboriginal organizations that speak firsthand about their social world and that of the people they work in proximity with. Another fundament of Indigenist research is resistance—of developing alternative discourses to those created without Aboriginal people [
63]. We deliberately sought to resist the normalization of dominant sociocultural experiences and lives and to highlight that sociocultural determinants are different, as are health causality pathways. We know and reveal here that health has never been a consequence of Aboriginal people’s inability to meet parity for a series of dominant social concepts [
64].
Strengths and Limitations
A key strength of this paper is its process, which deliberately sought to centre Aboriginal ways of knowing, doing, and being within social-epidemiology. Co-analysis meant that rather than relying on assumptions in the literature around social determinants, the community the data related to was able to articulate the social contexts and social processes that happen in its social world. This acknowledged the constructionist nature of epidemiological knowledge, but also the expert voices of Aboriginal people and organizations. As highlighted by Brough et al., epidemiology is not a value-free science and embedded within are the understandings, values, and positioning of those who create it [
1]. Aboriginal identities and sociocultural conditions can be only partially known through dominant frameworks that provide one worldview and interpretation of the world. Aboriginal people too have legitimate understandings and interpretations of their identities and sociocultural realities [
65].
Here, Aboriginal voices have asked fundamentally different social questions of the data than researchers of dominant Victorian institutions have asked in the past [
66,
67]. Rather than asking how we are different to non-Aboriginal people, by examining social determinants of dominant populations, it was asked, what are the unique social determinants specific to Aboriginal health? This frame was only possible because the dataset was available. Regrettably, when the Victorian Department of Education and Training conducted subsequent health and wellbeing surveys in Victorian schools in 2014 and 2018, they did not include an Aboriginal module. This means that health and social dimensions unique to the live of Aboriginal adolescents were not captured. We make the strong recommendation that future health and social surveys that collect an Aboriginal identifier must also collect health and social data that most accurately describes the social worlds of Aboriginal participants. These measures are best developed by or with Aboriginal organizations [
28].
In recognizing key strengths of this research, we are also mindful of its limitations—in particular, the poor statistical power that came from a small sample size. In general, PCA components for small datasets are not very generalizable to the wider population, but the high loadings on components (>0.800) we saw here suggests that components were likely revealing of the latent structure in the wider community [
42]. This limited sample size was a consequence of the sampling method, which selected for equal proportions of youth by area and school type and overlooked how this would impact Aboriginal participation. We recommend all future surveys aim to achieve greater power through targeted sampling of schools with Aboriginal students or conversely through Aboriginal networks or organizations. With larger sample size, a more nuanced exploration of health and its structural and cultural determinants for subpopulations can be conducted.
Time has also elapsed since the study; however, we believe the findings are still relevant, as many of the structural issues, particularly those relating to child removal and policing, have in fact intensified.
In interpreting findings, we realize that the HOWRU Aboriginal population is unlikely to be representative of all Aboriginal adolescents in Victoria. Firstly, there are adolescents who may not be comfortable reporting Aboriginal status in a government survey. Secondly, the survey relates to adolescents attending school, so it does not capture all Aboriginal adolescents. Data for 2010, the year after this survey was conducted, reveals that retention rates for students in years 10 to 12 was 50.9% for Aboriginal students, and it is likely that those not in school have different health and social profiles [
68]. For example, it is likely that prevalence estimates of experiences of racism in school are likely to be underestimated, as racism associates with school disengagement and nonattendance [
69].
Quantitative survey data is also unlikely to capture Aboriginal culture or structures and mechanisms of Australian society in their full complexity. In assigning sociocultural phenomena to a series of questions and numbers, we may not be measuring what we think we are [
70]. We also realize there is a tendency of quantitative research to essentialize culture. We know that the Victorian population is heterogenous and that there are many expressions of Aboriginal culture and many encounters with the dominant culture, for these adolescents are all cultural beings. We know that the HOWRU Survey data did not include measures for Aboriginal cultural constructs of family, identity, and self-determination, which were identified as cultural determinants in the Victorian literature and discussed in early meetings with the collaborative. Future design of an Aboriginal module should consider indicators that capture these cultural concepts. We were also limited by the measure we used to capture “Aboriginal health and wellbeing”. Although the K-10 has been used by others and has excellent agreement with a culturally modified K-5, it is a universal rather than Aboriginal-specific measure of health [
71]. It is also a measure of distress rather than wellbeing. Since 2009, much work has been done to develop Aboriginal measures of health, and there are now measures such as the Aboriginal Risk and Resilience Questionnaire developed by Dr Graham Gee and the Victorian Aboriginal Health Service that could be used in present-day surveys to measure health and wellbeing [
72].
We could not ascertain from the data if observed relationships were causal because of the cross-sectional nature of the HOWRU Survey. However, there is already strong evidence by way of art, music, protests, biography, and qualitative accounts that colonization characterized by generational displacement, child removal, policing, and racism is bad for Aboriginal health [
12]. All our study did was quantify this association in a palatable, empirical form that Western policymakers will see as “evidence”. We are also mindful that expressions of health and cultural or structural determinants can change across the life course, and associations reported here would need to be tested via longitudinal study to empirically demonstrate that structural and cultural determinants impact on health. We also cannot determine the intensity of social–cultural phenomena. Our conceptual framework is also one-dimensional and excludes time and space on health, aside from the inclusion of history as a contextual structural determinant.