The Effect of Rapport on Data Quality in Face-to-Face Interviews: Beneficial or Detrimental?
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Sample
2.2. Baseline Assessment
2.3. Recruitment and Training of Interviewers
2.4. Measurements
2.4.1. Rapport
2.4.2. Data Quality
- 1.
- Missing responses. A computer-assisted personal interview (CAPI) was used for the purposes of the NESDA study. The CAPI does not allow questions to be skipped, so the interviewer has to make a conscious decision to record an answer as missing. Due to the use of CAPI, the total amount of missing responses per interview was expected to be low. The following two categories were created: ‘one missing responses or none’ and ‘two or more missing responses’. To this end, a sum score was created for missing responses using 678 questions in total on the following topics: demography, medication, health, healthcare use, childhood trauma, important negative events, and suicidal behaviour. These particular topics from the NESDA interview were selected because the associated questions were mandatory for all the respondents, whereas those involving other topics (such as depression, anxiety, manic disorder, and alcohol use) were only answered by a subgroup of respondents (missing by design).
- 2.
- Socially desirable responses. We measured differences in the distribution of responses to sensitive questions, since socially desirable responses are expected for questions of a sensitive nature. To this end, we selected questions on topics that had been reported to be sensitive. For a topic to be considered sensitive, one of the following criteria must apply: the question must be intrusive, the respondents must have concerns about the consequences of answering such questions honestly, or the question must elicit answers that are perceived to be socially undesirable [12]. The following questions meet these criteria:Income 1: “In general, do you have enough money to buy the food that you and your family need?” (yes/not always)Income 2: “At the end of the month, do you have money left, do you have just enough money or do you not have enough money?”(yes, enough/no, not enough)Income 3: “Have you ever experienced serious financial difficulties?” (no/yes)Misdemeanour 1: “Have you ever had dealings with the police or the courts in connection with a misdemeanour?”(no/yes)“Yes” responses to the first and second questions about income were defined as socially desirable, as were “no” responses to the third and fourth questions. Socially desirable responses were all coded as “0” and used as the reference category in the analyses.
- 3.
- Consistency of response. Answers to a comparable question—in two data collection modes—were compared, to measure consistency of responses. The respondents were asked to fill in a questionnaire at home before attending a face-to-face interview. The following questions were used in the self-report questionnaire (first question) and in the face-to-face interview (second question):“When you drink, how many glasses of alcohol do you drink on a typical day?” (self-assessment)1 – 2, 3 – 4, 5 – 6, 7 – 9, or 10 or more“On the days that you used alcohol in the past 12 months, how many glasses would you typically drink in one day?” (face-to-face assessment)…….. alcoholic beveragesResponses to the face-to-face interview were classified according to the ordinal categories used in the self-report questionnaire. Next, the responses to both questions were compared by creating the following two categories: 1. Same response to both questions; 2. Different response to both questions.
2.5. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Missing Responses
3.2. Socially Undesirable Responses
3.3. Consistency of Response
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Billiet, J.; Loosveldt, G. Improvement of the quality of responses to factual survey questions by interviewer training. Public Opin. Q. 1988, 52, 190–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lavin, D.; Maynard, D.W. Standardization vs. rapport: Respondent laughter and interviewer reaction during telephone survey. Am. Sociol. Rev. 2001, 66, 453–479. Available online: www.jstor.com/stable/3088888 (accessed on 18 March 2020). [CrossRef]
- Ongena, Y.P.; Dijkstra, W. A model of cognitive processes and conversational principles in survey interview interaction. Appl. Cognit. Psychol 2007, 21, 145–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Olsen, K.; Smyth, J.D.; Dykema, J.; Holbrook, A.L.; Kreuter, F.; West, B.T. Interviewer Effects from a Total Survey Error Perspective, 1st ed.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- West, B.T.; Blom, A.G. Explaining interviewer effects: A research synthesis. J. Surv. Stat. Methodol. 2017, 5, 175–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Andersen, H.; Mayerl, J. Responding to socially desirable and undesirable topics: Different types of response behaviour? Methods Data Anal. 2019, 13, 7–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahn, R.L.; Cannell, C.F. The Dynamics of Interviewing: Theory, Technique, and Cases; John Wiley & Sons: New York, NY, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Bell, K.; Fahmy, E.; Gordon, D. Quantitative conversations: The importance of developing rapport in standardised interviewing. Qual. Quant. 2016, 50, 193–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tickle-Degnen, L.; Rosenthal, R. The nature of rapport and its nonverbal correlates. Psychol. Inq. 1990, 1, 285–293. Available online: www.jstor.com/stable/1449345 (accessed on 18 March 2020). [CrossRef]
- Garbanski, D.; Schaeffer, N.C.; Dykema, J. Interviewing practices, conversational practices and rapport: Responsiveness and engagement in the standardized survey interview. Sociol. Methodol. 2016, 46, 1–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Horsfall, M.; Eikelenboom, M.; Smit, J.H. The effect of rapport between interviewer and respondent on attrition. J. Public Health Epidemiol. 2017, 3, 1041–1047, ISSN: 24730661. [Google Scholar]
- Tourangeau, R.; Yan, T. Sensitive questions in surveys. Psychol. Bull. 2007, 133, 859–883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hill, R.J.; Hall, N.E. A Note on Rapport and the Quality of Interview Data. Southwest. Soc. Sci. Q. 1963, 44, 247–255. [Google Scholar]
- Weiss, C.H. Validity of welfare mothers’ interview responses. Public Opin. Q. 1968, 32, 622–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goudy, W.J.; Potter, H.R. Interview Rapport: Demise of a concept. Public Opin. Q. 1975, 39, 529–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van der Drift, J.; Derksen, W. Critical review—Interview style and interview results. Mens En Maatsch. 1985, 60, 187–197. [Google Scholar]
- Mensch, B.S.; Kandel, D.B. Underreporting of substance use in a national longitudinal youth cohort: Individual and interviewer effects. Public Opin. Q. 1988, 52, 100–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Belli, R.F.; Lepkowski, J.M.; Kabeto, M.U. The respective roles of cognitive processing difficulty and conversational rapport on the accuracy of retrospective reports of doctor’s office visits. In Proceedings of the Seventh Conference on Health Survey Research Methods, Williamsburg, VA, USA, 24–27 September 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Belli, R.F.; Lee, E.H.; Stafford, F.P.; Chou, C.H. Calendar and question-list survey methods: Association between interviewer behaviors and data quality. J. Off. Stat. 2004, 20, 185–218. [Google Scholar]
- Conrad, F.G.; Schober, M.F.; Jans, M.; Orlowski, R.A.; Nielsen, D.; Levenstein, R. Comprehension and engagement in survey interviews with virtual agents. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cordova Cazar, A.L. Wellbeing and Data Quality in the American Time Use Survey (ATUS) from a Total Survey Error Perspective. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE, USA, 2016. Available online: http://search.proquest.com.libraryproxy.griffith.edu.au/docview/1868871923 (accessed on 12 October 2021).
- Olson, K.; Bilgen, I. The role of interviewer experience on acquiescence. Public Opin. Q. 2011, 75, 99–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, H.; Conrad, F.G.; Kreuter, F. The Relationship between Interviewer-Respondent Rapport and Data Quality. J. Surv. Stat. Methodol. 2021, 9, 429–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Penninx, B.W.J.H.; Beekman, A.T.F.; Smit, J.H.; Zitman, F.G.; Nolen, W.A.; Spinhoven, P.; Cuijpers, P.; De Jong, P.J.; Van Marwijk, H.W.J.; Assendelft, W.J.J.; et al. The Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA): Rationale, objectives and methods. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 2008, 17, 121–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pekár, S.; Brabec, M. Generalized estimating equations: A pragmatic and flexible approach to the marginal GLM modelling of correlated data in the behavioural sciences. Ethology 2018, 124, 86–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
General Sample | Subsample for Consistency Variable | ||
---|---|---|---|
Variable | High Level of Rapport (%) | Variable | High Level of Rapport (%) |
Gender | Gender | ||
Female (n = 1926) | 84.6 | Female (n = 1527) | 84.9 |
Male (n = 971) | 82.5 | Male (n = 844) | 82.5 |
Age *** | Age *** | ||
18–24 (n = 391) | 93.2 | 18–24 (n = 327) | 93.3 |
25–39 (n = 831) | 86.4 | 25–39 (n = 668) | 87.0 |
40–49 (n = 689) | 83.1 | 40–49 (n = 568) | 83.1 |
50–65 (n = 986) | 78.4 | 50–65 (n = 808) | 78.6 |
Level of education | Level of education | ||
Basic (n = 192) | 78.0 | Basic (n = 126) | 77.8 |
Intermediate (n = 1695) | 84.5 | Intermediate (n = 1350) | 84.8 |
High (n = 1010) | 83.8 | High (n = 895) | 83.8 |
Variable | % |
---|---|
Rapport Low (n = 483) High (n = 2414) Missing (n = 0) | 16.7 83.3 0 |
Total no. of missing responses One or none (n = 2090) Two or more (n = 807) | 72.2 27.8 |
Socially desirable responses Income 1 Enough money to buy food? Yes (n = 2611) No (n = 286) Missing (n = 0) | 90.1 9.9 0 |
Income 2 Enough money at the end of the month Yes (n = 2559) No (n = 336) Missing (n = 2) | 88.3 11.6 0.1 |
Income 3 Ever had serious financial problems? No (n = 2190) Yes (705) Missing (n = 2) | 75.6 24.3 0.1 |
Ever had dealings with police or court? No (n = 2405) Yes (n = 491) Missing (n = 1) | 83.0 17.0 0.0 |
Consistency of response Discrepancy in report of alcohol intake face to face interview vs. self-report * Inconsistent response (n = 1555) Consistent response (n = 816) Missing (not applicable) | 34.4 65.6 |
Beta Estimate | Std Error | 95% CI | Odds Ratio | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total no. of missing responses | −0.225* | 0.089 | (−0.400, −0.050) | 0.798 |
socially desirable responses Income 1 Income 2 Income 3 Problems with the law | −0.426 ** −0.479 ** −0.356 ** −0.323 * | 0.147 0.140 0.105 0.155 | (−0.715, −0.138) (−0.752, −0.0205) (−0.562, −0.150) (−0.626, −0.019) | 0.653 0.620 0.701 0.724 |
Consistency in reporting alcohol intake – F-t-F vs. self-report | −0.133 | 0.100 | (−0.329, 0.062) | 0.875 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Horsfall, M.; Eikelenboom, M.; Draisma, S.; Smit, J.H. The Effect of Rapport on Data Quality in Face-to-Face Interviews: Beneficial or Detrimental? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 10858. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182010858
Horsfall M, Eikelenboom M, Draisma S, Smit JH. The Effect of Rapport on Data Quality in Face-to-Face Interviews: Beneficial or Detrimental? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(20):10858. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182010858
Chicago/Turabian StyleHorsfall, Melany, Merijn Eikelenboom, Stasja Draisma, and Johannes H. Smit. 2021. "The Effect of Rapport on Data Quality in Face-to-Face Interviews: Beneficial or Detrimental?" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 20: 10858. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182010858
APA StyleHorsfall, M., Eikelenboom, M., Draisma, S., & Smit, J. H. (2021). The Effect of Rapport on Data Quality in Face-to-Face Interviews: Beneficial or Detrimental? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(20), 10858. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182010858