Next Article in Journal
Reduced Vastus Medialis/Lateralis EMG Ratio in Volleyballers with Chronic Knee Pain on Sports-Specific Surfaces: A Pilot Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Multiple Risk Assessment of Heavy Metals in Surface Water and Sediment in Taihu Lake, China
Previous Article in Journal
Model Test Study on the Enhancement of Ecological Self-Repairing Ability of Surface Slope Soil by New Polymer Composites
Previous Article in Special Issue
Concept for the Evaluation of Carcinogenic Substances in Population-Based Human Biomonitoring
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effect of Short- to Long-Term Exposure to Ambient Particulate Matter on Cognitive Function in a Cohort of Middle-Aged and Older Adults: KoGES

Division of Population Health Research, Department of Precision Medicine, Korea National Institute of Health, Korea Disease Control and Prevention Agency, 200 Osongsaengmyeong 2-ro, Osong-eup, Heungdeok-gu, Cheongju-si 28160, Chungcheongbuk-do, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(16), 9913; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19169913
Submission received: 5 July 2022 / Revised: 5 August 2022 / Accepted: 9 August 2022 / Published: 11 August 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Environmental Exposure, Health Effects and Risk)

Abstract

:
Exposure to ambient air pollution and its threat to human health is a global concern, especially in the elderly population. Therefore, more in-depth studies are required to understand the extent of the harmful effects of particulate matter (PM) based on duration and levels of exposure. An investigation was conducted to determine the association between short- (1–14 days), medium- (1, 3, and 6 months), and long-term (1, 2, and 3 years) exposure to air pollutants (PM2.5 and PM10) and cognitive function among Koreans (4175 participants, mean age 67.8 years, 55.2% women) aged over 50 years. Higher levels of PM2.5 exposure for short to long term and PM10 exposure for medium to long term were found to be associated with decreased cognitive function, as indicated by lower scores of the Mini-Mental State Examination adopted in Korean (K-MMSE). There were significant effect modifications by sex, age group, alcohol consumption, physical activity, and smoking status in the association between long-term PM2.5 and PM10 exposure and cognitive function. These findings, which underscore the importance of the efforts to reduce the exposure levels and durations of air pollutants, especially in the vulnerable elderly population, provide evidence for establishing more stringent policies for air pollution regulations.

1. Introduction

Air pollution, which is a critical global concern due to its threat to public health and welfare [1], is regarded as one of the leading causes of disease based on an estimated 79 risk factors in 195 countries from 1990 to 2015 [2]. Chronic exposure to outdoor air pollution contributes to the pathogenesis of major cardiovascular and respiratory diseases [3] via the direct and indirect (i.e., triggering oxidative stress and activating inflammatory pathways) actions of particles on the cardiovascular system [4]. Among the various types of air pollutants, airborne particulate matter (PM), a heterogeneous combination of particles of different sizes and types, including chemical compounds, has been extensively studied for its adverse health-related effects [5]. The PM types are further classified based on the diameter, of which PM10 includes particles with a diameter of 10 µm or less, while PM2.5 includes particles with diameters of 2.5 µm or less [6]. Although particles ≤10 µm are inhaled into the lungs, PM2.5 has been postulated to pose more adverse health effects due to its ability to further penetrate the lower parts of the respiratory system, such as small bronchi and pulmonary alveoli, and also due to presence of heavy metals and carcinogens [7,8]. Despite rapid industrialization and urbanization in South Korea, the government has made tremendous efforts to lower PM levels over the decades [9]. However, evidence has demonstrated that even lower levels of PM exposure are associated with decreased cognitive function, suggesting the need to further lower the concentration of ambient PM [10].
In 2020, the elderly population aged 60 years and older was reported to comprise 15.7% of the total population (51,829,136) in Korea [11]. The proportion of the elderly population is expected to increase to 46.4% by 2070 [11]. With a rapidly increasing elderly population, maintaining cognitive function until later age has become the aim for healthy aging. To achieve this, identifying the risk factors for cognitive impairment is crucial for delaying the onset and speed of cognitive decline. As a modifiable risk factor, there has been an increased interest in investigating the harmful effects of air pollution on cognitive function in the elderly population.
Understanding the degree of adverse health effects attributed to air pollution across various regions and demographics is crucial because the extent of these effects is highly dependent upon the sources and compositions of PM and the duration of exposure, as well as the susceptibility, vulnerability, and characteristics of the exposed population [12]. Therefore, more in-depth studies are required to understand the extent of the harmful effects of PM based on the duration and levels, and the types of exposure that can be translated in the Korean population, particularly in the vulnerable middle-aged-to-elderly population. Importantly, there is a need for a better proactive response to air pollution, especially among vulnerable aging populations.
We hypothesized that higher exposure to PM is associated with decreased cognitive function. With respect to the duration of exposure, we hypothesized that long-term exposure to PM is more closely related to cognitive performance than short- or medium-term PM exposures. Among PM2.5 and PM10 exposures, we further hypothesized that the association would be more prominent with PM2.5 compared to PM10 exposure. The present study investigated the association between short-, medium-, and long-term exposure to ambient PM, including PM2.5 and PM10, on cognitive function among Korean Genome and Epidemiology Study (KoGES) Ansan and Ansung participants over 50 years of age.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Population

The KoGES is a large prospective population-based study initiated in the early 2000s by Korean government entities to investigate public health issues [13]. The sample in this study comprised subjects from the Ansan and Ansung study who attended the seventh follow-up (eighth examination) in 2015–2016 and participated in an aging sub-study. The Ansan and Ansung study recruited participants from urban (Ansan) and rural (Ansung) areas to collect data from two distinctive community-dwelling populations. Detailed information on the KoGES Ansan and Ansung study, including design and methodology, has been previously described [13].
In the present investigation, we used the most recent and largest follow-up data available for air pollution (PM2.5 and PM10) and cognitive assessment scores. A flowchart of the study participants is shown in Figure 1. Among 6318 participants from the seventh follow-up, 2132 individuals had not participated in the aging sub-study and thus were excluded from the cognitive test. An additional 11 participants were excluded because of missing covariates, such as body mass index (BMI) (n = 9), education level (n = 5), marital status (n = 3), physical activity (n = 3), smoking (n = 2), alcohol consumption (n = 3), diabetes mellitus (n = 1), hypertension (n = 2), and cerebrovascular disease (n = 6), resulting in a total of 4175 participants available for investigation. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Korea National Institute of Health and the Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. All participants provided their written consent.

2.2. Cognitive Function Assessment

The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) is one of the most widely used tools to assess cognitive impairment by evaluating mental status. The MMSE is also commonly used to screen individuals for possible dementia in primary care settings [14]. The MMSE adopted in the study was in the Korean language (K-MMSE), and it has been previously validated and administered by trained interviewers [15]. The K-MMSE comprises seven items that assess cognitive function in the following dimensions: orientation, memory and registration, attention and calculation, recall, and language. The K-MMSE scores range from 0 to 30, with higher scores indicating better cognitive performance. The continuous MMSE score was dichotomized using a conventional cutoff of 24 points [16], i.e., participants with a K-MMSE score of 24 points or higher were classified as having a normal cognitive function, while those with a score ≤23 were categorized as having decreased cognitive function.

2.3. Ambient Particulate Matters

The ambient PM2.5 and PM10 levels before the study visit of individual levels were collected. Depending on the duration of PM exposure, we categorized short-, medium-, and long-term exposures as follows:
  • Short-term air pollution exposure data included PM2.5 and PM10 exposures on the date of the visit (day 1), and the average of 2-, 3-, 4-, 5-, 6-, 7-, 8-, 9-, 10-, 11-, 12-, 13-, and 14-day counting from the date of each participant’s study visit;
  • Medium-term air pollution exposure data included average PM2.5 and PM10 exposure levels over 1, 3, and 6 months;
  • Long-term air pollution exposure data included average PM2.5 and PM10 exposures over 1, 2, and 3 years.
Air quality parameters (e.g., PM2.5 and PM10) and meteorological databases (e.g., humidity and temperature) were established using the Community Multiscale Air Quality (CMAQ) model, which is based on a chemical transport model. Of note, no PM measurement methods were used in this study to assess air quality data. After quantifying the PM2.5 and PM10 concentrations every 1 km2 (1 km × 1 km) and the meteorological data every 81 km2 (9 km × 9 km), air quality data were generated based on a geocoding method, where the local exposure data were tabulated by regressing the gridded data as a weighted sum based on the border of the city, county, and district. Air quality and meteorological data were linked to the study participants’ data according to the date of the study visit and participants’ residential addresses. A more detailed methodology for the generation and linkage of air quality and meteorological data has been described previously [17,18].

2.4. Potential Covariates

Potential covariates were selected from the review of the literature associated with exposure to air pollution and cognitive function. In the KoGES Ansan and Ansung eighth examination (seventh follow-up), trained interviewers asked a series of questions to ascertain information regarding demographic characteristics, socioeconomic status, medical history, health-related lifestyle behaviors (e.g., smoking status, alcohol consumption), and use of medications.
BMI was calculated using participants’ weight in kg and height in m2 (kg/m2). Seated systolic and diastolic blood pressures at rest were measured three times and the average of three measurements were used. Diabetes mellitus was defined as fasting plasma glucose ≥ 126 mg/dL, hemoglobin A1c ≥ 6.5%, diagnosis of diabetes mellitus, and/or use of insulin or oral hypoglycemic medications. Hypertension was defined as systolic blood pressure ≥ 140 mmHg, diastolic blood pressure ≥ 90 mmHg, diagnosis of hypertension, and/or use of anti-hypertensive medication. Hyperlipidemia was defined as having at least one of the following conditions: high cholesterol, high triglycerides, low high-density lipoprotein (HDL), diagnosed with hyperlipidemia, or taking lipid-lowering medication. Cerebrovascular disease was defined as at least one of the following conditions: myocardial infarction, congestive heart failure, coronary artery disease, and/or cerebrovascular disease.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics, including means and standard deviations (SDs) for continuous variables and frequencies and percentages for categorical variables, were used to summarize participant characteristics of cognitive function on exposure to ambient air pollutants. For ambient PM levels and K-MMSE scores, Pearson correlation coefficients adjusted with and without age were computed to explore the relationships between PM and cognitive function.
For the primary analysis, the associations between short-, medium-, and long-term exposures to PM and cognitive function were assessed using a (1) multivariable-adjusted linear regression model for continuous K-MMSE scores and (2) multivariable-adjusted logistic regression model for dichotomous K-MMSE outcomes (normal cognitive function vs. decreased cognitive function), while each PM level was included as a dependent variable in a separate statistical model.
The β-coefficients from the linear regression models were based on an interquartile range increase at each PM level, while the odds ratios from the logistic regression models were based on a one-unit increase in each PM level.
The list of potential covariates included age in years, sex (women or men), BMI in kg/m2, educational level (below elementary school, elementary school, middle and high school, college and above), marital status (married, separated or divorced, widowed, others (e.g., single, living together)), geographical region (Ansan or Ansung), physical activity (yes or no as to whether the study participants regularly exercise to make their bodies sweat at least once a week), smoking (never, former, current), alcohol consumption (never, former, moderate, heavy), diabetes mellitus (yes or no), hypertension (yes or no), hyperlipidemia (yes or no), cerebrovascular disease (yes or no), prior diagnosis of cancer (yes or no), relative temperature in °C, relative humidity in %, and season of participants’ study visit. We included relative temperature and humidity, as well as the season of the study visit, based on their relationship with PM concentrations documented in the literature [19,20].
To avoid multicollinearity, the variation inflation factor (VIF) was examined before including potential covariates in the statistical model. Variables with a VIF greater than 10 were removed from the list of covariates and were not adjusted in the statistical models. The humidity, temperature, and season of the study visit were incorporated into the statistical models because of their direct relationships with ambient air pollutants.
Linearity tests for the association between PM and cognitive function were performed based on fitting semiparametric generalized additive models while adjusting the identical list of covariates described above [21]. Each semiparametric generalized additive model included the PM variable as a spline smoothing spline term and other variates as strict linear terms. To assess the potential non-linear association, models using the tertiles of each PM exposure level were constructed to examine their association with cognitive function, while comparing the lowest PM exposure group (tertile 1 as a reference) with intermediate (tertile 2) and highest (tertile 3) PM exposure groups.
As a secondary analysis, multivariable-adjusted linear analysis with a forward selection procedure was performed to identify the sets of variables that predicted cognitive function the most. Each PM level was entered into a separate model, with variables added to the model based on their significance level (p-value cutoff of 0.5). The list of variables included in the model was the same as the potential covariates included in the primary analysis, except for relative temperature, humidity, and season, which were included in the primary analysis because of their relationship with air pollutants.
We assessed the statistical significance of the potential effect modifiers by including a term for the interaction between PM exposures and each of the following variable in the multivariable-adjusted linear model: sex, age group (50–65 years, 65–75 years, >75 years), alcohol consumption, physical activity, and smoking status. Further, we performed stratified subgroup analyses according to the potential effect modifiers.
A two-tailed p-value of less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Statistical analyses were conducted using SAS software (version 9.4; SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA).

3. Results

Study Sample Characteristics

Descriptive statistics provided in Table 1 reveal the demographic, physical, clinical, and lifestyle characteristics of the 4175 study participants. The distribution of all ambient PM levels and K-MMSE scores was approximately normal, with skewness of ±2 and kurtosis of ±7. Among the 4175 participants, the mean age (SD) was 67.8 (7.9) years, with a range of 52–82 years. Of the group, 55.2% were women, while the mean BMI (SD) was 24.5 (3.3) kg/m2, with 40.9% of participants classified as obese (i.e., BMI of 25 kg/m2 or higher). More than three-quarters of the participants were married (n = 3293), while 36.5% had completed middle and high school. Hypertension showed the highest prevalence rate (52.7%), followed by hyperlipidemia (43.9%) and diabetes mellitus (22.0%). Cerebrovascular disease was least prevalent (7.2%), followed by cancer (11.7%). A total of 926 of 4175 (22.2%) participants were classified as having decreased cognitive function (K-MMSE score of 23 or less).
The distributions of PM2.5 and PM10 levels according to the duration of exposure (short, medium, and long term) are shown in Table 2. The average exposure concentrations of both PM2.5 and PM10 gradually increased from short to long term, reaching a peak at the 3-year average. As the PM levels generally decreased over the years of this study population, the 3-year average of PM2.5 and PM10 levels exhibited the highest value. The average (SD) 3-year exposures to PM2.5 and PM10 were 28.7 (4.4) µg/m3 and 52.4 (3.5) µg/m3, respectively.
The results of the unadjusted and age-adjusted partial Pearson correlations between PM exposure and cognitive scores are shown in Table 3. Simple Pearson correlations indicated that short-, medium-, and long-term exposures to PM2.5 were negatively associated with cognitive scores, ranging from −0.35 to −0.06, whereas only medium- to long-term exposures (average of 10 to 14 days, average of 1 and 6 months, and average of 1, 2, and 3 years) to PM10 were negatively associated with cognitive scores, ranging from −0.34 to −0.03. This trend of association generally persisted when these correlations were adjusted for age. Several exceptions include the following: 1-day PM2.5 value and short-term PM10 exposures became non-significant.
For continuous cognitive scores as an outcome, the associations between various PM exposures and cognitive scores are presented in Table 4. All potential covariates listed in Section 2 were included in the multivariable-adjusted model, except for geographical location, because of a high VIF of greater than 10. Most of the short-, medium-, and long-term exposures to PM2.5 were negatively associated with cognitive scores, confirming that higher exposure to PM2.5, regardless of the exposure duration, was associated with decreased cognitive function. For instance, an IQR increase (9.4 µg/m3) in 3-year average PM2.5 was associated with a 1.16-point decrease in the K-MMSE score. In addition, the higher concentrations of short-term exposure to PM2.5 were significantly associated with cognitive scores, except for the day of the study visit (i.e., 1-day) and the 2-day average. For PM10 exposure, the 1–12-day average concentrations were not associated with cognitive function. With no exceptions, associations with cognitive score were seen for 13-day average exposure and after for an IQR increase in PM10, and this included all of the medium- to long-term exposures to PM10.
Significant nonlinear associations were detected by fitting semiparametric models for 8-day (p = 0.02), 10-day (p = 0.02), and 3-year (p = 0.04) average exposure levels of PM2.5 on cognitive score (P for linearity, Table 4). Multivariable-adjusted regression and logistic analyses were repeated using tertiles of PM exposure levels (Supplementary Tables S1 and S2). As listed in Supplementary Table S2, the analysis by tertiles of PM levels further showed that intermediate and high levels of exposure to short-, medium-, and long-term PM2.5 (tertile 2 and 3) were closely associated with decreased cognitive function, as compared to the lowest (tertile 1) exposure to PM2.5, suggesting the importance of the level of exposure. For 2-year average PM2.5, a high exposure level of PM2.5 (tertile 3) was associated with a 0.76-point decrease in K-MMSE score compared with those who had the lowest level of PM2.5 exposure (tertile 1). For medium- to long-term exposure to PM10, intermediate (tertile 2) to high (tertile 3) levels of PM10 exposure were more closely associated with lower K-MMSE scores compared to the lowest PM10 exposure (tertile 1).
For cognitive function as a dichotomous variable (normal vs. decreased cognitive function), the relationships between the various durations of PM exposure and cognitive function are shown in Supplementary Table S1. Consistent with the results derived from the multivariable-adjusted linear regression models, short-, medium-, and long-term exposure to higher levels of PM2.5 and medium- to long-term exposure to PM10 were associated with decreased cognitive function. For PM2.5, except for the 1-day PM exposure level, higher short-term exposure levels were associated with decreased cognitive function, whereas most of the short-term exposure to PM10 was not associated with decreased cognitive function. Similar trends were noted in the analysis according to the tertiles of PM2.5 and PM10.
Forward selection regression analyses identified a subset of variables predictive of cognitive function (Table 5). Variables that reflect short-, medium-, and long-term exposure to PM2.5 were selected by this procedure, except for the 1-day PM2.5 exposure level. For PM10, only medium- to long-term exposure to ambient air pollution was included in the selection model.
The model consistently selected educational level, age, physical activity, diabetes mellitus, marital status, and smoking status. Based on the results of the multivariable-adjusted regression model, older age and diabetes were associated with decreased cognitive function. Being married, exercising regularly, and having a higher educational level were generally associated with higher cognitive function (data not shown). Depending upon the type of air pollution data in different periods, smoking status was entered in the model in the 6th or 7th order, and the current-smoker group was associated with decreased cognitive function. BMI was also selected when long-term exposure to PM was considered in the pool of model selection. The R2 value ranged from 0.37 to 0.39, confirming that this list of variables explains 37–39% of the variance in cognitive function.
Results from the multivariable-adjusted linear regression analyses with the interaction term between PM and selected potential effect modifiers on cognitive score, and the subgroup analysis stratified by each group, are shown in Supplementary Tables S3–S7. The interaction terms were significant for sex, age group, alcohol consumption, physical activity, and smoking status with long-term exposure to ambient PM2.5 and PM10 on cognitive score. Based on the stratified analysis, we found that the adverse effect of long-term PM exposure on cognitive function was higher in (1) women compared to men, (2) >75 years group compared to younger age groups, (3) non-drinkers compared to former or current drinkers, (4) non-exercise group compared to exercise group, and (5) never smokers compared to current or former smokers.

4. Discussion

4.1. Principal Findings

The current study explored the associations between ambient PM exposure levels under various durations, including short-, medium-, and long-term exposures to PM2.5 and PM10, and cognitive function in community-dwelling, middle-aged-to-elderly adults in South Korea. Our principal findings are 4-fold. First, higher exposure to ambient PM2.5 and PM10 concentrations during the medium- to long-term period was associated with decreased cognitive function, even after adjusting for multiple covariates that are known to be associated with air pollution exposure and cognitive function. Second, between PM2.5 and PM10, only higher exposure to PM2.5 concentrations during the short term was associated with decreased cognitive performance after accounting for various covariates, confirming the differences in the association of PM2.5 and PM10 on cognitive function during the short-term exposure period. Third, this study found several factors that were significantly associated with cognitive function, including PM exposure, education level, age, physical activity, diabetes mellitus, marital status, and smoking status. BMI was also selected when long-term air pollution data were considered in the model. Fourth, significant effect modifications of sex, age group, alcohol consumption, physical activity, and smoking status were observed in the association between long-term PM2.5 and PM10 exposure and cognitive function. Overall, these findings add to the growing body of literature showing that medium- and long-term exposure to ambient PM is associated with cognitive decline in middle-aged and older adults in Korea. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the association of ambient PM2.5 and PM10 exposure under various durations of short, medium, and long term on cognitive function in middle-aged-to-elderly populations in Korea for a relatively large sample size of 4175 participants.

4.2. In the Context of the Current Literature

The current literature lacks consistency in reporting the association between air pollution exposure and cognitive performance, presumably due to the differences in participants’ characteristics, exposure levels of ambient PM, the methodology and techniques in assessing PM, duration of PM exposure, study designs, outcomes, and covariates [22,23,24]. Therefore, a direct comparison between our results and those of prior studies requires caution. However, when compared with the studies that assessed cognitive function using the MMSE, the trend of the association is favorable in supporting the evidence of the harmful effect of PM on cognitive function. A study of 1054 Taiwanese participants over 60 years of age reported a significant association between yearly estimated exposure to PM2.5 and decreased MMSE score (i.e., decreased cognitive function; p = 0.039), and a relationship between yearly exposure to PM10 and decreased MMSE score assessing the subdomain of the cognitive score, including language, construction, and obey (p = 0.013) [25]. The mean level of PM2.5 and PM10 exposures in this Taiwanese population was higher than the exposure levels in the current study (35.2 vs. 27.7 for PM2.5 and 62.9 vs. 50.6 for PM10 for the Taiwanese study vs. the current study, respectively). Another prospective study of 13,324 Chinese participants with a mean age of 82.4 years assessed the relationship between average annual PM2.5 exposure between 2002 and 2014 and the incidence of poor cognitive function determined using the Chinese version of the MMSE. This study suggested that long-term exposure to PM2.5, as well as 3-year average exposure to PM2.5, were associated with an increased risk of poor cognitive function [26]. Among the studies in South Korea, a study of 1484 elderly participants detected gender differences, with women showing a higher risk of decreased cognitive function assessed by the K-MMSE when exposed to PM10; however, no main effect was observed between PM10 exposure and cognitive function [13]. Another Korean study of 2896 participants older than 70 years of age reported a significant association between the yearly assessed levels of PM2.5, PM10, and the Korean version of the MMSE, which was consistent with our findings. Our study results add to the growing body of evidence that higher exposure to ambient PM2.5 during the short, medium, and long term, as well as PM10 exposure during the medium to long term, are associated with an adverse effect on cognitive function in the middle-aged and older Korean population.
The exposure levels of ambient PM2.5 and PM10 observed in our study population were lower than those reported in China [20], India [27], and Pakistan [28], higher than the levels reported in the United States (US) and Canada [29,30], and within similar ranges as reported in France [31]. Zhou et al. reported the ranges of ambient PM in China using 589 pairs of data encompassing 57 urban and rural cities and regions [20]. The ranges of ambient PM were ~60–100 µg/m3 for PM2.5 and 100–170 µg/m3 for PM10 across urban and rural areas in northern and southern China [20]. These ranges are at least twice as high as those observed in the current dataset assessed in South Korea. Owing to the different levels of exposure to ambient PM, the adverse effects of air pollution may differ depending on the geographical location of the individuals.
Based on the National Ambient Air Quality Standards (NAAQS) established by the US Environmental Protection Agency (EPA), the primary standard for PM2.5 should not exceed 12.0 µg/m3 annually and 35 µg/m3 based on a 24 h average, and the 24 h primary standard for PM10 does not exceed 150 µg/m3 more than once per year on average over a 3-year period. The Korean air quality standard is slightly different, with a 24 h standard of 35 µg/m3 for PM2.5 and 100 µg/m3 for PM10. The annual standard is 15 µg/m3 for PM2.5 and 50 µg/m3 for PM10 [32]. The World Health Organization (WHO) 2021 global air quality guidelines provide the most stringent levels, with a 24 h standard of 15 µg/m3 for PM2.5 and 45 µg/m3 for PM10, and an annual standard of 5 µg/m3 for PM2.5 and 15 µg/m3 for PM10. The annual levels of PM2.5 and PM10 reported in the current study exceeded the Korean air quality standards, whereas the 24 h levels of PM2.5 and PM10 levels were lower than the Korean standards [33]. The lack of association between 1-day PM2.5 level and short-term exposure to PM10 on cognitive function may be due to the lower levels of ambient PM concentrations that the study participants were exposed to. Notably, the overall PM exposure levels of the current study participants exceeded the WHO air quality guidelines, which provide the most conservative levels. Therefore, the study sample included in the current analysis may be at high risk of the harmful effects of air pollution due to the high levels of long-term PM exposure.
This study included a list of covariates that are associated with cognitive function. It is well established by the current literature that the male sex [34], education [35], marriage [36,37], and regular exercise [38] are associated with better cognitive function. In contrast, higher BMI [39], smoking [40], excessive alcohol consumption [41], diabetes mellitus [42], elevated blood pressure [43], cerebrovascular disease [44], and cancer treatment or conditions [45] are related to decreased cognitive function. The results of our forward selection regression (Table 5) model identified several factors from this pool of well-established variables and mirrored the findings from previous studies. Of note, prior studies have explored the underlying mechanisms of these factors. For instance, differences in the type and level of sexual hormone secretions, as well as in the brain structure and functions, may explain the influence of sex on cognitive function [46]. In addition, educational attainment may be causally related to cognitive development, thus affecting cognitive function.
Smoking is a modifiable behavioral factor that can significantly affect health. The association between smoking and cognitive health has been documented extensively, with the evidence suggesting that smoking cessation reduces the associated adverse risks [47,48]. In the context of ambient PM exposure and cognitive function, smoking status as an effect modifier showed different aspects of the association, e.g., never smokers, rather than smokers or quitters, were more prone to the adverse effects of ambient PM exposure [49]. The stratified analysis based on smoking status in this study revealed that PM exposure was related to a decrease in cognitive function among the never-smoker group, which is consistent with the prior finding by Yao et al. [49]. Our study found that most of the associations in former- and current-smoker groups were insignificant. Never smokers may be more vulnerable to the adverse effect of ambient PM exposure as they have not been directly exposed to substances, viz. tobacco, that harm the human body; the bodily reactions and cognitive abilities of never-smokers may be less active than those of smokers or quitters.
Participants from the current study were recruited from two distinctive regions, with one characterized by an urban area (Ansan) and the other by a rural area (Ansung). The two regions had different PM2.5 and PM10 levels, with the rural area having higher levels of both PM2.5 and PM10 during short- to long-term exposure compared to the urban area, which we suspect was influenced by other factors such as the topography of the area and meteorological conditions [50]. The overall regional altitude is higher in Ansung than in Ansan; elevation is closely related to meteorological conditions and, thus, to ambient PM levels. In addition, prior studies have reported that higher PM levels are associated with lower temperature, wind speed, and precipitation due to diffusion and wet deposition [50,51]. Moreover, higher PM is related to lower average relative humidity [51]. Differences in the meteorological data between the two regions were observed, with Ansan having statistically higher levels of average temperature, wind speed, insolation, and surface pressure, and lower relative humidity, which may explain the differences in the PM levels. When the geographical region was entered into the regression model, we found a high VIF between the air pollution data and other variables; thus, the region was removed from the model.
The current study observed that short-, medium-, and long-term exposure to PM2.5 was associated with a decreased cognitive score, but only medium- to long-term exposure to PM10 was associated with cognitive function. The current study hypothesized that PM2.5 would be more closely associated with cognitive function than PM10 due to the following reasons: both PM2.5 and PM10 are inhalable in the lungs; however, due to the smaller size of the particles (2.5 µm or less in diameter), PM2.5 is suspected of causing more adverse health issues than PM10. Ambient PM carries various contaminants (e.g., dioxins, iron, lead) on their surfaces, which act as vehicles for delivering toxins to the brain, causing direct and indirect damage [12]. Considering its smaller particle size, larger surface area, and slower sedimentation speed, PM2.5 is a more suitable carrier for chemical substances than PM10 [52]. Importantly, PM2.5 is more likely to travel and accumulate on the surface of the deeper parts of the lung, triggering tissue damage and inflammation, whereas PM10 is more likely to stay and deposit on the surfaces of the airways, causing less damage [53]. Accordingly, short-term exposure to PM10 may not have a significant influence on cognitive function compared with short-term exposure to PM2.5. As noted above, in conjunction with the Korean air quality standards, the non-significant association between short-term PM10 exposure and cognitive function may be attributed to the lower levels of PM10 exposure.
Emission sources can provide valuable information on ambient PM. Emissions from non-combustion sources are prone to produce disproportionately large and coarse particles that are less likely to enter the deeper side of the lungs or bloodstream than smaller particles [54,55]. In contrast, combustion sources tend to generate large numbers of smaller particles that are biologically active. In addition, emission sources from mobile sources tend to emit particles into the breathing zone instead of aloft through a stack [56]. The smaller particles can enter the bloodstream and brain straight via the olfactory bulbs [57], and they (e.g., ultrafine particles) also tend to coagulate and condense onto larger particles, resulting in the rapid change of the particle size profile. Subsequently, it is essential to know the types of sources and their locations within the study areas. Using particular PM assessment approaches (i.e., the land-use regression model for assessing exposure to vehicle exhaust [58] or the satellite imagery and the Hybrid Single Particle Lagrangian Integrated Trajectory model to classify natural and anthropogenic PM sources [59]) could facilitate the elucidation of the health effects of PM that originate from specific sources [60,61].

4.3. Potential Physiological Mechanisms

The physiological mechanism underlying the health effects of ambient PM exposure, particularly on cognitive function, remains unclear. Discussions of the potential mechanisms that may explain our findings are warranted. One explanation is that metals and various compounds present in ambient PMs enter the circulatory system and trigger systemic inflammation and oxidative stress, which further exhibit harmful effects on cognitive function [62,63]. Ambient PM consists of various types of metals, such as aluminum, lead, arsenic, mercury, iron, copper, nickel, chromium, cadmium, manganese, and zinc, and some of these levels exceed the limits of the WHO or the respective country’s air quality standards [64,65]. These heavy metals, which are present at high levels in polluted ambient air, enter the human body via direct inhalation through the mouth and nose/olfactory epithelium, absorption via the exposed skin, and ingestion into the gastrointestinal tract [66]. As confirmed by a rat model exposed to PM2.5, heavy metals bound to PM2.5 were accumulated in the blood and various organs [67]. For example, aluminum accumulation was significantly increased in the cerebral cortex of PM2.5-treated rats [67]. Aluminum is one of the factors suspected to contribute to the pathogenesis of neurodegenerative diseases [68].
Adverse health effects of other components of ambient PM produced by incomplete combustion, such as polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAHs), dioxins, furans, and dioxin-like polychlorinated biphenyls, have also been extensively documented in the literature [52,69,70]. Rats exposed to PM2.5 containing metallic elements and PAHs (e.g., lead, manganese, aluminum, arsenic, acenaphthene, pyrene, naphthalene, acenaphthylene, benzo[b]fluoranthene, and chrysene; all PAHs > 0.1 mg/mL) had impaired spatial learning, memory, inquiring ability, and sensory function, explained by changes in the ultrastructure of mitochondria and myelin sheaths, as well as abnormal expression in the apoptosis-related proteins [71]. Consequently, the mixture of these PM components may have been linked to pathogenesis. In another recent study, PM2.5 exposure induced pathological injury (i.e., changes in the values of inflammatory cytokines and oxidative stress factors) and deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) damage related to micro-ribonucleic acids (microRNAs) and DNA methylation in rats, suggesting the epigenetic modification capacity of PM exposure [66]. The specific mechanisms require further elucidation; however, the key underlying path of PM-induced effects may include inflammation, oxidative stress, mitochondrial dysfunction, microglial activation, disturbed protein homeostasis, epigenetic modification, genotoxicity, and neuronal death, which leads to neurodegeneration [72]. Future studies are warranted to determine the extent of the association between various types of PMs, including nanoparticles, and to clarify the causal mechanisms between PM exposure and cognitive function.

4.4. Strengths and Limitations

The strengths of this study include: (1) a comprehensive list of PM2.5 and PM10 exposure variables over short-, medium-, and long-term durations; (2) a well-characterized community-dwelling study design based on a relatively large sample of both women and men with an extensive list of covariates; and (3) the merging of air quality data with each participant’s address provided more accurate individual exposure levels of ambient air pollution.
A limitation is the nature of the observational epidemiological study, which limits the causal inferences from our findings. As the PM2.5 exposure duration increased from short to long term, higher levels of PM exposure were associated with more decreased K-MMSE scores, implying the adverse health effect of more prolonged PM exposure on cognitive function. A few exceptions were noted where medium-term exposure had larger effect sizes than some of the long-term exposure levels (i.e., 6-month PM2.5 exposure and 3-month PM10 exposure levels). Although this study adjusted season, relative temperature, relative humidity, and a comprehensive list of factors as covariates in the statistical model, it is possible that some of the unmeasured factors could have led to such results. This study used outdoor PM exposure levels based on the geographic location of the participants’ residences. Our study population was not assessed for indoor PM levels or sources of indoor PM emissions, such as residential heating and cooking. Accordingly, we were unable to consider the indoor air pollution exposure of the participants, which may have resulted in exposure misclassification. A prior study noted that indoor PM level is related to outdoor PM level due to the exchange of air between the two environments; however, indoor PM levels can potentially exceed outdoor PM levels [73,74]. In addition, individual PM exposure is highly dependent on the indoor air environment as people spend most of their time indoors. Thus, both indoor and outdoor PM levels should be considered in upcoming research. Personal particle exposure monitors could be utilized for the assessment of outdoor and indoor PM levels in future studies [75]. This study used PM2.5 and PM10 as measures of ambient air pollution exposure because these are routinely measured for monitoring purposes and have international and national guidelines for air quality standards. However, much of the current research focuses on even smaller particles known as ultrafine/nanoparticles/PM0.1, whose aerodynamic diameter is 0.1 µm (100 nm) or less [46,55]. These ultrafine particles have larger reactive surface areas, greater capacity to carry harmful surface chemicals, higher deposition in the alveoli of the lungs, a complex composition of combustion-derived ultrafine PM, and greater ability to pass into the circulation and thus undergo rapid accumulation in peripheral organs. Accordingly, ultrafine particles are thought to exhibit even more harmful health effects compared with those from coarse or fine particles [46]. Given that PM10 and PM2.5 contain PM0.1, the associations observed in this current study may be partially attributed to PM0.1. Our study population was recruited from two regions in South Korea, which were rural (Ansung) and medium-sized urban areas (Ansan), and were older than 50 years; thus, the generalizability of our findings is not applicable to individuals living in regions with different levels of ambient PM exposure and other types of population. In addition, chemical analysis of PM2.5 or PM10 on monitoring samples was unavailable in the current study. Further, this study did not measure particle number counts by size category of PM, which can provide valuable information regarding the source of pollutants [76]. Therefore, future investigations that use both the levels of PM and the number of particles during short- to long-term periods to explore their association with cognitive functions are warranted.

5. Conclusions

In community-dwelling Korean adults over 50 years old, the current study reported that higher levels of PM2.5 during short-, medium-, and long-term durations, as well as PM10 during the medium- to long-term duration, were associated with decreased cognitive function, even after accounting for risk factor correlates of ambient air pollution and cognitive function. While variables that are known to be associated with cognitive function are considered, exposure to PM was selected as a predictor of cognitive performance, suggesting that exposure to ambient air pollution should also be considered as a risk factor.
Overall, our results suggest an adverse effect of both PM2.5 and PM10 during the medium to long term, as well as short-term exposure to PM2.5, on the cognitive ability of middle-aged and elderly adults in South Korea. While exposure to air pollution is considered one of the modifiable risk factors, reducing ambient PM requires tremendous effort by multiple levels and types of national and regional government entities. This study underscored the importance of further efforts to reduce the levels and duration of the air pollutant exposures, especially in the vulnerable elderly population, and provided evidence for strengthening policies for air pollution regulations.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph19169913/s1, Supplementary Table S1: Relationship between PM2.5 and PM10 with Decreased Cognitive Function (n = 4175). Supplementary Table S2: Relationship between Tertile of PM2.5 and PM10 and Cognitive Score (n = 4175). Supplementary Table S3: Association between PM2.5 and PM10 and Cognitive Score by Sex (Women = 2305; Men = 1870). Supplementary Table S4: Association between PM2.5 and PM10 and Cognitive Score by Age Group (50–65 years = 1665; 65–75 years = 1483; >75 years = 1027). Supplementary Table S5: Association between PM2.5 and PM10 and Cognitive Score by Alcohol Consumption (Never = 2356; Former = 318; Current = 1501). Supplementary Table S6: Association between PM2.5 and PM10 and Cognitive Score by Physical Activity (No = 2982; Yes = 1193). Supplementary Table S7: Association between PM2.5 and PM10 and Cognitive Score by Smoking Status (Never = 2750; Former = 1007; Current = 418).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.J.L., J.H.K. and K.L.; Methodology, J.J.L. and J.H.K.; Software, J.J.L.; Formal Analysis, J.J.L.; Investigation, J.J.L., J.H.K. and K.L.; Resources: D.S.S.; Data Curation: D.S.S.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, J.J.L.; Writing—Review and Editing, J.J.L., J.H.K., D.S.S. and K.L.; Supervision: K.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Korea National Institute of Health, Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2022-NI-057-00).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Korea National Institute of Health and the Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (IRB#: 2022-04-04-P-A).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data used in this study belong to the Korea National Institute of Health and the Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Due to restrictions placed on the data by the Personal Information Protection Act of Korea, the dataset cannot be made publicly available.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Feng, S.; Gao, D.; Liao, F.; Zhou, F.; Wang, X. The health effects of ambient PM2. 5 and potential mechanisms. Ecotoxicol. Environ. Saf. 2016, 128, 67–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Cohen, A.J.; Brauer, M.; Burnett, R.; Anderson, H.R.; Frostad, J.; Estep, K.; Balakrishnan, K.; Brunekreef, B.; Dandona, L.; Dandona, R. Estimates and 25-year trends of the Global Burden of Disease attributable to ambient air pollution: An analysis of data from the global burden of diseases study 2015. Lancet 2017, 389, 1907–1918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. World Health Organization. Ambient (Outdoor) Air Pollution. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/ambient-(outdoor)-air-quality-and-health (accessed on 26 July 2022).
  4. Du, Y.; Xu, X.; Chu, M.; Guo, Y.; Wang, J. Air particulate matter and cardiovascular disease: The epidemiological, biomedical and clinical evidence. J. Thorac. Dis. 2016, 8, E8. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bates, J.T.; Fang, T.; Verma, V.; Zeng, L.; Weber, R.J.; Tolbert, P.E.; Abrams, J.Y.; Sarnat, S.E.; Klein, M.; Mulholland, J.A. Review of acellular assays of ambient particulate matter oxidative potential: Methods and relationships with composition, sources and health effects. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2019, 53, 4003–4019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. Report On The Environment—Particulate Matter Emissions. Available online: https://www.epa.gov/roe/ (accessed on 13 April 2022).
  7. Qin, S.; Li, B.; Wang, X.; Huang, H.; Zeng, M.; Xiao, F.; Xu, X. Metal element detection and carcinogenicity risk assessment of PM2. 5 samples. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 2020, 39, 1273–1276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Li, Z.; Wen, Q.; Zhang, R. Sources, health effects and control strategies of indoor fine particulate matter (PM2.5): A review. Sci. Total Environ. 2017, 586, 610–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Air Korea. Air Pollutants. Available online: https://www.airkorea.or.kr/web/airmatter?Pmenu_No=130 (accessed on 13 April 2022).
  10. Almetwally, A.A.; Bin-Jumah, M.; Allam, A.A. Ambient air pollution and its influence on human health and welfare: An overview. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 2020, 27, 24815–24830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Major Population Indicators (Ratio, Population Growth Rate, Population Structure, Dependency Ratio, Etc.)/NationwideFuture Population Estimation, National Statistical Office (Reference: 02-2012-9114, 042-481-2261). Available online: https://kosis.kr/statHtml/statHtml.do?mode=tab&orgid=101&tblid=DT_1BPA002 (accessed on 13 April 2021).
  12. Peeples, L. How air pollution threatens brain health. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 13856–13860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Kim, Y.; Han, B.-G.; Group, K. Cohort profile: The Korean genome and epidemiology study (koges) consortium. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2017, 46, E20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Creavin, S.T.; Wisniewski, S.; Noel-Storr, A.H.; Trevelyan, C.M.; Hampton, T.; Rayment, D.; Thom, V.M.; Nash, K.J.; Elhamoui, H.; Milligan, R. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the detection of dementia in clinically unevaluated people aged 65 and over in community and primary care populations. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2016, 2016, CD011145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kang, Y.; Na, D.-L.; Hahn, S. A validity study on the Korean Mini-Mental State Examination (K-MMSE) in dementia patients. J. Korean Neurol. Assoc. 1997, 15, 300–308. [Google Scholar]
  16. Kim, H.; Noh, J.; Noh, Y.; Oh, S.S.; Koh, S.-B.; Kim, C. Gender difference in the effects of outdoor air pollution on cognitive function among elderly in Korea. Front. Public Health 2019, 7, 375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hwang, M.-J.; Sung, J.; Yoon, M.; Kim, J.-H.; Yun, H.-Y.; Choi, D.-R.; Koo, Y.-S.; Oh, K.; Yun, S.; Cheong, H.-K. Establishment of the Korea National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey air pollution study dataset for the researchers on the health impact of ambient air pollution. Epidemiol. Health 2021, 43, e2021015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Koo, Y.; Choi, D.; Yun, H.; Yoon, G.; Lee, J. A development of PM concentration reanalysis method using cmaq with surface data assimilation and MAIAC AOD in Korea. J. Korean Soc. Atmos. Environ. 2020, 36, 558–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Tian, X.; Fang, Z.; Liu, W. decreased humidity improves cognitive performance at extreme high indoor temperature. Indoor Air 2021, 31, 608–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Zhou, X.; Cao, Z.; Ma, Y.; Wang, L.; Wu, R.; Wang, W. Concentrations, correlations and chemical species of PM2.5/PM10 based on published data in China: Potential implications for the revised particulate standard. Chemosphere 2016, 144, 518–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Cai, W. Fitting generalized additive models with the gam procedure. In Sas 9.2, Sas Global Forum; Sas Institute Inc.: Cary, NC, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  22. Weuve, J.; Bennett, E.E.; Ranker, L.; Gianattasio, K.Z.; Pedde, M.; Adar, S.D.; Yanosky, J.D.; Power, M.C. Exposure to air pollution in relation to risk of dementia and related outcomes: An updated systematic review of the epidemiological literature. Environ. Health Perspect. 2021, 129, 96001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Park, S.Y.; Han, J.; Kim, S.H.; Suk, H.W.; Park, J.E.; Lee, D.Y. Impact of Long-Term Exposure to Air Pollution on Cognitive Decline in Older Adults Without Dementia. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2022, 86, 553–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Gao, X.; Coull, B.; Lin, X.; Vokonas, P.; Spiro, A.; Hou, L.; Schwartz, J.; Baccarelli, A.A. Short-term air pollution, cognitive performance and nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drug use in the Veterans Affairs Normative Aging Study. Nat. Aging 2021, 1, 430–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Chen, M.-C.; Wang, C.-F.; Lai, B.-C.; Hsieh, S.-W.; Chen, S.-C.; Hung, C.-H.; Kuo, C.-H. Air pollution is associated with poor cognitive function in taiwanese adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Wang, J.; Li, T.; Lv, Y.; Kraus, V.B.; Zhang, Y.; Mao, C.; Yin, Z.; Shi, W.; Zhou, J.; Zheng, T. Fine particulate matter and poor cognitive function among Chinese older adults: Evidence from a community-based, 12-year prospective cohort study. Environ. Health Perspect. 2020, 128, 67013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Maji, K.J.; Dikshit, A.K.; Deshpande, A. Disability-adjusted life years and economic cost assessment of the health effects related to PM2.5 and PM10 pollution in mumbai and delhi, in india from 1991 to 2015. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 2017, 24, 4709–4730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Mehmood, T.; Tianle, Z.; Ahmad, I.; Li, X.; Shen, F.; Akram, W.; Dong, L. Variations of PM2.5, PM10 Mass Concentration and Health Assessment in Islamabad, Pakistan. Iop Conf. Ser. Earth Environ. Sci. 2018, 133, 012031. [Google Scholar]
  29. Celo, V.; Yassine, M.M.; Dabek-Zlotorzynska, E. Insights into Elemental Composition and Sources of Fine and Coarse Particulate Matter in Dense Traffic Areas in Toronto and Vancouver, Canada. Toxics 2021, 9, 264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. Particulate Matter (PM2.5) Trends. Available online: https://www.epa.gov/air-trends/particulate-matter-pm25-trends (accessed on 13 April 2022).
  31. Duchesne, J.; Gutierrez, L.-A.; Carrière, I.; Mura, T.; Chen, J.; Vienneau, D.; de Hoogh, K.; Helmer, C.; Jacquemin, B.; Berr, C. Exposure to ambient air pollution and cognitive decline: Results of the prospective Three-City cohort study. Environ. Int. 2022, 161, 107118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Air Korea. Atmospheric Environment Standards. Available online: https://www.airkorea.or.kr/web/contents/contentview/?Pmenu_No=133&cntnts_No=7 (accessed on 13 April 2022).
  33. World Health Organization. Who Global Air Quality Guidelines: Particulate Matter (PM2.5 and PM10), Ozone, Nitrogen Dioxide, Sulfur Dioxide And Carbon Monoxide; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  34. Matthews, F.; Marioni, R.; Brayne, C. Examining the influence of gender, education, social class and birth cohort on MMSE tracking over time: A population-based prospective cohort study. BMC Geriatr. 2012, 12, 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Lövdén, M.; Fratiglioni, L.; Glymour, M.M.; Lindenberger, U.; Tucker-Drob, E.M. Education and cognitive functioning across the life span. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 2020, 21, 6–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Shin, S.H.; Kim, G.; Park, S. Widowhood status as a risk factor for cognitive decline among older adults. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2018, 26, 778–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Xu, P.-R.; Wei, R.; Cheng, B.-J.; Wang, A.-J.; Li, X.-D.; Li, H.-B.; Sun, L.; Du, J.; Sheng, J.; Liu, K.-Y. The association of marital status with cognitive function and the role of gender in Chinese community-dwelling older adults: A cross-sectional study. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 2021, 33, 2273–2281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Barnes, J.N. Exercise, cognitive function, and aging. Adv. Physiol. Educ. 2015, 39, 55–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Cournot, M.; Marquie, J.; Ansiau, D.; Martinaud, C.; Fonds, H.; Ferrieres, J.; Ruidavets, J. Relation between body mass index and cognitive function in healthy middle-aged men and women. Neurology 2006, 67, 1208–1214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Mons, U.; Schöttker, B.; Müller, H.; Kliegel, M.; Brenner, H. History of lifetime smoking, smoking cessation and cognitive function in the elderly population. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2013, 28, 823–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kim, J.W.; Lee, D.Y.; Lee, B.C.; Jung, M.H.; Kim, H.; Choi, Y.S.; Choi, I.-G. Alcohol and cognition in the elderly: A review. Psychiatry Investig. 2012, 9, 8–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Mansur, R.B.; Lee, Y.; Zhou, A.J.; Carmona, N.E.; Cha, D.S.; Rosenblat, J.D.; Bruins, R.; Kakar, R.; Rasgon, N.L.; Lovshin, J.A. Determinants of cognitive function in individuals with type 2 diabetes mellitus: A meta-analysis. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry 2018, 30, 38–50. [Google Scholar]
  43. Birns, J.; Kalra, L. Cognitive function and hypertension. J. Hum. Hypertens. 2009, 23, 86–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Kalaria, R.N. Cerebrovascular disease and mechanisms of cognitive impairment: Evidence from clinicopathological studies in humans. Stroke 2012, 43, 2526–2534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Asher, A.; Myers, J.S. The effect of cancer treatment on cognitive function. Clin. Adv. Hematol. Oncol. 2015, 13, 441–450. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  46. Miller, M.R.; Newby, D.E. Air pollution and cardiovascular disease: Car sick. Cardiovasc. Res. 2020, 116, 279–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Baumgart, M.; Snyder, H.M.; Carrillo, M.C.; Fazio, S.; Kim, H.; Johns, H. Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: A population-based perspective. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2015, 11, 718–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Sabia, S.; Elbaz, A.; Dugravot, A.; Head, J.; Shipley, M.; Hagger-Johnson, G.; Kivimaki, M.; Singh-Manoux, A. Impact of smoking on cognitive decline in early old age: The Whitehall II cohort study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2012, 69, 627–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Yao, Y.; Wang, K.; Xiang, H. association between cognitive function and ambient particulate matters in middle-aged and elderly chinese adults: Evidence from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). Sci. Total Environ. 2022, 828, 154297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Zalakeviciute, R.; López-Villada, J.; Rybarczyk, Y. Contrasted effects of relative humidity and precipitation on urban PM2.5 pollution in high elevation urban areas. Sustainability 2018, 10, 2064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Li, Y.; Chen, Q.; Zhao, H.; Wang, L.; Tao, R. Variations in PM10, PM2.5 and PM1.0 in an urban area of the Sichuan Basin and their relation to meteorological factors. Atmosphere 2015, 6, 150–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Shou, Y.; Huang, Y.; Zhu, X.; Liu, C.; Hu, Y.; Wang, H. A Review of the possible associations between ambient PM2.5 exposures and the development of alzheimer’s disease. Ecotoxicol. Environ. Saf. 2019, 174, 344–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. California Air Resources Board. Inhalable Particulate Matter and Health (PM2.5 and PM10). Available online: https://ww2.arb.ca.gov/resources/inhalable-particulate-matter-and-health#:~:text=on%20particle%20size.-,pm2.tissue%20damage%2c%20and%20lung%20inflammation (accessed on 13 April 2022).
  54. Calderón-Garcidueñas, L.; Ayala, A. Air Pollution, Ultrafine Particles, and Your brain: Are Combustion Nanoparticle Emissions and Engineered Nanoparticles Causing Preventable Fatal Neurodegenerative Diseases and Common Neuropsychiatric Outcomes? Environ. Sci. Technol. 2022, 56, 6847–6856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Kwon, H.-S.; Ryu, M.H.; Carlsten, C. Ultrafine particles: Unique physicochemical properties relevant to health and disease. Exp. Mol. Med. 2020, 52, 318–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Querol, X.; Alastuey, A.; Rodriguez, S.; Plana, F.; Mantilla, E.; Ruiz, C.R. Monitoring of PM10 and PM2.5 around primary particulate anthropogenic emission sources. Atmos. Environ. 2001, 35, 845–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Morawska, L.; Zhang, J.J. Combustion sources of particles. 1. Health relevance and source signatures. Chemosphere 2002, 49, 1045–1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Oudin, A.; Forsberg, B.; Adolfsson, A.N.; Lind, N.; Modig, L.; Nordin, M.; Nordin, S.; Adolfsson, R.; Nilsson, L.-G. Traffic-related air pollution and dementia incidence in northern Sweden: A longitudinal study. Environ. Health Perspect. 2016, 124, 306–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Guan, Q.; Liu, Z.; Yang, L.; Luo, H.; Yang, Y.; Zhao, R.; Wang, F. Variation in PM2.5 source over megacities on the ancient Silk Road, northwestern China. J. Clean. Prod. 2019, 208, 897–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Sinharay, R.; Gong, J.; Barratt, B.; Ohman-Strickland, P.; Ernst, S.; Kelly, F.J.; Zhang, J.J.; Collins, P.; Cullinan, P.; Chung, K.F. Respiratory and cardiovascular responses to walking down a traffic-polluted road compared with walking in a traffic-free area in participants aged 60 years and older with chronic lung or heart disease and age-matched healthy controls: A randomised, crossover study. Lancet 2018, 391, 339–349. [Google Scholar]
  61. Gerlofs-Nijland, M.E.; Bokkers, B.G.; Sachse, H.; Reijnders, J.J.; Gustafsson, M.; Boere, A.J.F.; Fokkens, P.; Leseman, D.L.; Augsburg, K.; Cassee, F.R. Inhalation Toxicity profiles of particulate matter: A comparison between brake wear with other sources of emission. Inhal. Toxicol. 2019, 31, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Han, B.; Xu, J.; Zhang, Y.; Li, P.; Li, K.; Zhang, N.; Han, J.; Gao, S.; Wang, X.; Geng, C. Associations of Exposure to Fine Particulate Matter Mass and Constituents with Systemic Inflammation: A Cross-Sectional Study of Urban Older Adults in China. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2022, 56, 7244–7255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Cherbuin, N.; Walsh, E.; Baune, B.; Anstey, K. Oxidative stress, inflammation and risk of neurodegeneration in a population sample. Eur. J. Neurol. 2019, 26, 1347–1354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Kermani, M.; Jonidi Jafari, A.; Gholami, M.; Arfaeinia, H.; Shahsavani, A.; Fanaei, F. Characterization, possible sources and health risk assessment of PM2.5-bound Heavy Metals in the most industrial city of iran. J. Environ. Health Sci. Eng. 2021, 19, 151–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Li, F.; Yan, J.; Wei, Y.; Zeng, J.; Wang, X.; Chen, X.; Zhang, C.; Li, W.; Chen, M.; Lü, G. PM2.5-bound heavy metals from the major cities in China: Spatiotemporal distribution, fuzzy exposure assessment and health risk management. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 286, 124967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Zhao, Z.; Luo, X.-S.; Jing, Y.; Li, H.; Pang, Y.; Wu, L.; Chen, Q.; Jin, L. In Vitro assessments of bioaccessibility and bioavailability of PM2.5 trace metals in respiratory and digestive systems and their oxidative potential. J. Hazard. Mater. 2021, 409, 124638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Li, Q.; Liu, H.; Alattar, M.; Jiang, S.; Han, J.; Ma, Y.; Jiang, C. The preferential accumulation of heavy metals in different tissues following frequent respiratory exposure to PM2.5 in rats. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 16936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Mold, M.J.; O’Farrell, A.; Morris, B.; Exley, C. Aluminum and tau in neurofibrillary tangles in familial Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. Rep. 2021, 5, 283–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Fadel, M.; Ledoux, F.; Seigneur, M.; Oikonomou, K.; Sciare, J.; Courcot, D.; Afif, C. Chemical profiles of PM2.5 emitted from various anthropogenic sources of the Eastern Mediterranean: Cooking, wood burning, and diesel generators. Environ. Res. 2022, 211, 113032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Bandyopadhyay, A. Neurological disorders from ambient (urban) air pollution emphasizing UFPM and PM2.5. Curr. Pollut. Rep. 2016, 2, 203–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Zhang, Q.; Li, Q.; Ma, J.; Zhao, Y. PM2.5 Impairs neurobehavior by oxidative stress and myelin sheaths injury of brain in the rat. Environ. Pollut. 2018, 242, 994–1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Jankowska-Kieltyka, M.; Roman, A.; Nalepa, I. The Air We Breathe: Air Pollution as a Prevalent Proinflammatory Stimulus Contributing to Neurodegeneration. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 647643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Zhang, L.; Ou, C.; Magana-Arachchi, D.; Vithanage, M.; Vanka, K.S.; Palanisami, T.; Masakorala, K.; Wijesekara, H.; Yan, Y.; Bolan, N. Indoor particulate matter in urban households: Sources, pathways, characteristics, health effects, and exposure mitigation. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Liu, C.; Zhang, Y. Relations between indoor and outdoor PM2.5 and constituent concentrations. Front. Environ. Sci. Eng. 2019, 13, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Wang, K.; Chen, F.-E.; Au, W.; Zhao, Z.; Xia, Z.-L. Evaluating the feasibility of a personal particle exposure monitor in outdoor and indoor microenvironments in Shanghai, China. Int. J. Environ. Health Res. 2019, 29, 209–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Onat, B.; Stakeeva, B. Personal exposure of commuters in public transport to PM2. 5 and fine particle counts. Atmos. Pollut. Res. 2013, 4, 329–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flowchart of study method.
Figure 1. Flowchart of study method.
Ijerph 19 09913 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of the study participants (n = 4175).
Table 1. Characteristics of the study participants (n = 4175).
CharacteristicMean or NStandard Deviation or %
Age (years)67.87.9
Sex (%)
Women230555.2
Men187044.8
Age Group (%)
50–65 years166539.9
65–75 years148335.5
Above 75 years102724.6
BMI (kg/m2)24.53.3
BMI Group (%) a
Underweight1112.7
Normal Weight128730.8
Overweight106925.6
Obese 170840.9
Marital Status (%)
Married329378.9
Separated or Divorced962.3
Widowed76418.3
Others (e.g., Single, Living Together)220.5
Education Level (%)
Below Elementary School87921.1
Elementary School130731.3
Middle and High School152536.5
College and Above46411.1
Season (%)
Spring111826.8
Summer156137.4
Fall123629.6
Winter2606.2
Geographical Location (%)
Ansan156737.5
Ansung260862.5
Physical Activity (%)
No298271.4
Yes119328.6
Alcohol Consumption (%)
Never235656.4
Former3187.6
Moderate Drinker116627.9
Heavy Drinker3358.0
Smoking Status (%)
Never275065.9
Former100724.1
Current41810.0
Diabetes Mellitus (%)
No325578.0
Yes92022.0
Hypertension (%)
No197747.4
Yes219852.7
Cerebrovascular Disease
No387692.8
Yes2997.2
Hyperlipidemia (%)
No234156.1
Yes183443.9
Cancer (%)
No368888.3
Yes48711.7
K-MMSE Score25.93.6
K-MMSE Category (%) b
Normal Cognitive Function324977.8
Decreased Cognitive Function92622.2
BMI, Body Mass Index; K-MMSE, Korean version of Mini-Mental State Examination. a Underweight was defined as BMI less than 18.5 kg/m2; normal weight was defined as BMI 18.5 kg/m2 or higher and less than 23 kg/m2; overweight was defined as BMI 23 kg/m2 or higher and less than 25 kg/m2; obese was defined as BMI 25 kg/m2 or higher. b Normal cognitive function was defined as MMSE score of 24 or higher; decreased cognitive function was defined as MMSE score of 23 or lower.
Table 2. Distribution of PM2.5 and PM10 levels according to duration of exposure (n = 4175).
Table 2. Distribution of PM2.5 and PM10 levels according to duration of exposure (n = 4175).
Average ValuesPM2.5 (µg/m3)PM10 (µg/m3)
MeanStandard DeviationMeanStandard Deviation
1-Day26.013.047.021.3
2-Day26.011.846.619.0
3-Day25.910.746.517.5
4-Day25.99.846.716.6
5-Day25.99.146.915.4
6-Day25.98.547.114.6
1-Week25.98.047.213.9
8-Day25.87.647.213.3
9-Day25.77.347.112.7
10-Day25.77.247.212.5
11-Day25.77.147.212.4
12-Day25.77.047.212.4
13-Day25.77.047.212.4
2-Week25.87.047.312.4
1-Month26.36.248.110.8
3-Month27.25.450.29.2
6-Month28.24.752.05.9
1-Year27.74.150.62.6
2-Year28.14.351.53.0
3-Year28.74.452.43.5
Table 3. Pearson correlations between PM2.5 and PM10 with cognitive score, with and without age adjustment (n = 4175).
Table 3. Pearson correlations between PM2.5 and PM10 with cognitive score, with and without age adjustment (n = 4175).
Average VariablesPM2.5 (µg/m3)PM10 (µg/m3)
rp-ValueAge-Adjusted rp-Valuerp-ValueAge-Adjusted rp-Value
1-Day−0.060.0002−0.020.240.0020.890.010.35
2-Day −0.08<0.0001−0.040.02−0.010.370.00030.99
3-Day −0.10<0.0001−0.050.001−0.020.17−0.010.74
4-Day −0.10<0.0001−0.060.0003−0.020.20−0.0040.81
5-Day −0.11<0.0001−0.060.0003−0.020.26−0.0020.90
6-Day −0.11<0.0001−0.050.001−0.020.210.0020.92
1-Week −0.11<.0001−0.050.001−0.020.240.010.70
8-Day −0.12<0.0001−0.060.0003−0.020.140.010.74
9-Day −0.13<0.0001−0.06<0.0001−0.030.060.0020.91
10-Day −0.14<0.0001−0.06<0.0001−0.030.040.0030.83
11-Day −0.14<0.0001−0.07<0.0001−0.030.030.0040.79
12-Day −0.14<0.0001−0.07<0.0001−0.040.020.0050.77
13-Day −0.14<0.0001−0.07<0.0001−0.030.020.0040.78
2-Week −0.14<0.0001−0.06<0.0001−0.030.030.010.73
1-Month −0.16<0.0001−0.060.0002−0.040.020.020.22
3-Month −0.18<0.0001−0.050.002−0.020.220.050.001
6-Month −0.22<0.0001−0.07<0.0001−0.040.010.040.01
1-Year −0.32<0.0001−0.18<0.0001−0.29<0.0001−0.16<0.0001
2-Year −0.35<0.0001−0.19<0.0001−0.33<0.0001−0.18<0.0001
3-Year −0.35<0.0001−0.19<0.0001−0.34<0.0001−0.19<0.0001
Table 4. Relationship between PM2.5 and PM10 and cognitive score (n = 4175).
Table 4. Relationship between PM2.5 and PM10 and cognitive score (n = 4175).
Average ValueInterquartile RangeEffect Size aStandard Error ap-Value95% Confidence IntervalP for Linearity b
PM2.5 (µg/m3)
1-Day15.7−0.050.060.39−0.160.060.70
2-Day 13.6−0.100.050.07−0.200.010.30
3-Day 13.0−0.160.060.005−0.28−0.051.00
4-Day 12.3−0.200.060.001−0.32−0.090.61
5-Day 11.7−0.230.060.0004−0.35−0.101.00
6-Day 11.5−0.240.070.0004−0.38−0.110.71
1-Week 11.2−0.270.070.0002−0.41−0.130.19
8-Day 10.4−0.280.07<0.0001−0.42−0.140.02
9-Day 10.1−0.300.07<0.0001−0.44−0.160.85
10-Day 9.6−0.310.07<0.0001−0.45−0.180.02
11-Day 9.3−0.340.07<0.0001−0.48−0.210.44
12-Day 9.1−0.360.07<0.0001−0.49−0.220.61
13-Day 9.3−0.380.07<0.0001−0.52−0.240.26
2-Week 9.3−0.380.07<0.0001−0.52−0.240.09
1-Month8.4−0.490.08<0.0001−0.65−0.320.56
3-Month7.6−0.680.11<0.0001−0.89−0.460.45
6-Month 7.9−0.840.12<0.0001−1.08−0.600.11
1-Year 7.3−0.820.15<0.0001−1.12−0.520.35
2-Year 8.9−1.240.21<0.0001−1.65−0.820.11
3-Year 9.4−1.160.23<0.0001−1.61−0.700.04
PM10 (µg/m3)
1-Day25.10.050.060.37−0.060.160.58
2-Day 22.00.010.060.86−0.100.120.17
3-Day 20.7−0.050.060.39−0.170.060.53
4-Day 19.6−0.080.060.19−0.200.040.33
5-Day 18.6−0.100.070.14−0.230.030.10
6-Day 17.8−0.100.070.17−0.240.040.53
1-Week 17.9−0.100.080.18−0.250.050.61
8-Day 18.3−0.130.080.12−0.290.030.15
9-Day 18.4−0.150.090.10−0.320.030.48
10-Day 17.6−0.130.090.14−0.300.040.65
11-Day 17.5−0.150.090.09−0.320.020.47
12-Day 17.9−0.170.090.06−0.350.010.15
13-Day 18.0−0.200.090.03−0.38−0.020.09
2-Week 18.2−0.210.090.02−0.39−0.030.12
1-Month18.5−0.410.140.004−0.69−0.130.24
3-Month16.0−0.630.180.001−0.99−0.270.20
6-Month 9.3−0.430.150.003−0.72−0.140.40
1-Year 4.7−0.460.120.0002−0.70−0.210.75
2-Year 6.1−0.760.16<0.0001−1.07−0.440.37
3-Year 7.2−0.850.18<0.0001−1.20−0.490.64
a Effect sizes and standard errors are shown per interquartile range increase for each PM level. b Linearity tests for the association between PM and cognitive function were performed based on fitting semiparametric generalized additive models.
Table 5. Relationship between exposure to PM2.5 and PM10 and cognitive score as determined by a forward selection model (n = 4175).
Table 5. Relationship between exposure to PM2.5 and PM10 and cognitive score as determined by a forward selection model (n = 4175).
Average VariablesInterquartile RangeEffect Size aStandard Error ap-ValueAdjusted R2Order Entered for PMEntered Order
PM2.5 (µg/m3)
1-Day15.69---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
2-Day 13.63−0.090.050.070.377Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking, PM2.5
3-Day 13.02−0.140.050.010.387Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking, PM2.5
4-Day 12.31−0.170.060.0020.376Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
5-Day 11.70−0.180.060.0020.376Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
6-Day 11.52−0.180.060.0030.376Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
1-Week 11.16−0.180.060.0030.376Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
8-Day 10.36−0.180.060.0020.376Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
9-Day 10.11−0.200.060.0010.376Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
10-Day 9.59−0.200.060.0010.386Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
11-Day 9.33−0.210.060.00030.376Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
12-Day 9.04−0.210.060.00020.376Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
13-Day 9.25−0.220.060.00020.386Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
2-Week 9.30−0.210.060.00030.376Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
1-Month 8.42−0.210.060.0010.376Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM2.5, Smoking
3-Month 7.60−0.150.070.020.377Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking, PM2.5
6-Month 7.89−0.350.08<0.00010.385Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, PM2.5, Marital Status, Smoking, BMI
1-Year 7.27−0.880.09<0.00010.394Education, Age, PM2.5, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking, BMI
2-Year 8.93−1.090.10<0.00010.393Education, Age, PM2.5, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking, BMI
3-Year 9.35−1.120.10<0.00010.393Education, Age, PM2.5, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking, BMI
PM10 (µg/m3)
1-Day25.07---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
2-Day 21.98---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
3-Day 20.70---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
4-Day 19.64---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
5-Day 18.59---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
6-Day 17.82---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
1-Week 17.94---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
8-Day 18.30---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
9-Day 18.37---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
10-Day 17.57---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
11-Day 17.50---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
12-Day 17.92---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
13-Day 18.01---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
2-Week 18.22---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
1-Month 18.49---0.37Not EnteredEducation, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking
3-Month 15.950.270.080.0010.376Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, PM10, Smoking
6-Month 9.310.180.070.010.377Education, Age, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking, PM10
1-Year 4.69−0.730.09<0.00010.383Education, Age, PM10, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking, BMI
2-Year 6.07−0.930.10<0.00010.393Education, Age, PM10, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking, BMI
3-Year 7.15−1.040.10<0.00010.393Education, Age, PM10, Physical Activity, Diabetes, Marital Status, Smoking, BMI
a Effect sizes and standard errors are shown per interquartile range increase for each PM level.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lee, J.J.; Kim, J.H.; Song, D.S.; Lee, K. Effect of Short- to Long-Term Exposure to Ambient Particulate Matter on Cognitive Function in a Cohort of Middle-Aged and Older Adults: KoGES. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 9913. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19169913

AMA Style

Lee JJ, Kim JH, Song DS, Lee K. Effect of Short- to Long-Term Exposure to Ambient Particulate Matter on Cognitive Function in a Cohort of Middle-Aged and Older Adults: KoGES. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(16):9913. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19169913

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lee, Jane J., Ji Hyun Kim, Dae Sub Song, and Kyoungho Lee. 2022. "Effect of Short- to Long-Term Exposure to Ambient Particulate Matter on Cognitive Function in a Cohort of Middle-Aged and Older Adults: KoGES" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 16: 9913. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19169913

APA Style

Lee, J. J., Kim, J. H., Song, D. S., & Lee, K. (2022). Effect of Short- to Long-Term Exposure to Ambient Particulate Matter on Cognitive Function in a Cohort of Middle-Aged and Older Adults: KoGES. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(16), 9913. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19169913

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop