Next Article in Journal
Machine Learning, Deep Learning, and Mathematical Models to Analyze Forecasting and Epidemiology of COVID-19: A Systematic Literature Review
Previous Article in Journal
Sleep Satisfaction May Modify the Association between Metabolic Syndrome and BMI, Respectively, and Occupational Stress in Japanese Office Workers
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Increased Risk Perception, Distress Intolerance and Health Anxiety in Stricter Lockdowns: Self-Control as a Key Protective Factor in Early Response to the COVID-19 Pandemic

1
Faculty of Education, University of Hamburg, 20146 Hamburg, Germany
2
Leibniz Institute for Science and Mathematics Education (IPN), 24118 Kiel, Germany
3
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Babeş-Bolyai University, 400084 Cluj-Napoca, Romania
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(9), 5098; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19095098
Submission received: 30 March 2022 / Revised: 19 April 2022 / Accepted: 20 April 2022 / Published: 22 April 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Mental Health)

Abstract

:
Studies provide evidence that distress, (health) anxiety, and depressive symptoms were high during the first weeks of COVID-19 lockdown restrictions, decreasing over time (possibly due to individuals’ protective psychological factors). Relations between different lockdown restrictions, mental health issues, and protective factors need to be explored, since even small lockdown effects might increase the risk of future mental health issues. We merged objective lockdown stringency data with individual data (N = 1001) to examine differences in lockdown effects in strict lockdown (Romania) and mild lockdown (Hungary) conditions between March and May 2020 on stressors and mental health symptoms, taking protective factors into account. The stricter lockdown in Romania revealed higher levels of perceived risk of infection, distress intolerance, and COVID-19 health anxiety. Protective psychological factors were not affected by the lockdown measures. Surpassing psychological flexibility and resilient coping, self-control proved to be the most promising protective factor. It is recommended that future research merge objective data with study data to investigate the effects of different COVID-19 lockdown measures on mental health and protective factors. Policy decisions should consider lockdown-dependent consequences of mental health issues. Intervention programs are suggested to mitigate mental health issues and to strengthen peoples’ protective psychological factors.

1. Introduction

In an early stage of the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) pandemic, several countries reacted with strict lockdown and quarantine policies, causing higher levels of mental health symptoms. A higher risk of increased anxiety and depressive symptoms was reported for younger, female, lower-educated, lower-earning persons, or persons living alone [1,2,3]. Prati and Mancini [4] in their meta-analysis found small average effects of lockdowns on anxiety and depression, with heterogeneous effect sizes that might be ascribable to differences in lockdowns, samples and study designs. Small mental health effects of lockdowns (0.22 ≤ g ≤ 0.38) were found for studies including participants from Great Britain [5,6], China [7,8,9], and the United States of America [10]. Highest effect sizes were reported for Poland (g = 0.64; [11]), one of many Eastern European countries where prevalence rates for anxiety (30%) and depression (27%) increased in the general population during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic [12]. However, the findings of Zhang et al. [12] indicate heterogeneity of lockdown-dependent mental health symptoms among different populations in Eastern Europe. The authors [12] assume that differences in levels of lockdown restrictions might have differentially affected individuals’ mental health and note a lack of studies on that topic for eleven Eastern European countries including Hungary and Romania. Therefore, they call for further research gaining deeper insights into the relations between country-specific differences in COVID-19 restrictions and mental health issues. Furthermore, the role of mediating variables such as the perceived risk of a SARS-CoV-2 infection, or distress intolerance, remains unclear [4]. Initial studies show that the COVID-19 lockdowns are associated with health-related anxiety (e.g., [5]) and negative automatic thoughts (e.g., [6]) due to increased distress (intolerance) or perceived risk of getting infected. Gaining deeper insights into these relations is important since COVID-19-dependent health anxiety and negative automatic thoughts might be prerequisites for illness anxiety disorder, dysthymia, or depression [13,14,15].
During the lockdowns between March and May 2020, however, the initially high levels of anxiety and depressive symptoms decreased rapidly [1,2,3], indicating that protective psychological factors may help with adapting to the circumstances (cf. [4]). Prati and Mancini [4] call for studies investigating in more detail the interplay between different levels of lockdown measures, induced stressors, early mental health issues, and personal characteristics. Zhang et al. [12] mention that among other Eastern European countries, Hungary and Romania had not yet been subject to a single study on this topic up until today. In addition to studying differences in perceived risk of SARS-CoV-2 infection, distress intolerance, COVID-19 health anxiety, and negative automatic thoughts under different lockdown conditions, in this study we aimed to examine associations between these conditions and protective psychological factors.
To investigate such relations Salanti et al. [16] suggest merging individual data with the geographical and temporal characteristics of COVID-19 lockdowns provided in the Oxford Coronavirus Government Response Tracker (OxCGRT) dataset [17]. For our research the Stringency Index (SI) included in OxCGRT is most important. This daily measure contains nine governmental response indicators for each country, such as school and workplace closures, stay-at-home requirements or information campaigns (for details see [18]) and ranges from 0 (no restrictions) to 100 (maximum restrictions).

1.1. Strict vs. Mild Lockdown Measures in Romania vs. Hungary and Their Psychological Consequences

To investigate differences in lockdown measures, induced psychological distress, mental health symptoms, and protective factors, we followed the call from Zhang et al. [12] and studied the neighboring countries Hungary and Romania which differed in the stringency of their lockdown measures (cf. [19]).
After the state of emergency call on 11 March 2020 in Hungary [20], relatively mild personal restrictions followed until the end of May, whereas Romania (state of emergency call on 16 March 2020) introduced a strict nationwide lockdown on 24 March for the same period (cf. [21]). In Hungary small social events were permitted and people were encouraged to leave their homes only to purchase essential goods, to use medical services, for work or caregiving tasks. In Romania people were obliged to stay home and a signed statement was mandatory for people leaving their homes. The military supported the police in enforcing these restrictions (cf. [22]). The average SI between March and end of May 2020 indicates a significantly higher level of lockdown restrictions in Romania than in the European Union and Hungary (see Figure 1). It must be noted that Romania had more COVID-19 cases per million and a higher positive infection rate and reproduction rate than Hungary, whereas there were no differences in COVID-19 deaths per million and tests per thousand (details in Supplementary Materials).
To date the impact of the different lockdown measures in these countries on mental health issues is unknown [12]. Prior to the pandemic, Hungary’s prevalence rates for anxiety disorders, dysthymia, and depression in 2019 were higher than Romania’s for those under 70 and for women (IHME [23]). Following Prati and Mancini [4], even small effects of COVID-19 lockdown measures on anxiety and depressive symptoms might cause significant public mental health problems. Thus, insights into the relations between COVID-19 restrictions, induced stressors, mental health issues and personal characteristics are needed to develop mental health intervention programs (e.g., [24]).
Following Witte and Allen [25], we assume that one major psychological function of governmental restrictions is increasing people’s perception of risk of getting infected, so that they engage in preventive behaviors [26,27] such as social distancing or hand washing (e.g., [28]). In addition, lockdown-dependent perceived risk of infection might be a stressor [4] inducing depressive symptoms [29] and health anxiety [30]. Strict lockdown measures might also induce distress due to challenges such as working at home [4] or parenting [31]. Lockdown-dependent distress can exceed individuals’ distress tolerance threshold—their capacity to withstand aversive emotional distress [32]. During this pandemic, distress intolerance has been positively related to higher risks for anxiety and depression [33]. Fergus et al. [34] assume that distress intolerance is particularly associated with health anxiety.
High levels of health anxiety may lead to severe misinterpretations of bodily sensations. Regarding COVID-19, coughing for example may be interpreted as a sign of infection, in turn further increasing general anxiety [35]. Rosebrock and David [36] call for research addressing catastrophic thoughts about COVID-19 and negative automatic self-related thoughts as prerequisites for depression.
In respect of Hungary there is some evidence of higher levels of perceived risk of infection, emotional distress, and mental health symptoms during the first COVID-19 wave, between March and May 2020 [37,38], with women and younger people the most affected [39]. Comparable research in Romania is scarce. Ştefănuţ et al. [40] found Romanian females showing higher levels of depression, anxiety and distress. Mental health in Romania may have been affected more negatively than Hungary due to the stricter lockdown.

1.2. Protective Psychological Factors in Early Response to the COVID-19 Pandemic

To explore the possible role that psychological factors played in protecting individuals from distress and mental health symptoms during the first COVID-19 restrictions [4], we focused on three major adaptive traits (i.e., psychological flexibility, resilience and self-control) that help people dealing with environmental and social distress (e.g., cf. [41]) also during the COVID-19 pandemic.
First, psychological flexibility, the ability to adapt and to respond effectively to fluctuating situational demands by reconfiguring mental resources [42], predicted higher levels of well-being and lower COVID-19-related distress, anxiety [43,44] and problems due to social isolation [45].
Second, resilient coping, the ability to rebound from and adapt to stressors allows people to recover from or adjust to misfortune or change [46]. Resilience is associated with lower levels of depression, anxiety [47], and COVID-19-related distress [48].
Third, trait self-control—the capacity to override or change inner responses, to interrupt undesired behavioral tendencies, and to refrain from acting on them [49,50]—can play an important role during the COVID-19 pandemic. Trait self-control correlates positively with tolerance for COVID-19 generated distress [51] and with goal-directed behavior regardless of COVID-19 related uncertainties [52]. Self-control moderates the relationship between perceived severity of COVID-19 illness and mental health issues [53], is related to COVID-19 health-protective behavior and to supporting governmental regulations during lockdown [54].
Although it is assumed that all three protective psychological factors are stable traits, there is no study so far investigating whether different COVID-19 lockdown measures affected these traits. Different studies (as mentioned above) provide evidence that psychological flexibility, resilience and self-control helped people dealing with the circumstances during the first COVID-19 restrictions. However, up until today it is unknown which of these psychological factors had the strongest protective effect on mental health issues during the COVID-19 lockdowns.

1.3. The Present Research

First, we investigated whether stricter lockdown measures in the early COVID-19 pandemic in Romania induced higher levels of perceived risk of a SARS-CoV-2 infection, distress intolerance, COVID-19 health anxiety and negative automatic thoughts, compared to Hungary, assuming that a stricter lockdown has a stronger negative impact on these variables.
Second, we explored the relation of the different lockdown measures to protective psychological factors (psychological flexibility, resilient coping, and trait self-control), perceived risk of a SARS-CoV-2 infection, distress intolerance, COVID-19 health anxiety, and negative automatic thoughts. Particularly, we tested whether protective factors prevent negative effects on risk perception, distress intolerance, negative automatic thoughts, and COVID-19 health anxiety.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The sample comprised N = 1001 adults of the general population from Hungary and Romania with a university degree (see Table 1 for a detailed description). None of the participants had been diagnosed with COVID-19. The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Babeş-Bolyai University (RO). Informed consent was obtained from all participants. Data were collected online between 15 April and 31 May 2020 using Google Forms. The study was promoted on social media (e.g., Facebook) platforms using a Snowball sampling technique. Since states of emergency were called on 11 March in Hungary and 16 March in Romania, followed by a national lockdown in Romania on 24 March 2020, participants had been confronted with governmental restrictions for at least one month before participation.
To estimate lockdown-dependent treatment effects, we applied propensity score matching (PSM), controlling for possible confounds (e.g., socio-demographics, see below). The matched sample comprised N = 406 (nHungary = 203 and nRomania = 203) participants. We used G*Power 3.1.9.7 (Universität Düsseldorf: Psychologie-HHU, Düsseldorf, Germany) [55] for a sensitivity analysis of the paired t-tests applied, comparing outcomes between the matched samples. Given α = 0.05 and N = 406 participants, a targeted power of 0.95 would reveal small COVID-19 lockdown effects (dz = 0.179) as reported by Prati and Mancini [4] on anxiety and depression.

2.2. Instruments

English items were translated into Hungarian and Romanian by bilingual speakers. Cronbach’s alpha was acceptable to excellent for all used scales (0.82 ≤ αHungary ≤ 0.95; 0.76 ≤ αRomania ≤ 0.94). For all measures higher scores indicate a higher level of the variable.
Socio-demographics and COVID-19-Related Variables. All variables (e.g., “Have you already been tested for the coronavirus?”) were assessed using single-item measures (see Table 1).
COVID-19 Knowledge. COVID-19 knowledge was assessed using seven items (adapted from [56]) including three correct (e.g., “The incubation period of COVID-19 can be 14 days.”) and four incorrect (e.g., “Children are most vulnerable to COVID-19.”) true-false statements.
Official COVID-19 Dataset. We used official governmental data provided on a daily basis between 11 March (State of Emergency call in Hungary) and 31 May 2020 in the World in Data “Coronavirus Pandemic (COVID-19)” dataset [17] and in the OxCGRT dataset [18]. We calculated personalized (weighted) stringency indices (PSIW) for the stringency conditions between 11 March and the day of participation (mean stringency index values), weighted by the derivative of the natural logarithmic function. The weight implies that PSIW scores are higher, the earlier that persons participated in the present study, since the first weeks in lockdown had the strongest negative impact on mental health issues [2]. Details about the calculation of the PSIW can be found in the statistical procedure section.
Perceived Risk of SARS-CoV-2 Infection. Seven items measured risk perception based on Brug et al. [57], Commodari [58] and Ibuka et al. [59]. Two items each assessed perceived likelihood of infection with SARS-CoV-2 (e.g., “I am at higher risk than others of coronavirus (COVID-19) contamination.”) and susceptibility to disease, three items the perceived severity of the disease. All items were rated on a 5-point Likert scales anchored at 1 ‘not agree at all’ and 5 ‘totally agree’.
Distress Intolerance. Seven items of the frustration discomfort [60] subscale ‘emotional intolerance’ measured intolerance of emotional distress (e.g., “I can’t stand situations where I might feel upset.”) on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 ‘absent’ to 5 ‘very strong’.
COVID-19 Health Anxiety. The six-item brief version of the Health Anxiety Inventory [61] was adapted to assess COVID-19 health anxiety, including questions concerning psychological thoughts and reactions to bodily sensations (e.g., “If I hear about the new Coronavirus (COVID-19) I never/sometimes/often/always think I have it myself.”). Every item comprised four statements and was scored according to the choice of statements between one and four indicating level of health anxiety.
Negative Automatic Thoughts. The Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire [62] contains 15 statements describing negative-dysfunctional self-related automatic thoughts (e.g., “I don’t think I can go on.”). For each statement the frequencies of these thoughts were rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 “not at all” to 5 “all the time”.
Trait Self-Control. Trait self-control was measured with the 13 items (e.g., “I am good at resisting temptations.”) of the Brief Self-Control Scale [49,50] that were rated on a 5-point Likert scale, anchored at 1 ‘not at all like me’ and 5 ‘very much like me’.
Psychological Flexibility. Twenty items (e.g., “There are usually many possible ways to do things.”) from Ben-Itzhak et al. [63] were used to measure psychological flexibility. All items were rated on a 6-point Likert scale anchored at 1 ‘not agree at all’ and 6 ‘totally agree’.
Resilient Coping. The Brief Resilient Coping Scale is a 4-item measure (e.g., “I actively look for ways to replace the losses I encounter in life.”) assessing resilient coping [64]. Items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale, anchored at 1 ‘not at all like me’ and 5 ‘very much like me’.

2.3. Statistical Procedures

To examine the effects of different COVID-19 stringency conditions on our outcome variables, we combined the OxCGRT COVID-19 data [18] with the data in the present study. We calculated a weighted personalized stringency index score PSIW as follows:
P S I W   ( x ) = 1 j ( x ) ( 1 n j ( x ) i = 1 n j ( x ) S I [ y ( x ) ,   i ] )   ,  
where the stringency index score PSIW for person x is the mean value over the daily published OxCGRT stringency indices SI in the corresponding country y between 11 March 2020 (i = 1) and day j, when person x participated in the present study, multiplied by the weight W. W is f ( j ( x ) ) = 1 j ( x ) , representing the derivative of the natural logarithmic function. We decided to use 1 j ( x ) as a weight for the PSIW score, since Batterham et al. [1], Fancourt et al. [2] and Saunders et al. [3] reported higher levels of anxiety and depressive symptoms in the first weeks during the lockdowns in different countries, decreasing over the course of time. Fancourt et al. [2] suggest that individuals adapted to the circumstances after a few more weeks in lockdown. In line with these findings, we assumed that COVID-19-dependent personal restrictions had a more negative impact on individuals in the first weeks of the COVID-19 crisis (i.e., at the beginning of our data collection), decreasing over the course of time. The weight W implies that a person x’s PSIW score becomes higher, the earlier the person participates in the present study. Taken together, higher PSIW scores indicate on average higher levels of restrictions experienced by a person between March 11 and the day of participation, while taking the time course of the lockdown into account.
Furthermore, we applied PSM to investigate research question 1. PSM is commonly used in public health research to estimate causal treatment effects, adjusting for confounds in observational data when randomization is not possible (for an overview see [65]). In our study, we wanted to investigate marginal treatment effects [66], the propensity score being defined as the probability of treatment assignment (mild lockdown vs. strict lockdown) as predicted by all observed baseline covariates, shown in Table 1, where are also presented tests of the imbalance in baseline characteristics before PSM, using two-sample t-tests for continuous variables, Chi-squared tests for categorical variables, and the standardized mean differences (SMD, equivalent to Cohen’s d) of any covariate before and after PSM, indicating the standardized bias between both countries (cf. [67]). SMD should be reduced to a minimum, at least to |SMD| < 0.25 after PSM [68,69]. Paired t-tests were then applied to test differences in the outcomes after PSM [70]. We decided to include gender, age, residency, household size, number of children, employment, job status, received SARS-CoV-2 test, infected acquaintances, deaths acquaintances, COVID-19 knowledge, bank loan, self-control, psychological flexibility and resilient coping as covariates for estimating unbiased propensity scores. The selection of these covariates was mainly empirically driven, since previous research found substantial relations between them and COVID-19-related mental health symptoms [1,2,3,43,48,53,56,71]. A detailed description of how PSM was implemented in the current study is presented in Supplementary Materials.
To investigate the second research question, we applied path modeling using Mplus 8.6 (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, USA) [72] with the full sample (before PSM). All variables were modeled as manifest variables, and relations between all variables were allowed to vary freely. Thus, a saturated model with df = 0 and no fit statistics resulted. Since we were interested in indirect effects, we used the maximum likelihood estimator with bootstrapping (20,000 iterations) and confidence intervals [73]. We tested direct, indirect and total effects. Correlations between variables at the same levels (predictors, mediators, outcomes) were allowed. We included age, gender, employment, COVID-19 knowledge, received SARS-CoV-2 test, infected acquaintances and country as covariates in the model, due to substantial correlations with the outcomes (see Table 2). There were no missing data.
(All our data are available here: https://figshare.com/s/441d5df99b9120c3b6a0 (accessed on 29 December 2021)

3. Results

3.1. Governmental Restrictions and the Early COVID-19 Situation in Hungary and Romania

Referring to the World of Data COVID-19 dataset [17], the mean values of the SI for Hungary, Romania and the European Union differed significantly from zero: F(2, 243) = 21.250, p0.001, η2p = 0.146. As shown in Figure 1, the mean level of governmental restrictions was significantly higher for Romania than for Hungary (MDiff = 8.90, SE = 1.44, p0.001, 95%-CI [5.58, 12.36]) and for the European Union (MDiff = 6.84, SE = 1.44, p0.001, 95%-CI [3.45, 10.23]). This result indicates that, compared to the Hungarian government, the Romanian government responded more strictly to the COVID-19 outbreak.

3.1.1. Research Question 1: Differences between Hungary and Romania

Before-PSM significant differences and |SMD| > 0.25 between Hungarian and Romanian participants were found in gender, age, number of children, COVID-19 knowledge, COVID-19 cases and deaths among participants’ acquaintances, self-control, psychological flexibility and resilient coping, and in the propensity score (Table 1 on the right). After PSM, SMD was reduced to a minimum (0.00 ≤ SMD ≤ 0.16) and the overall balance test [74] indicates optimal balancing (χ2 (15) = 5.285, p = 0.989) between both groups’ baseline characteristics (Table 1, on the left). Thus, differences in the outcomes are not due to these confounding variables. Detailed results of the PSM are presented in Supplementary Materials.
The significant treatment check (Table 1, at the bottom) indicates that the Romanian governmental COVID-19 restrictions between 11 March 2020 and the day of participation were stricter compared to Hungary (dz = 1.061). The Romanian participants showed significantly higher levels of perceived risk of SARS-CoV-2 infection, distress intolerance and COVID-19 health anxiety than those from Hungary. No significant differences were found regarding negative automatic thoughts. Correlations between all COVID-19 related variables and the protective psychological factors are presented in Table 2.

3.1.2. Research Question 2: Lockdown Stringency Conditions and Protective Factors as Predictors of COVID-19 Related Outcome Variables

The results of our path model are presented in Figure 2 and Table 3. Perceived risk of infection showed a strong relation to COVID-19 health anxiety but no significant relation to negative automatic thoughts. The latter were most strongly predicted by distress intolerance, while a weak relation was found between distress intolerance and health anxiety. The relation between PSIW and health anxiety was stronger than between PSIW and automatic thoughts. PSIW directly predicted perceived risk of infection but not distress intolerance. Perceived risk of infection mediated the relations between PSIW and health anxiety. No significant direct effect between PSIW and any of the protective psychological factors was found.
For the protective factors, weak negative relations were found between resilient coping and distress intolerance, automatic thoughts, and health anxiety. The indirect effects of resilient coping via distress intolerance to automatic thoughts and health anxiety were also significant. Higher levels of psychological flexibility were weakly and negatively related to distress intolerance, health anxiety and automatic thoughts. Also, weak, significant indirect effects of psychological flexibility on negative thoughts and health anxiety were found via distress intolerance. Resilient coping and psychological flexibility were both not significantly related to risk perception.
Trait self-control turned out to be the most promising protective factor in the early COVID-19 pandemic in Hungary and Romania, evidenced by the strongest negative direct effects on automatic thoughts, perceived risk of infection, and distress intolerance. We found significant indirect effects of self-control on health anxiety via perceived risk of infection and on automatic thoughts and health anxiety via distress intolerance

4. Discussion

The present study investigated psychological effects of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic, considering different lockdown characteristics. Our first aim was to examine whether stricter lockdown measures in Romania led to higher levels of distress and mental health symptoms than did the milder restrictions in Hungary. Second, we investigated the relations between perceived risk of a SARS-CoV-2 infection, distress intolerance, health anxiety, negative automatic thoughts and the protective psychological factors psychological flexibility, resilient coping and trait self-control. To investigate whether these variables were differentially affected by contrasting lockdown measure stringency levels we followed Salanti et al. [16] in merging temporal and geographical information about lockdown restrictions with our individual data. The PSIW scores proved to be a promising measure for investigating lockdown-dependent effects. Due to the nature of lockdowns and social isolation during the pandemic, we used convenience samples, reducing the accurate representation of the respective populations in Hungary and Romania. Our sample comprised almost exclusively adult women, who were the most negatively affected by lockdown measures in the early stage of the COVID-19 pandemic [1,2,3]. Furthermore, all participants have an academic degree and >80% of them living in cities. It has to be noted that participants with severely compromised mental health also completed the survey. This was indicated by extreme values in the negative automatic thoughts questionnaire. Since we were interested in effects of different lockdown stringencies on mental health issues of the general population, participants scoring extremely high on negative automatic thoughts were excluded in all our analyses. Thus, to a certain degree all our results are representative for Romanian and Hungarian adult women with an academic degree, living in cities.
Investigating the first research question we applied PSM, controlling for personal characteristics and COVID-19 related variables as possible confounds. As hypothesized, Romanian participants living under stricter lockdown showed higher levels of risk perception, distress intolerance and COVID-19 health anxiety than did Hungarians. No significant differences were found in negative automatic thoughts. Considering that prior to COVID-19 both countries’ prevalence rates were higher for anxiety than for depression [23], lockdown-dependent effects on COVID-19 health anxiety (but not on negative automatic thoughts) might be due to a higher vulnerability to anxiety in both countries. Interestingly, prior to the pandemic prevalence rates for anxiety, dysthymia and depression were lower in Romania than Hungary, whereas we found higher levels of distress intolerance, perceived risk and health anxiety in Romania during the pandemic. Our results are in line with findings reported in a current meta-analysis [75], where stricter lockdowns were associated with higher levels of various negative emotional symptoms such as anxiety. Following the discussion by the authors, we also assume that varying levels of COVID-19-related mental health issues across countries depended on specific lockdown characteristics (e.g., length and stringency level). More precisely, the degrees of isolation, reduced social contact and general restrictions in daily life were directly caused by corresponding stringency levels of lockdown restrictions.
According to self-determination theory, satisfaction of the basic psychological needs for autonomy, social relatedness and competence is a prerequisite of well-being [76] and mental health [77]. This implies that lacking satisfaction of the basic psychological needs could be an explanation for increasing mental health issues. In stricter lockdowns, higher levels of isolation might have increased feelings of being controlled by the government, undermining the satisfaction of autonomy needs. Furthermore, we assume that reduced social contacts in stricter lockdowns led to less need satisfaction of social relatedness and reduced the possibility of gaining feedback concerning one’s own competence. Thus, lacking satisfaction of the basic psychological needs in stricter lockdowns might explain higher levels of individuals’ distress intolerance, health anxiety and other negative emotional symptoms. Since we can only speculate, further research is required investigating effects of lockdown restrictions on basic psychological need satisfaction and mental health.
Although differences in perceived risk, distress intolerance and health anxiety were small, in the long run this might result in significant public health problems (cf. [4]). Our results raise the question whether stricter lockdowns might impact people’s rational decision making and preventive behaviors. As discussed by Asmundson and Taylor [35,78] high levels of perceived risk of infection and health anxiety might lead to avoiding medical assistance, due to the risk of contagion at medical facilities. Alternatively, they might visit multiple facilities to ensure their health. Thus, rigorous controls on compliance with lockdown rules in Romania may have fostered inadequate behaviors in people with COVID-19 symptoms. Mild lockdown measures in Hungary, however, may not have led people to follow adequate preventive behaviors [27]. The different outcomes in the two countries highlight the need for well-balanced governmental lockdown strategies and effective risk communication in the context of pandemic outbreaks.
Applying path modeling to the second research question provided support for the assumptions of Witte and Allen [25]; higher levels of lockdown measures, indicated by PSIW, were related to higher levels of perceived risk of SARS-CoV-2 infection. Both variables had detrimental effects on health anxiety. As discussed above, adequate policy responses to future pandemic outbreaks should consider relations between personal restrictions, risk perception and health anxiety.
Negative automatic thoughts were most strongly related with distress intolerance, weakly with PSIW and not at all with risk perception. Stricter lockdown restrictions had no direct relation to people’s tolerance for emotional distress. This needs to be discussed in detail, since we found higher distress intolerance in Romanians than Hungarians in the PSM sample. One possible explanation may be the different statistical models used. In the path model the variance of distress intolerance was partly explainable by protective factors, which might have impeded the direct effect of PSIW on distress intolerance. Moreover, we assume that COVID-19 side effects such as worries about losing one’s job or negative attitudes toward the governmental restrictions might have negatively affected distress intolerance and mental health issues [4]. Further research may provide insights into these relations.
To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first providing empirical evidence that three key protective factors were not affected by lockdown measures. Previous research has shown psychological flexibility and resilient coping as preventive of negative automatic thoughts, health anxiety, and distress intolerance, but not risk perception [47,48,79,80].
The detrimental effects of lockdown measures were most strongly mitigated by trait self-control. Self-control was related to lower risk perception of infection. Inhibiting impulses and regulating one’s own behavior and thoughts might foster social distancing, reduce exposure to risky situations, and reduce COVID-19 health anxiety. Self-controlled individuals seem to have greater capacity for emotional tolerance, indicating that they are less prone to emotional distress, which in turn helps them regulate negative automatic thoughts and prevent health anxiety. In line with previous findings, self-controlled individuals seem to use adequate strategies to deal with COVID-19-related stressors [51], and perceived lower risk of infection may be due to higher levels of goal-orientation [52] and engagement in health-protective behavior.
Summarized, all protective factors were beneficial during the early COVID-19 pandemic. Thus, we suggest implementing public intervention programs to enhance people’s resilience, psychological flexibility, and self-control. For example, Joyce et al. [81] found that interventions combining cognitive behavioral therapy and mindfulness techniques are best to enhance resilience. Masuda and Tully [82] emphasize psychological flexibility in psychological interventions for anxiety, depressions, distress or somatization. Promoting psychological flexibility enables people to shift their focus from symptoms to underlying processes, increasing their mental health [83]. Self-control training [84] may also help. Yang et al. [85] found self-control training reduced depressive symptoms.
Against these new insights into relations between lockdown measures and psychological outcomes, we have to discuss some limitations. First, as already mentioned above our sample comprised non-COVID-19-diagnosed participants and almost exclusively adult women with an academic degree. Thus, our results may only be generalized to this group. Since various studies suggest that women in particular suffered from mental health issues during the early lockdown restrictions [1,2,3], our results may be valid for the most relevant population. Nevertheless, further studies are required to extend our research to a more demographically representative sample.
Second, PSM always neglects unobserved covariates [66]. Here, negative attitudes toward lockdown measures that may have negative effects on mental health might have been of particular interest [4]. However, during the early COVID-19 pandemic public approval of lockdown measures was high across countries [86]. Moreover, differences in cultures and economies may affect the results. Nevertheless, considering lower pre-pandemic prevalence rates for anxiety and depression in Romania than in Hungary [23], Romania’s higher levels of risk perception, health anxiety, and distress intolerance may have been affected by stricter lockdown measures.
Third, missing information about participants’ previous mental health diagnoses prevents ruling out whether the country differences are solely a function of earlier mental health issues. Nor do we know whether mental health symptoms increased during the lockdown compared to the baseline before COVID-19. Future research drawing on longitudinal studies can bring more insights into the effects of different lockdown strategies. We have tried to compensate for these weaknesses by controlling for confounds in all of our analyses, to provide initial results on relations between lockdown measures, psychological outcomes and protectors.

5. Conclusions

To the best of our knowledge, this is one of the first studies merging objective stringency index data with individual data to investigate effects of different lockdown measures on stressors, mental health symptoms, and protective psychological factors in the early COVID-19 pandemic. In their systematic review Zhang et al. [12] mentioned that Romania and Hungary had not been subject to a single study on that topic. Therefore, we compared these two neighboring countries with very different lockdown measures. Whereas Romania had one of the strictest lockdowns in the European Union, Hungary’s government was much less so. We found that stricter lockdown measures increased people’s perceived risk of SARS-CoV-2 infection, distress intolerance and health anxiety, while the protective psychological factors of psychological flexibility, resilient coping and self-control were not affected during the first lockdown in 2020. Furthermore, our study gained deeper insights into the relations between restriction levels, protective factors, stressors, health anxiety and negative automatic thoughts.
Policy decisions about the severity of lockdowns in pandemics should take two major issues into account. First, strict lockdowns might increase perceived risk of infection and health anxiety in the population. This might affect people’s behavior respective to medical support. On the one hand, medical facilities may be visited too little. Being infected with SARS-CoV-2 but not going to the doctor increases the risk of infecting others. On the other hand, multiple doctors might be consulted, increasing strain on health care resources. Second, mild lockdowns may lead to the population’s perception of risk being low, resulting in inadequate COVID-19 health-protective behavior. Thus, policy decisions about the stringency of lockdown measures need to consider the tradeoff between infections, deaths and healthcare capacities but also inductions on the population’s experiences and behavior. Finally, our results suggest the need to implement intervention programs to mitigate negative effects of COVID-19 lockdowns on mental health, and to strengthen protective psychological factors.
Considering specific COVID-19 pandemic characteristics and the stringency of containment policies to investigate lockdown measure effects on changes in mental health (see also [16]) is a viable option for future research. This approach might especially be useful for gaining deeper insights into the effects of different lockdown lengths and stringencies on the increased rates of unemployment in various countries over the course of the COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., [86,87]). It has to be noted that unemployment is positively related to poor (mental) health (e.g., [88,89]). Therefore, stricter and longer lockdowns might have directly affected both, mental health symptoms due to isolation, and unemployment due to business closure [87]. In addition, different lockdown characteristics could have indirectly caused detrimental effects on mental health. This would be the case if stricter lockdowns lead to a higher rate of business closure in the first place and as a consequence, to increasing prevalence rates for mental diseases due to unemployment.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph19095098/s1, Table S1: COVID-19 Characteristics in Hungary and Romania between 11 March and 31 May 2020; Table S2: Sample Sizes of the Discarded, Unmatched and Matched Participants in the Treatment and Control Conditions; Figure S1: Distributions, densities and balancing of the propensity scores before and after matching. Refs [17,65,67,68,69,70,90,91,92,93] are cited in the Supplementary file.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation and data collection were performed by I.K., E.E.M., K.S. and K.K.-J. Data analysis was performed by C.L. and J.R. The first draft of the manuscript was written by C.L. and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The publication of this article was partially supported by the Development Fund 2022 of the Babes-Bolyai University Cluj-Napoca, Romania.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Scientific Council of the Babeş-Bolyai University, 400084 Cluj-Napoca in Romania (protocol code 6075/23 April 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

All data are available: https://figshare.com/s/441d5df99b9120c3b6a0 (accessed on 29 December 2021).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Stephen McLaren for his editorial support during preparation of this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Batterham, P.J.; Calear, A.L.; McCallum, S.M.; Morse, A.R.; Banfield, M.; Farrer, L.M.; Gulliver, A.; Cherbuin, N.; Rodney Harris, R.M.; Shou, Y.; et al. Trajectories of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms during the COVID-19 Pandemic in a Representative Australian Adult Cohort. Med. J. Aust. 2021, 214, 462–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Fancourt, D.; Steptoe, A.; Bu, F. Trajectories of Anxiety and Depressive Symptoms during Enforced Isolation Due to COVID-19 in England: A Longitudinal Observational Study. Lancet Psychiatry 2021, 8, 141–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Saunders, R.; Buckman, J.E.J.; Fonagy, P.; Fancourt, D. Understanding Different Trajectories of Mental Health across the General Population during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Psychol. Med. 2021, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Prati, G.; Mancini, A.D. The Psychological Impact of COVID-19 Pandemic Lockdowns: A Review and Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies and Natural Experiments. Psychol. Med. 2021, 51, 201–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Daly, M.; Robinson, E. High-Risk Drinking in Midlife Before Versus During the COVID-19 Crisis: Longitudinal Evidence From the United Kingdom. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2021, 60, 294–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Jackson, S.E.; Garnett, C.; Shahab, L.; Oldham, M.; Brown, J. Association of the COVID-19 Lockdown with Smoking, Drinking and Attempts to Quit in England: An Analysis of 2019–2020 Data. Addiction 2021, 116, 1233–1244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Lei, L.; Huang, X.; Zhang, S.; Yang, J.; Yang, L.; Xu, M. Comparison of Prevalence and Associated Factors of Anxiety and Depression Among People Affected by versus People Unaffected by Quarantine During the COVID-19 Epidemic in Southwestern China. Med. Sci. Monit. 2020, 26, e924609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Li, H.Y.; Cao, H.; Leung, D.Y.P.; Mak, Y.W. The Psychological Impacts of a COVID-19 Outbreak on College Students in China: A Longitudinal Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Liu, N.; Zhang, F.; Wei, C.; Jia, Y.; Shang, Z.; Sun, L.; Wu, L.; Sun, Z.; Zhou, Y.; Wang, Y.; et al. Prevalence and Predictors of PTSS during COVID-19 Outbreak in China Hardest-Hit Areas: Gender Differences Matter. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 287, 112921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Zimmermann, M.; Bledsoe, C.; Papa, A. Initial Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on College Student Mental Health: A Longitudinal Examination of Risk and Protective Factors. Psychiatry Res. 2021, 305, 114254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bojanowska, A.; Kaczmarek, Ł.D.; Koscielniak, M.; Urbańska, B. Changes in Values and Well-Being amidst the COVID-19 Pandemic in Poland. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0255491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Zhang, S.X.; Miller, S.O.; Xu, W.; Yin, A.; Chen, B.Z.; Delios, A.; Dong, R.K.; Chen, R.Z.; McIntyre, R.S.; Wan, X.; et al. Meta-Analytic Evidence of Depression and Anxiety in Eastern Europe during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 2022, 13, 2000132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Beck, A.T. Thinking and Depression: I. Idiosyncratic Content and Cognitive Distortions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1963, 9, 324–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Chatterjee, S.S.; Barikar, C.M.; Mukherjee, A. Impact of COVID-19 Pandemic on Pre-Existing Mental Health Problems. Asian J. Psychiatr. 2020, 51, 102071. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Scarella, T.M.; Laferton, J.A.C.; Ahern, D.K.; Fallon Brian, A.; Barsky, A.; Brian, A.F.; Barsky, A. The Relationship of Hypochondriasis to Anxiety, Depressive, and Somatoform Disorders. Psychosomatics 2016, 57, 200–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Salanti, G.; Cipriani, A.; Furukawa, T.A.; Peter, N.; Tonia, T.; Papakonstantinou, T.; Holloway, A.; Leucht, S. An Efficient Way to Assess the Effect of COVID-19 on Mental Health in the General Population. Lancet Psychiatry 2021, 366, 19–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Ritchie, H.; Mathieu, E.; Rodés-Guirao, L.; Appel, C.; Giattino, C.; Ortiz-Ospina, E.; Hasell, J.; Macdonald, B.; Beltekian, D.; Roser, M. Coronavirus Pandemic (COVID-19). Available online: https://ourworldindata.org/coronavirus (accessed on 20 April 2021).
  18. Hale, T.; Angrist, N.; Goldszmidt, R.; Kira, B.; Petherick, A.; Phillips, T.; Webster, S.; Cameron-blake, E.; Hallas, L.; Majumdar, S.; et al. A Global Panel Database of Pandemic Policies (Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker). Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021, 5, 529–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Plümper, T.; Neumayer, E. Lockdown Policies and the Dynamics of the First Wave of the SARS-CoV-2 Pandemic in Europe. J. Eur. Public Policy 2022, 29, 321–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Binder, K.; Diaz Crego, M.; Eckert, G.; Kotanidis, S.; Manko, R.; Del Monte, M. States of Emergency in Response to the Coronavirus Crisis: Situation in Certain Member States. Available online: https://www.europarl.europa.eu/RegData/etudes/BRIE/2020/649408/EPRS_BRI(2020)649408_EN.pdf (accessed on 23 April 2021).
  21. Dascalu, S. The Successes and Failures of the Initial COVID-19 Pandemic Response in Romania. Front. Public Health 2020, 8, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Mantu, S. EU Citizenship, Free Movement, and COVID-19 in Romania. Front. Hum. Dyn. 2020, 2, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. IHME. The Institute for Health Metrics and Evaluation. Available online: https://vizhub.healthdata.org/gbd-compare (accessed on 23 April 2021).
  24. Gruber, J.; Prinstein, M.J.; Clark, L.A.; Rottenberg, J.; Abramowitz, J.S.; Albano, A.M.; Aldao, A.; Borelli, J.L.; Chung, T.; Davila, J.; et al. Mental Health and Clinical Psychological Science in the Time of COVID-19: Challenges, Opportunities, and a Call to Action. Am. Psychol. 2020, 76, 409–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Witte, K.; Allen, M. A Meta-Analysis of Fear Appeals: Implications for Effective Public Health Campaigns. Health Educ. Behav. 2000, 27, 591–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Rogers, R.W. A Protection Motivation Theory of Fear Appeals and Attitude Change1. J. Psychol. 1975, 91, 93–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Tagini, S.; Brugnera, A.; Ferrucci, R.; Mazzocco, K.; Compare, A.; Silani, V.; Pravettoni, G.; Poletti, B. It Won’t Happen to Me! Psychosocial Factors Influencing Risk Perception for Respiratory Infectious Diseases: A Scoping Review. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being 2021, 13, 835–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Alegria, K.E.; Fleszar-Pavlović, S.E.; Ngo, D.D.; Beam, A.; Halliday, D.M.; Hinojosa, B.M.; Hua, J.; Johnson, A.E.; McAnally, K.; McKinley, L.E.; et al. The Role of Risk Perceptions and Affective Consequences in COVID-19 Protective Behaviors. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2021, 28, 801–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Kim, A.W.; Nyengerai, T.; Mendenhall, E. Evaluating the Mental Health Impacts of the COVID-19 Pandemic: Perceived Risk of COVID-19 Infection and Childhood Trauma Predict Adult Depressive Symptoms in Urban South Africa. Psychol. Med. 2020, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Mohd Salleh Sahimi, H.; Azman, N.; Nik Jaafar, N.R.; Mohd Daud, T.I.; Baharudin, A.; Ismail, A.K.; Abdul Malek, A.Z.; Hassan, M.R.; Mohammed Nawi, A. Health Anxiety and Its Correlations with Self-Perceived Risk and Attitude on COVID-19 among Malaysian Healthcare Workers during the Pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 4879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Romero, E.; López-Romero, L.; Domínguez-álvarez, B.; Villar, P.; Gómez-Fraguela, J.A. Testing the Effects of COVID-19 Confinement in Spanish Children: The Role of Parents’ Distress, Emotional Problems and Specific Parenting. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Simons, J.S.; Gaher, R.M. The Distress Tolerance Scale: Development and Validation of a Self-Report Measure. Motiv. Emot. 2005, 29, 83–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Liu, C.H.; Zhang, E.; Wong, G.T.F.; Hyun, S.; Hahm, H. “Chris” Factors Associated with Depression, Anxiety, and PTSD Symptomatology during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Clinical Implications for U.S. Young Adult Mental Health. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 290, 113172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Fergus, T.A.; Bardeen, J.R.; Orcutt, H.K. Examining the Specific Facets of Distress Tolerance That Are Relevant to Health Anxiety. J. Cogn. Psychother. 2015, 29, 32–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Asmundson, G.J.G.; Taylor, S. How Health Anxiety Influences Responses to Viral Outbreaks like COVID-19: What All Decision-Makers, Health Authorities, and Health Care Professionals Need to Know. J. Anxiety Disord. 2020, 71, 102211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Rosebrock, L.; Cernis, E.; Lambe, S.; Waite, F.; Rek, S.; Petit, A.; Ehlers, A.; Clark, D.M.; Freeman, D. Catastrophic Cognitions about Coronavirus: The Oxford Psychological Investigation of Coronavirus Questionnaire [TOPIC-Q]. Psychol. Med. 2021, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Lábadi, B.; Arató, N.; Budai, T.; Inhóf, O.; Stecina, D.T.; Sík, A.; Zsidó, A.N. Psychological Well-Being and Coping Strategies of Elderly People during the COVID-19 Pandemic in Hungary. Aging Ment. Health 2022, 26, 570–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Marschalko, E.E.; Kotta, I.; Kalcza-Janosi, K.; Szabo, K.; Jancso-Farcas, S. Psychological Predictors of COVID-19 Prevention Behavior in Hungarian Women Across Different Generations. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Szabo, A.; Ábel, K.; Boros, S. Attitudes toward COVID-19 and Stress Levels in Hungary: Effects of Age, Perceived Health Status, and Gender. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 2020, 12, 572–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Ştefănuţ, A.M.; Vintilă, M.; Bucur, V.M.; Blaboli, T. Gender and Age-Related Differences in Depression, Anxiety and Stress during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Sectional Study. Available online: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=sih&AN=149558942&site=ehost-live (accessed on 18 April 2021).
  41. Tei, S.; Fujino, J. Roles of Trait Resilience, Flexibility, and Volitional Self-Control in Social Adaptation: An FMRI Study. Eur. Psychiatry 2021, 64, 734–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kashdan, T.B.; Rottenberg, J. Psychological Flexibility as a Fundamental Aspect of Health. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 30, 865–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Chong, Y.Y.; Chien, W.T.; Cheng, H.Y.; Kassianos, A.P.; Gloster, A.T.; Karekla, M. Can Psychological Flexibility and Prosociality Mitigate Illness Perceptions toward COVID-19 on Mental Health? A Cross-Sectional Study among Hong Kong Adults. Global. Health 2021, 17, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Dawson, D.L.; Golijani-Moghaddam, N. COVID-19: Psychological Flexibility, Coping, Mental Health, and Wellbeing in the UK during the Pandemic. J. Context. Behav. Sci. 2020, 17, 126–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Smith, B.M.; Twohy, A.J.; Smith, G.S. Psychological Inflexibility and Intolerance of Uncertainty Moderate the Relationship between Social Isolation and Mental Health Outcomes during COVID-19. J. Context. Behav. Sci. 2020, 18, 162–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Dyer, J.G.; McGuinness, T.M. Resilience: Analysis of the Concept. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 1996, 10, 276–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Song, S.; Yang, X.; Yang, H.; Zhou, P.; Ma, H.; Teng, C.; Chen, H.; Ou, H.; Li, J.; Mathews, C.A.; et al. Psychological Resilience as a Protective Factor for Depression and Anxiety Among the Public During the Outbreak of COVID-19. Front. Psychol. 2021, 11, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Osimo, S.A.; Aiello, M.; Gentili, C.; Ionta, S.; Cecchetto, C. The Influence of Personality, Resilience, and Alexithymia on Mental Health During COVID-19 Pandemic. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Tangney, J.P.; Baumeister, R.F.; Boone, A.L. High Self-Control Predicts Good Adjustment, Less Pathology, Better Grades, and Interpersonal Success. J. Pers. 2004, 72, 271–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Lindner, C.; Nagy, G.; Retelsdorf, J. The Dimensionality of the Brief Self-Control Scale—An Evaluation of Unidimensional and Multidimensional Applications. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2015, 86, 465–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Schnell, T.; Krampe, H. Meaning in Life and Self-Control Buffer Stress in Times of COVID-19: Moderating and Mediating Effects With Regard to Mental Distress. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Kokkoris, M.D.; Stavrova, O. Staying on Track in Turbulent Times: Trait Self-Control and Goal Pursuit during Self-Quarantine. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2021, 170, 110454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Li, J.-B.; Yang, A.; Dou, K.; Cheung, R.Y.M.M. Self-Control Moderates the Association Between Perceived Severity of Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) and Mental Health Problems Among the Chinese Public. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Kukowski, C.; Bernecker, K.; Brandstätter, V. Self-Control and Beliefs Surrounding Others’ Cooperation Predict Own Health-Protective Behaviors and Support for COVID-19 Government Regulations: Evidence from Two European Countries. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2021, 16, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.-G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A Flexible Statistical Power Analysis Program for the Social, Behavioral, and Biomedical Sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Cheng, C.; Ng, A.-K. Psychosocial Factors Predicting SARS-Preventive Behaviors in Four Major SARS-Affected Regions. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 36, 222–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Brug, J.; Aro, A.R.; Oenema, A.; de Zwart, O.; Richardus, J.H.; Bishop, G.D. SARS Risk Perception, Knowledge, Precautions, and Information Sources, the Netherlands. Emerg. Infect. Dis. 2004, 10, 1486–1489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Commodari, E. The Role of Sociodemographic and Psychological Variables on Risk Perception of the Flu. SAGE Open 2017, 7, 215824401771889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Ibuka, Y.; Chapman, G.B.; Meyers, L.A.; Li, M.; Galvani, A.P. The Dynamics of Risk Perceptions and Precautionary Behavior in Response to 2009 (H1N1) Pandemic Influenza. BMC Infect. Dis. 2010, 10, 296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Harrington, N. The Frustration Discomfort Scale: Development and Psychometric Properties. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2005, 12, 374–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Salkovskis, P.M.; Rimes, K.A.; Warwick, H.M.C.C.; Clark, D.M. The Health Anxiety Inventory: Development and Validation of Scales for the Measurement of Health Anxiety and Hypochondriasis. Psychol. Med. 2002, 32, 843–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Netemeyer, R.G.; Williamson, D.A.; Burton, S.; Biswas, D.; Jindal, S.; Landreth, S.; Mills, G.; Primeaux, S. Psychometric Properties of Shortened Versions of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2002, 62, 111–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Ben-Itzhak, S.; Bluvstein, I.; Maor, M. The Psychological Flexibility Questionnaire (PFQ): Development, Reliability and Validity. Webmed Cent. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Sinclair, V.G.; Wallston, K.A. The Development and Psychometric Evaluation of the Brief Resilient Coping Scale. Assessment 2004, 11, 94–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. He, H.; Wu, P.; Chen, D.-G. Statistical Causal Inferences and Their Applications in Public Health Research; He, H., Wu, P., Chen, D.-G., Eds.; ICSA Book Series in Statistics; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; ISBN 978-3-319-41257-3. [Google Scholar]
  66. Austin, P.C.; Mamdani, M.M.; Stukel, T.A.; Anderson, G.M.; Tu, J.V. The Use of the Propensity Score for Estimating Treatment Effects: Administrative versus Clinical Data. Stat. Med. 2005, 24, 1563–1578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Austin, P.C. An Introduction to Propensity Score Methods for Reducing the Effects of Confounding in Observational Studies. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2011, 46, 399–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Cochran, W.G.; Rubin, D.B. Controlling Bias in Observational Studies: A Review. Matched Sampl. Causal Eff. 2006, 35, 30–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Rubin, D.B. Using Propensity Scores to Help Design Observational Studies: Application to the Tobacco Litigation. Health Serv. Outcomes Res. Methodol. 2001, 2, 365–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Austin, P.C. Comparing Paired vs. Non-paired Statistical Methods of Analyses When Making Inferences about Absolute Risk Reductions in Propensity-score Matched Samples. Stat. Med. 2011, 30, 1292–1301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Silva, W.A.D.; de Brito, T.R.S.; Pereira, C.R. Anxiety Associated with COVID-19 and Concerns about Death: Impacts on Psychological Well-Being. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2021, 176, 110772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Muthén, L.; Muthén, B. Mplus User’s Guide (8th ed.). Available online: https://www.statmodel.com/download/usersguide/MplusUserGuideVer_8.pdf (accessed on 15 March 2021).
  73. Preacher, K.J.; Selig, J.P. Advantages of Monte Carlo Confidence Intervals for Indirect Effects. Commun. Methods Meas. 2012, 6, 77–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Hansen, B.B.; Bowers, J. Covariate Balance in Simple, Stratified and Clustered Comparative Studies. Stat. Sci. 2008, 23, 219–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Carvalho, P.O.; Hülsdünker, T.; Carson, F. The Impact of the COVID-19 Lockdown on European Students’ Negative Emotional Symptoms: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Ryan, R.M.; Deci, E.L. Self-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic Motivation, Social Development, and Well-Being. Am. Psychol. 2000, 55, 68–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Schutte, N.S.; Malouff, J.M. Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction, Affect and Mental Health. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 40, 1228–1233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Asmundson, G.J.G.; Taylor, S. Coronaphobia: Fear and the 2019-NCoV Outbreak. J. Anxiety Disord. 2020, 70, 102196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Ruiz, F.J.; Odriozola-González, P. El Papel de La Inflexibilidad Psicológica En El Modelo Cognitivo de La Depresión de Beck En Una Muestra de Universitarios. An. Psicol. 2016, 32, 441–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Ruiz, F.J.; Suárez-Falcón, J.C.; Riaño-Hernández, D. Validity Evidence of the Spanish Version of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire–8 in Colombia. Span. J. Psychol. 2017, 20, E12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Joyce, S.; Shand, F.; Tighe, J.; Laurent, S.J.; Bryant, R.A.; Harvey, S.B. Road to Resilience: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Resilience Training Programmes and Interventions. BMJ Open 2018, 8, e017858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  82. Masuda, A.; Tully, E.C. The Role of Mindfulness and Psychological Flexibility in Somatization, Depression, Anxiety, and General Psychological Distress in a Nonclinical College Sample. J. Evid. Based. Complementary Altern. Med. 2012, 17, 66–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Østergaard, T.; Lundgren, T.; Zettle, R.D.; Landrø, N.I.; Haaland, V.Ø. Psychological Flexibility in Depression Relapse Prevention: Processes of Change and Positive Mental Health in Group-Based ACT for Residual Symptoms. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Friese, M.; Frankenbach, J.; Job, V.; Loschelder, D.D. Does Self-Control Training Improve Self-Control? A Meta-Analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 12, 1077–1099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Yang, X.; Zhao, J.; Chen, Y.; Zu, S.; Zhao, J. Comprehensive Self-Control Training Benefits Depressed College Students: A Six-Month Randomized Controlled Intervention Trial. J. Affect. Disord. 2018, 226, 251–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. YouGov COVID-19: Level of Support for Actions Governments Could Take. Available online: https://yougov.co.uk/topics/international/articles-reports/2020/03/17/level-support-actions-governments-could-take (accessed on 12 April 2021).
  87. Su, C.W.; Dai, K.; Ullah, S.; Andlib, Z. COVID-19 Pandemic and Unemployment Dynamics in European Economies. Econ. Res. Istraz. 2021, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Blustein, D.L.; Duffy, R.; Ferreira, J.A.; Cohen-Scali, V.; Cinamon, R.G.; Allan, B.A. Unemployment in the Time of COVID-19: A Research Agenda. J. Vocat. Behav. 2020, 119, 103436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Böckerman, P.; Ilmakunnas, P. Unemployment and Self-Assessed Health: Evidence from Panel Data. Health Econ. 2009, 18, 161–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  90. Rosenbaum, P.R.; Rubin, D.B. The Central Role of the Propensity Score in Observational Studies for Causal Effects. Biometrika 1983, 70, 41–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Harder, V.S.; Stuart, E.A.; Anthony, J.C. Propensity Score Techniques and the Assessment of Measured Covariate Balance to Test Causal Associations in Psychological Research. Psychol. Methods 2010, 15, 234–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  92. Thoemmes, F.J.; Kim, E.S. A Systematic Review of Propensity Score Methods in the Social Sciences. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2011, 46, 90–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Thoemmes, F. Propensity Score Matching in SPSS. arXiv 2012, arXiv:1201.6385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Description of the governmental stringency of lockdown conditions during the first months of the COVID-19 pandemic in Hungary and Romania. The dots and triangles represent the distribution of each individual’s SI score (not the PSIW), referring to the day of participation in the present study, and show that all participants had experienced more or less country-specific governmental restrictions for at least one month before participation.
Figure 1. Description of the governmental stringency of lockdown conditions during the first months of the COVID-19 pandemic in Hungary and Romania. The dots and triangles represent the distribution of each individual’s SI score (not the PSIW), referring to the day of participation in the present study, and show that all participants had experienced more or less country-specific governmental restrictions for at least one month before participation.
Ijerph 19 05098 g001
Figure 2. Results of the path model investigating effects of personalized stringency conditions (PSIW) and protective psychological factors on COVID-19 related outcome variables: direct effects. Correlations between variables at the same levels (predictors, mediators, outcomes) were allowed, but are omitted in favor of clarity.
Figure 2. Results of the path model investigating effects of personalized stringency conditions (PSIW) and protective psychological factors on COVID-19 related outcome variables: direct effects. Correlations between variables at the same levels (predictors, mediators, outcomes) were allowed, but are omitted in favor of clarity.
Ijerph 19 05098 g002
Table 1. Sample characteristics, baseline measures and standardized mean differences before (right; N = 1001) and after (left; N = 406) propensity score matching, and treatment effects on COVID-19 dependent variables (bottom).
Table 1. Sample characteristics, baseline measures and standardized mean differences before (right; N = 1001) and after (left; N = 406) propensity score matching, and treatment effects on COVID-19 dependent variables (bottom).
Sample N = 406 after PSM Sample N = 1001 before PSM
CharacteristicsCategoryHungary
(n = 203)
Romania
(n = 203)
Hungary
(n = 761)
Romania
(n = 240)
Fn (%); M (SD)Fn (%); M (SD)SMD Fn (%); M (SD)Fn (%); M (SD)SMDp
GenderMale21 (10.3%)21 (10.3%)0.00 54 (7.1%)31 (12.9%)0.200.020
Female182 (89.7%)182 (89.7%)0.00 707 (92.9%)209 (87.1%)−0.20
Age [range] 41.41 (10.02)
(23–67)
40.44 (12.16)
(19–79)
−0.09 50.43 (11.01)
(23–81)
38.35 (12.46)
(19–79)
−1.03<0.001
ResidencyVillage
City
27 (13.3%)
176 (86.7%)
34 (16.7%)
169 (83.3%)
0.10
−0.10
151 (19.8%)
610 (80.2%)
39 (16.3%)
201 (83.8%)
−0.08
0.10
0.251
Household size2.78 (1.29)2.74 (1.10)−0.03 2.61 (1.29)2.71 (1.10)0.080.166
Nr. of children1.01 (1.09)0.92 (1.03)−0.08 1.60 (1.21)0.79 (1.00)−0.73<0.001
EducationUniversity degree203 (100%)203 (100%)0.00 761 (100%)240 (100%)0.001.000
OccupationUnemployed27 (13.3%)29 (14.3%)0.03 154 (20.2%)32 (13.3%)−0.180.102
Employee119 (58.6%)119 (58.6%)0.00 407 (53.5%)144 (60.0%)0.12
Manager50 (24.6%)44 (21.7%)−0.07 163 (21.4%)51 (21.3%)0.00
CEO7 (3.4%)11 (5.4%)0.10 37 (4.9%)13 (5.4%)0.02
FinanceBank loan57 (28.1%)62 (30.5%)0.05 236 (31.0%)66 (27.5%)−0.070.301
COVID−19COVID-Knowledge
(Max. = 7 points)
6.11 (0.87)6.24 (0.73)0.16 6.13 (0.83)6.27 (0.72)0.180.022
Already tested 2 (1.0%)6 (3.0%)0.14 31 (4.1%)6 (2.5%)−0.080.989
Diagnosed 0 (0.0%)0 (0.0%)0.00 0 (0.0%)0 (0.0%)0.001.000
Cases acquaintances41 (20.2%)43 (21.2%)0.02 99 (13.0%)58 (24.2%)0.29<0.001
Deaths acquaintances5 (2.5%)5 (2.5%)0.00 13 (1.7%)8 (3.3%)0.100.038
Protective
factors
Trait self-control3.50 (0.60)3.52 (0.60)0.03 3.72 (0.62)3.46 (0.62)−0.42<0.001
Psych. flexibility4.90 (0.63)4.88 (0.65)−0.03 4.96 (0.65)4.85 (0.66)−0.170.033
Resilient coping 3.83 (0.64)3.82 (0.74)−0.01 3.99 (0.66)3.76 (0.76)−0.32<0.001
Propensity score (logit)0.35 (0.19)0.36 (0.20)0.05 0.18 (0.16)0.42 (0.23)1.21<0.001
Hungary
(n = 203)
Romania
(n = 203)
M (SD)M (SD)tpCohen’s dz
Treatment checkPSIW1.32 (0.14)1.57 (0.18)−14.975<0.0011.061
tpCohen’s dz
Dependent
variables
Per. risk of infection3.03 (0.87)3.22 (0.88)−2.0520.0420.148
Distress intolerance2.41 (1.00)2.64 (0.99)−2.5640.0110.180
COVID
Health anxiety
1.74 (0.48)1.87 (0.44)−2.8100.0050.204
Neg. automatic thoughts1.79 (0.79)1.90 (0.80)−1.3570.1170.095
Note. SMD, standardized mean differences before and after PSM. PSIW, personalized stringency index score weighted.
Table 2. Bivariate correlations for study variables.
Table 2. Bivariate correlations for study variables.
VariableMSD01020304050607080910111213
(01)COVID-19 knowledge6.170.80
(02)Already tested (1 = yes)0.020.140.08
(03)Cases acquaintances (1 = yes)0.210.410.080.10
(04)Deaths acquaintances (1 = yes)0.030.160.03−0.020.19
(05)Perceived risk of infection3.130.880.220.110.140.02
(06)Distress intolerance2.521.000.030.080.020.040.15
(07)COVID-19 health anxiety1.810.470.180.080.100.030.580.29
(08)Negative automatic thoughts1.840.790.030.050.000.000.190.640.29
(09)Trait self-control3.510.60−0.080.000.070.08−0.15−0.37−0.14−0.42
(10)Psychological flexibility4.890.64−0.020.000.06−0.020.02−0.26−0.18−0.320.19
(11)Resilient coping3.830.700.010.020.01−0.070.03−0.25−0.16−0.380.340.60
(12)Pers. stringency index (PSIW)1.450.200.130.04−0.020.040.220.090.250.120.01−0.03−0.07
(13)Age40.9311.14−0.09−0.080.130.06−0.08−0.09−0.16−0.10−0.010.110.01−0.11
(14)Gender (1 = female)0.900.31−0.010.050.070.000.050.090.060.08−0.010.06−0.03−0.01−0.01
Note. Bold indicates p < 0.05.
Table 3. Direct, indirect, and total effects of the personalized stringency index (PSIW) and psychological protective factors on COVID-19-related outcomes.
Table 3. Direct, indirect, and total effects of the personalized stringency index (PSIW) and psychological protective factors on COVID-19-related outcomes.
OutcomeDirectIndirectTotal
via RCvia RC & PROIvia RC & DIvia TSCvia TSC & PROIvia TSC & DIvia PFvia PF & PROIvia PF &
DI
via PROIvia DI
Personalized stringency index weighted (PSIW) to outcome
RC−0.086−0.086
[−0.175, 0.002] [−0.175, 0.002]
TSC−0.036−0.036
[−0.121, 0.049] [−0.121, 0.049]
PF−0.012−0.012
[−0.093, 0.067] [−0.093, 0.067]
PROI0.240 ***−0.0060.0050.0010.239 ***
[0.169, 0.312][−0.022, 0.000] [−0.006, 0.018] [−0.003, 0.009] [0.167, 0.311]
DI−0.0240.0080.0140.001−0.001
[−0.096, 0.047][0.000, 0.024] [−0.019, 0.047] [−0.005, 0.010] [−0.088, 0.086]
NAT0.078 *0.0100.0000.0040.0060.0000.0070.0010.0000.0000.011−0.0120.106 *
[0.015, 0.142][0.001, 0.027][−0.002, 0.000][0.000, 0.012][−0.009, 0.023][0.000, 0.001][−0.010, 0.024][−0.004, 0.008][0.000, 0.001][−0.003, 0.005][0.000, 0.025][−0.048, 0.024][0.017, 0.192]
CHA0.125 ***0.009−0.0030.002−0.0020.0020.0030.0010.0000.0000.118 ***−0.0040.250 ***
[0.061, 0.189][0.001, 0.023][−0.011, 0.000][0.000, 0.005][−0.011, 0.002][−0.003, 0.009][−0.003, 0.009][−0.006, 0.010][−0.003, 0.009][−0.001, 0.002][0.082, 0.156][−0.018, 0.009][0.177, 0.323]
Resilient coping to outcome
PROI0.0740.074
[−0.010, 0.158] [−0.010, 0.158]
DI−0.096 **−0.096 **
[−0.171, −0.018] [−0.171, −0.018]
NAT−0.118 ***0.003−0.048 *−0.163 ***
[−0.180, −0.055] [0.000, 0.012][−0.088, −0.010][−0.238, −0.086]
CHA−0.101 **0.036−0.018 *−0.083
[−0.171, −0.032] [−0.005, 0.078][−0.035, −0.004][−0.172, 0.005]
Trait self-control to outcome
PROI−0.126 ***−0.126 ***
[−0.192, −0.061] [−0.192, −0.061]
DI−0.387 ***−0.387 ***
[−0.445, −0.326] [−0.445, −0.326]
NAT−0.179 ***−0.006−0.194 ***−0.379 ***
[−0.235, −0.123] [−0.014, 0.000][−0.232, −0.161][−0.437, −0.320]
CHA0.059−0.062 ***−0.071 ***−0.074 *
[−0.001, 0.116] [−0.096, −0.030][−0.098, −0.046][−0.141, −0.007]
Psychological flexibility to outcome
PROI−0.048−0.048
[−0.124, 0.029] [−0.124, 0.029]
DI−0.078 *−0.078 *
[−0.145, −0.011] [−0.145, −0.011]
NAT−0.066 *−0.002−0.039 *−0.107 **
[−0.122, −0.010] [−0.009, 0.001][−0.074, −0.006][−0.178, −0.039]
CHA−0.089 **−0.023−0.014 *−0.126 **
[−0.151, −0.026] [−0.060, 0.014][−0.029, −0.003][−0.201, −0.050]
Note. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05. RC, resilient coping; TSC, trait self-control; PF, psychological flexibility; PROI, perceived risk of infection; DI, distress intolerance; NAT, negative automatic thoughts; CHA, COVID-19 health anxiety.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lindner, C.; Kotta, I.; Marschalko, E.E.; Szabo, K.; Kalcza-Janosi, K.; Retelsdorf, J. Increased Risk Perception, Distress Intolerance and Health Anxiety in Stricter Lockdowns: Self-Control as a Key Protective Factor in Early Response to the COVID-19 Pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 5098. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19095098

AMA Style

Lindner C, Kotta I, Marschalko EE, Szabo K, Kalcza-Janosi K, Retelsdorf J. Increased Risk Perception, Distress Intolerance and Health Anxiety in Stricter Lockdowns: Self-Control as a Key Protective Factor in Early Response to the COVID-19 Pandemic. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(9):5098. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19095098

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lindner, Christoph, Ibolya Kotta, Eszter Eniko Marschalko, Kinga Szabo, Kinga Kalcza-Janosi, and Jan Retelsdorf. 2022. "Increased Risk Perception, Distress Intolerance and Health Anxiety in Stricter Lockdowns: Self-Control as a Key Protective Factor in Early Response to the COVID-19 Pandemic" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 9: 5098. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19095098

APA Style

Lindner, C., Kotta, I., Marschalko, E. E., Szabo, K., Kalcza-Janosi, K., & Retelsdorf, J. (2022). Increased Risk Perception, Distress Intolerance and Health Anxiety in Stricter Lockdowns: Self-Control as a Key Protective Factor in Early Response to the COVID-19 Pandemic. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(9), 5098. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19095098

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop