The Characteristics of the Reduction of Interference Effect during Dual-Task Cognitive-Motor Training Compared to a Single Task Cognitive and Motor Training in Elderly: A Randomized Controlled Trial
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
Randomization
2.2. Procedure
2.3. Research Apparatus
2.4. Measures
2.4.1. Training Indicators
Time of Orientation and Planning
Number of Errors
2.5. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Orientation and Planning Time
3.2. Number of Errors
3.3. Orientation and Planning Time and the Number of Errors in General
4. Discussion
4.1. The Planning Time Will Be Longer, and the Number of Errors Will Be Larger during the Dual-Task Training, Compared to the Single-Task Training
4.2. The Planning Time Will Be Shorter, and the Number of Errors Will Be Reduced after All Types of Training
4.3. General Discussion
4.4. Limitation of the Study
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Al-Yahya, E.; Dawes, H.; Smith, L.; Dennis, A.; Howells, K.; Cockburn, J. Cognitive motor interference while walking: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 35, 715–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rieck, J.R.; Rodrigue, K.M.; Boylan, M.A.; Kennedy, K.M. Age-related reduction of BOLD modulation to cognitive difficulty predicts poorer task accuracy and poorer fluid reasoning ability. NeuroImage 2017, 147, 262–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rajtar-Zembaty, A.; Rajtar-Zembaty, J.; Sałkowski, A.; Starowicz-Filip, A.; Skalska, A. Global cognitive functioning and physical mobility in older adults with and without mild cognitive impairment: Evidence and implications. Folia Med. Crac. 2019, 59, 75–88. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A. Exploring the Central Executive. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 1996, 49, 5–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruya, B.; Tang, Y.-Y. Is Attention Really Effort? Revisiting Daniel Kahneman’s Influential 1973 Book Attention and Effort. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tan, J.; Zhao, Y.; Wang, L.; Tian, X.; Cui, Y.; Yang, Q.; Pan, W.; Zhao, X.; Chen, A. The Competitive Influences of Perceptual Load and Working Memory Guidance on Selective Attention. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0129533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krauzlis, R.J.; Bollimunta, A.; Arcizet, F.; Wang, L. Attention as an effect not a cause. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014, 18, 457–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tombu, M.N.; Asplund, C.L.; Dux, P.E.; Godwin, D.; Martin, J.W.; Marois, R. A Unified attentional bottleneck in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 13426–13431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stroop, J.R. Interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Experimantal Psychol. 1935, 18, 643–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruthruff, E.; Van Selst, M.; Johnston, J.C.; Remington, R. How does practice reduce dual-task interference: Integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening? Psychol. Res. 2006, 70, 125–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmidt, J.R.; Besner, D. The Stroop effect: Why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2008, 34, 514–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strobach, T.; Schubert, T. No evidence for task automatization after dual-task training in younger and older adults. Psychol. Aging 2017, 32, 28–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davidson, D.J.; Zacks, R.T.; Williams, C.C. Stroop Interference, Practice, and Aging. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2003, 10, 85–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Burger, L.; Fay, S.; Angel, L.; Borella, E.; Noiret, N.; Plusquellec, P.; Taconnat, L. Benefit of Practice of the Stroop Test in Young and Older Adults: Pattern of Gain and Impact of Educational Level. Exp. Aging Res. 2019, 46, 52–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ludwig, C.; Borella, E.; Tettamanti, M.; de Ribaupierre, A. Adult age differences in the Color Stroop Test: A comparison between an Item-by-item and a Blocked version. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2010, 51, 135–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tiernan, B.N.; Mutter, S.A. The effects of age and uncertainty in the Stroop priming task. Psychol. Aging 2021, 36, 452–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Der Elst, W.; Van Boxtel, M.P.J.; Van Breukelen, G.; Jolles, J. The Stroop Color-Word Test: Influence of age, sex, and educational; and normative date for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment 2006, 13, 62–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Leone, C.; Feys, P.; Moumdjian, L.; D’Amico, E.; Zappia, M.; Patti, F. Cognitive-motor dual-task interference: A systematic review of neural correlates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 75, 348–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wu, T.; Liu, J.; Hallett, M.; Zheng, Z.; Chan, P. Cerebellum and integration of neural networks in dual-task processing. NeuroImage 2013, 65, 466–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kwon, G.; Lim, S.; Kim, M.-Y.; Kwon, H.; Lee, Y.-H.; Kim, K.; Lee, E.-J.; Suh, M. Individual differences in oscillatory brain activity in response to varying attentional demands during a word recall and oculomotor dual task. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Malcolm, B.R.; Foxe, J.J.; Butler, J.S.; De Sanctis, P. The aging brain shows less flexible reallocation of cognitive resources during dual-task walking: A mobile brain/body imaging (MoBI) study. NeuroImage 2015, 117, 230–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Emeester, D.; Eal-Yahya, E.; Edawes, H.; Emartin-Fagg, P.; Epiñon, C. Associations between prefrontal cortex activation and H-reflex modulation during dual task gait. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mirelman, A.; Maidan, I.; Bernad-Elazari, H.; Nieuwhof, F.; Reelick, M.; Giladi, N.; Hausdorff, J.M. Increased frontal brain activation during walking while dual tasking: An fNIRS study in healthy young adults. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2014, 11, 85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Folstein, M.F.; Folstein, S.E.; McHugh, P.R. “Mini-Mental State”. A Practical Method for Grading the Cognitive State of Patients for the Clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975, 12, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shapiro, S.S.; Wilk, M.B. An Analysis of Variance Test for Normality (Complete Samples). Biometrika 1965, 52, 591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunn, O.J. Multiple Comparisons among Means. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1961, 56, 52–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- King, B.M.; Minium, E.W. Statistical Reasoning in Psychology and Education; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Wilcoxon, F. Individual comparisons by ranking methods. Biometrics 1945, 1, 80–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spearman, C. The Proof and Measurement of Association between Two Things. Am. J. Psychol. 1905, 15, 72–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
CM M (SD) | CT M (SD) | MT M (SD) | C M (SD) | p-Value ANCOVA | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 71.7 (5.0) | 72.4 (5.4) | 70.1 (6.2) | 70.5 (4.1) | 0.413 |
Years of education | 15.8 (2.0) | 15.2 (2.9) | 15.1 (2.2) | 14.9 (2.9) | 0.983 |
MMSE | 29.0 (1.0) | 28.9 (1.0) | 29.4 (0.7) | 29.1 (1.0) | 0.975 |
Gender | F—14 M—5 | F—17 M—3 | F—13 M—6 | F—16 M—4 | 0.757 |
Level | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Orientation and planning time 1 | CM M (SD) | 7.19 (2.70) | 10.29 (2.43) | 12.37 (3.07) | 15.02 (3.52) | 16.99 (6.25) | 12.25 (3.77) | 12.89 (7.12) | 8.71 (10.99) |
CT M (SD) | 10.45 (2.93) | 14.13 (2.94) | 18.55 (5.29) | 22.82 (6.04) | 24.63 (10.36) | 19.79 (10.25) | 21.08 (9.46) | 6.27 (1.45) | |
MT M (SD) | 5.10 (2.33) | 3.64 (2.34) | 2.67 (0.85) | 2.45 (0.88) | 2.37 (1.11) | 2.29 (1.30) | 2.39 (1.47) | 2.02 (0.66) | |
U CM-CT (p < 0.05) | 74.0 (0.01) | 60.0 (0.01) | 61.0 (0.01) | 51.0 (0.001) | 97.0 (0.01) | 56.0 (0.01) | 56.0 (0.01) | 36.0 (0.24) | |
U CM-MT (p < 0.05) | 87.0 (0.01) | 14.0 (0.001) | 0.0 (0.001) | 0.0 (0.001) | 0.0 (0.001) | 0.0 (0.001) | 0.0 (0.001) | 3.0 (0.001) | |
U CT-MT (p < 0.05) | 33.0 (0.001) | 6.0 (0.001) | 0.0 (0.001) | 0.0 (0.001) | 0.0 (0.001) | 0.0 (0.001) | 5.0 (0.001) | 0.0 (0.001) | |
Number of errors | Group CM M (SD) | 0.96 (1.05) | 1.43 (1.43) | 1.75 (1.37) | 1.66 (1.40) | 1.87 (2.18) | 0.85 (0.56) | 1.17 (1.64) | 0.36 (0.97) |
Group CT M (SD) | 0.1.8 (0.20) | 0.31 (0.31) | 0.25 (0.37) | 0.16 (0.39) | 0.15 (0.23) | 0.1 (0.11) | 0.12 (0.13) | 0.1 (0.12) | |
Group MT M (SD) | 0.23 (0.14) | 0.29 (0.29) | 0.25 (0.37) | 0.16 (0.39) | 0.15 (0.23) | 0.1 (0.11) | 0.12 (0.13) | 0.1 (0.12) | |
U CM-CT (p < 0.05) | 54.5 0.001 | 53.0 0.001 | 77.0 0.001 | 57.5 0.001 | 41.0 0.001 | 25.0 0.001 | 47.5 0.001 | 24.0 0.05 | |
U CM-MT (p < 0.05) | 69.0 0.001 | 46.0 0.001 | 23.0 0.001 | 12.0 0.001 | 18.0 0.001 | 25.0 0.001 | 27.0 0.001 | 38.0 0.001 | |
U CT-MT (p < 0.05) | 131.0 0.05 | 179.0 0.49 | 84.0 0.001 | 105.0 0.001 | 139.0 0.17 | 137.5 0.31 | 86.5 0.02 | 52.0 0.25 |
Difficulty Levels Compared | 1–2 | 2–3 | 3–4 | 4–5 | 5–6 | 6–7 | 7–8 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Orientation and planning time | CM W (p) | 56.0 (0.01) | 103.0 (0.01) | 108.0 (0.01) | 160.0 (0.28) | 89.0 (0.01) | 142.0 (0.36) | 31.0 (0.01) |
CT W (p) | 79.0 (0.01) | 103.0 (0.01) | 108.0 (0.01) | 182.0 (0.41) | 101.0 (0.02) | 120.0 (0.29) | 1.0 (0.01) | |
MT W (p) | 82.0 (0.01) | 115.0 (0.07) | 127.0 (0.13) | 135.0 (0.20) | 138.0 (0.22) | 147.0 (0.32) | 133.0 (0.18) | |
Number of errors | CM W (p) | 138.0 (0.11) | 151.0 (0.19) | 170.0 (0.38) | 175.5 (0.45) | 107.0 (0.02) | 144.0 (0.39) | 72.0 (0.05) |
CT W (p) | 131.5 (0.03) | 135.5 (0.04) | 138.0 (0.05) | 152.0 (0.15) | 131.0 (0.17) | 76.0 (0.02) | 36.0 (0.1) | |
MT W (p) | 169.0 (0.49) | 130.0 (0.16) | 107.0 (0.05) | 136.5 (0.21) | 156.0 (0.43) | 153.0 (0.39) | 131.0 (0.16) |
Indicator | CM M (SD) | CT M (SD) | MT M (SD) | U (p < 0.05) CM-CT | U (p < 0.05) CM-MT | U (p < 0.05) CT-MT |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Orientation and planning time | 95.71 (6.14) | 137.72 (8.82) | 22.92 (1.79) | 15.0 (0.04) | 0.0 (0.01) | 0.0 (0.01) |
Number of errors | 1.26 (1.71) | 0.46 (1.71) | 0.17 (0.25) | 7.0 (0.01) | 0.0 (0.01) | 14.0 (0.03) |
Training Group | r Test | p < 0.05 |
---|---|---|
CM | 0.74 | 0.04 |
CT | 0.70 | 0.06 |
MT | 0.90 | 0.002 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wiśniowska, J.; Łojek, E.; Olejnik, A.; Chabuda, A. The Characteristics of the Reduction of Interference Effect during Dual-Task Cognitive-Motor Training Compared to a Single Task Cognitive and Motor Training in Elderly: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 1477. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20021477
Wiśniowska J, Łojek E, Olejnik A, Chabuda A. The Characteristics of the Reduction of Interference Effect during Dual-Task Cognitive-Motor Training Compared to a Single Task Cognitive and Motor Training in Elderly: A Randomized Controlled Trial. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(2):1477. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20021477
Chicago/Turabian StyleWiśniowska, Justyna, Emilia Łojek, Agnieszka Olejnik, and Anna Chabuda. 2023. "The Characteristics of the Reduction of Interference Effect during Dual-Task Cognitive-Motor Training Compared to a Single Task Cognitive and Motor Training in Elderly: A Randomized Controlled Trial" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 2: 1477. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20021477
APA StyleWiśniowska, J., Łojek, E., Olejnik, A., & Chabuda, A. (2023). The Characteristics of the Reduction of Interference Effect during Dual-Task Cognitive-Motor Training Compared to a Single Task Cognitive and Motor Training in Elderly: A Randomized Controlled Trial. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(2), 1477. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20021477