Next Article in Journal
Long- and Short-Term Trends in Outpatient Attendance by Speciality in Japan: A Joinpoint Regression Analysis in the Context of the COVID-19 Pandemic
Next Article in Special Issue
The Participatory Approach in Healthcare Establishments as a Specific French Organizational Model at Hospital Department Level to Prevent Burnout among Caregivers: What Are the Perceptions of Its Implementation and Its Potential Contributions by These Caregivers?
Previous Article in Journal
Comment on Marsigliante et al. Effects on Children’s Physical and Mental Well-Being of a Physical-Activity-Based School Intervention Program: A Randomized Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 1927
Previous Article in Special Issue
Workplace Bullying Seen from the Perspective of Bystanders: Effects on Engagement and Burnout, Mediating Role of Positive and Negative Affects
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Study of the Relationship between Sensory Processing Sensitivity and Burnout Syndrome among Speech and Language Therapists

by
Jimmy Bordarie
1,* and
Caroline Mourtialon
2
1
Qualité de vie et Santé Psychologique, University of Tours, 37041 Tours, France
2
APHP Hôpital Sainte-Périne, 75016 Paris, France
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20(23), 7132; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20237132
Submission received: 12 October 2023 / Revised: 10 November 2023 / Accepted: 28 November 2023 / Published: 1 December 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Occupational Health Psychology: From Burnout to Well-Being)

Abstract

:
Background: Burnout syndrome can arise due to either situational factors such as working conditions, or dispositional factors such as certain temperaments, like a high sensory processing sensitivity. We aim to address the relative absence of studies on speech-language therapists and seek to determine the role of high sensitivity for these healthcare workers in relation to burnout syndrome. Method: The sample consisted of 602 female speech-language pathologists who anonymously completed a questionnaire measuring burnout (ProQOL 5th edition) and sensory processing sensitivity (HSPS-FR). Results: The results revealed that 77.41% of the participants reported moderate or high burnout scores. Furthermore, the analyses revealed that highly sensitive participants are more vulnerable to burnout than others. Conclusion: This study highlights the negative impact of high sensory processing sensitivity on burnout. However, according to the kind of sensitivity, we discuss the way in which highly sensitive caregivers can master this sensitivity in order to use it as a strength in their professions and to spare themselves from suffering deleterious effects, such as compassion fatigue and/or burnout.

1. Introduction

Burnout (BO) syndrome is defined as a psychological reaction to prolonged occupational stress that is influenced by both the individual and the organisational context [1]. It has been widely studied since Maslach and Jackson [2,3] first theorised it. In France, according to prevalence studies, BO could affect up to 34% [4] of the French working population, with 13% suffering from high burnout. The most vulnerable groups appear to be women (44% suffer from psychological distress) and those under 29 years of age (59%). There is an extensive literature focus in particular on BO among healthcare professionals due to its multiple consequences such as job dissatisfaction, intention to change jobs or professions [5], increased anxiety, depression, or even addiction [6], or its negative effects on patient care [7]. Healthcare workers are a particularly vulnerable group, with a higher prevalence of the so-called “caregiver burnout syndrome” (CSBS) [8] than the general population. More specifically, self-employed nurses are the most affected by this syndrome (56.5%), along with speech therapists (48.6%), chiropodists (48.5%), orthoptists (39.8%), and physiotherapists (32%) [9].
This syndrome may be due to various organisational factors, such as poor interpersonal relations and a heavy workload [10] or its uneven distribution and unstable team dynamics [11,12]. Some demographic variables also seem to play a role, revealing higher levels of BO for individuals who are young, female, and with a lower level of education than others [13,14]. Finally, CSBS is characterised by certain specificities, notably due to the emotional demands of these professions and the suffering that carers can be subjected to [15], leading to a dehumanisation of the relationship with patients [16]. Some studies are interested in other factors that may explain BO, notably through the study of specific qualities or dispositional variables. Indeed, among the qualities expected of a career, certain characteristics such as empathy [17], involvement, awareness, and sensitivity [18] are thought to heighten the appearance of BO [16,19]. Some researchers are also interested in high sensory sensitivity as a factor of vulnerability [20,21].
High sensitivity to sensory processing can be defined as a stable temperamental trait [22] affecting about 30% of the population [23]. This trait allows individuals to adapt more easily to changing environmental conditions [24]. A highly sensitive individual is characterised by a higher level of sensory processing than others [25] and has stronger responses to environmental stimuli. Some publications confirm the adaptive function of sensory processing sensitivity (SPS) to environmental stressors [26,27]. Some studies have found positive effects such as better interpersonal skills in highly sensitive individuals [28,29,30]. However, other research attests to the vulnerability induced by high sensitivity, leading to higher levels of anxiety and/or depression (e.g., [31,32,33,34]), perceived stress (e.g., [35,36]), and lower health scores [37].
The literature has confirmed the existence of several components in SPS and their role in the expression of symptoms [30,38,39,40,41,42,43,44]. While the literature often refers to a three-factor model [38], there is some research evidence of four components [39,40]. The first three components include the Ease of Excitation (EOE) and the Low Sensory Threshold (LST), which refer to stimuli that can be experienced as negative. These two components generally tend to increase the level of distress related to general anxiety and depression [30,41]. Their roles may differ with respect to depressive symptomatology and studies reach different conclusions, associating it with either EOE [41] or LST [42]. Aesthetic Sensitivity (AES) refers to one’s deep appreciation of, and connection with, the arts and seems to be a resource allowing, for example, some highly sensitive people to express a greater sense of well-being [43]. It may even play a protective role against depression, to which this component is sometimes negatively correlated [44]. But it may also be a vulnerability factor concerning anxiety in general [30,38,41,42]. Finally, the fourth component, Controlled Harm Avoidance (CHA), is related to the tendency of some highly sensitive individuals to seek to implement certain coping strategies, by being more conscientious, for example [39,40]. However, questions remain about this component, and in particular about its true nature. Described as such, it appears to be a direct consequence of high SPS, rather than one of the contributing factors. In this sense, it could be viewed more as a coping strategy, leading individuals to seek solutions or alternatives to better manage their sensitivity.
In the context of health, studies have found that higher levels of BO and compassion fatigue are correlated to highly sensitive scores for health care workers. Meyerson et al. [20] showed that highly sensitive dentists reported higher levels of BO and lower levels of quality of life than others. Their BO rates were positively associated with EOE and LST and negatively associated with AES. Perez-Chacon et al. [21] pointed out that EOE positively influenced the “emotional exhaustion” component of the BO, while it negatively influenced the “personal fulfilment” component. The latter was positively influenced by the LST. Compassion satisfaction was positively influenced by the EOE and the AES. Specifically, regarding caring and helping professions, compassion satisfaction is described as a protection against BO [14,45] as it reduces its negative effects [21,46]. Furthermore, the literature review [14] suggests that socialisation at work promotes the development of compassionate satisfaction. In this respect, strong social networks would notably reduce the effect of BO [47,48].
While most research on burnout amongst health professions has focused on doctors and nurses [49], there has been little research on other health professions, such as speech and language therapists (SLTs). Brito-Marcelino’s literature review [50] found only six articles on burnout among SLTs, three of which were published before 2000. Yet, like rehabilitation professionals, occupational therapists or physiotherapists, speech-language therapists are also at high risk of BO [51]. Recently, the study by Kasbi et al. [52] showed that 55.5% of Iranian SLTs were reported to have moderate to high BO, mainly due to emotional exhaustion and depersonalisation.
As no study seems to have addressed this topic with the French SLT population, one of the main objectives is to investigate the rate of burnout according to their degree of sensory processing sensitivity (SPS). In line with the literature, we pursue three objectives:
  • To compare the proportion of SLTs expressing high BO with the general population’s rate.
  • To investigate the relation between SLTs’ sociodemographic characteristics and BO scores.
  • To determine the prevalence of sensory processing sensitivity among SLTs and its association with burnout scores.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

Our sample consisted of 602 French-speaking SLTs, all women. Among the respondents, 82.39% (n = 496) are from France and 17.61% (n = 106) from Belgium. Sociodemographic characteristics are provided Table 1. The sample is not representative. In France for example, in 2019, there were 25,607 speech and language therapists: 96.8% were female, 81.12% were self-employed or mixed, 7.33% were hospital-based, and 11.2% were other salaried).

2.2. Procedure

The questionnaire was created as a Google Form and distributed via social networks, several SLT Facebook pages, and a SLT welfare page. It was also relayed through private SLT networks and by word of mouth. Participants were invited to respond online and were informed that their responses would be anonymous and confidential. Prior to completing the questionnaire, participants were informed of the objectives of the study and were explicitly asked for their consent to continue the study. To access the questionnaire, participants had to click on “accept and continue” after having read the consent form and given consent to participate. Responses to the Google Form were opened between 20 December 2021 and 28 February 2022. The estimated completion time was approximately 10–15 min.

2.3. Measures

The questionnaire consisted of 37 items, excluding the sociodemographic questions.
Burnout (BO) was measured with ProQOL 5th version [53]. In our sample, the internal consistency of burnout (BO) was satisfactory (α = 0.801; 10 items). The response modalities were given from 1 (never) to 5 (very often) on a Likert scale. Items 1, 4, 15, 17 and 29 from the BO subscale were reversed. According to the ProQOL manual, a score of 22 or less corresponds to a low level of BO. A score between 23 and 41 corresponds to a moderate level, and a score of 42 and above corresponds to a high level.
Sensory processing sensitivity was measured using the HSPS-FR [39], which is a self-report questionnaire. The scale consists of 27 items. Internal consistency of the overall scale was excellent (α = 0.918). That of the three components was satisfactory: ease of excitation (EOE) (α = 0.836), low sensory threshold (LST) (α = 0.850), and aesthetic sensitivity (AES) (α = 0.737). That of controlled harm avoidance (CHA) was quite low (α = 0.54), but it does correspond with the literature [39,40]. The response modalities were given from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) on a Likert scale. An overall high sensitivity score can be calculated by adding up all of the items. The higher the score, the higher the sensitivity. Within a categorical perspective, Lionetti et al. [23] devised a way of considering three groups according to their degrees of sensitivity: “low sensitivity” (individual’s total scores strictly below 113), “medium sensitivity” (scores between 113 and 137), and “highly sensitive” (scores strictly above 137).
Socio-demographic questions were asked, such as gender (Female; Male; Non-binary; but due to the very low number of responses from male (9) and non-binary (3), we decided to discard these data), age (ages were presented in 10 year increments, starting at 20 years old), country of training (France; Belgium), the number of years of professional practice (strictly less than 5 years; between 5 and 10 years; between 11 and 20 years; strictly more than 20 years) and the practice setting (private practice; medical-social institution; hospital; mixed).

2.4. Analyses

Statistical analyses of the questionnaire responses were then carried out using JASP. Firstly, descriptive analyses were performed for BO and HSPS-FR and its subdimensions (EOE, LST, AES, CHA). As the data did not follow a normal distribution, non-parametric tests and Dunn’s post-hoc comparisons were used to measure the differences between the subgroups in our sample, according to socio-demographic characteristics. Then, linear regressions were performed to measure the predictive power and the effects of the variables upon each other.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Results

According to the low, medium, and high thresholds considered in the literature (Lionetti et al., 2019 [23]), 77.41% of the sample reported moderate or high scores of BO and 50.5% reported high scores of HSPS (Table 2).
Mean scores for BO revealed that all subgroups reported a moderate rate of BO and moderate scores of sensory processing sensitivity (except for the youngest and the ones working in medical-social structures whose scores are higher than 137, revealing a high sensory processing sensitivity) (Table 3).

3.2. Study of the Relationship between Socio-Demographic Characteristics, BO Scores, and SPS Scores

Median scores, inter-quartile ranges, Mann–Whitney and Kruskal–Wallis tests, and p values are given Table 3.
The results showed that there was a significant difference according to age category (p = 0.036) and number of years of practice (p = 0.027). Dunn’s post-hoc comparisons were carried out. In the first case, Dunn’s test showed that the difference was between the “20–29 years” group and the “30–39 years” group (p = 0.003). In the second case, Dunn’s test showed that the significant difference was between participants with “less than 5 years of practice” and those with “between 11 and 20 years of practice” (p = 0.003).
There were also significant differences with these two variables and SPS scores (p = 0.003 and p = 0.009 respectively). With regard to age, Dunn’s test showed the significant difference was between the oldest group (50 years old and more) and the two youngest groups (20–29 years (p < 0.001) and 30–39 years (p = 0.005)). These differences are also apparent for the number of years of practice between the ones with the longest service and all the other groups (Table 4). Dunn’s test also showed a difference between respondents who obtained their diploma in 3 years and those who obtained it in 5 years, with a lower BO score and a higher SPS score for those who obtained their diploma in 5 years.

3.3. Study of the Relationship between Sensory Processing Sensitivity and BO Scores

The Kruskall–Wallis test showed that SPS was significantly associated with BO scores (p < 0.001) (Table 5). Dunn’s post-hoc comparison test revealed that significant differences appeared between the three subgroups (hypo, medium, and highly sensitive) (Table 5).
Linear regression showed that a high sensory processing sensitivity leads to higher BO scores (F(4.597) = 27.363; p < 0.001). More specifically, BO was predicted by EOE (t = 5.178) and AES (t = −3.857) (p < 0.001) and LST (t = 2.877; p = 0.004) (Table 6, r2 = 0.155).

4. Discussion

This study looked at burnout and sensory processing sensitivity among French speech and language therapists, which is a poorly studied population [50]. We had several objectives: (1) to compare the proportion of SLTs expressing a high BO with the general population’s rate; (2) to investigate the relation between SLTs’ sociodemographic characteristics and BO scores and (3) to determine the prevalence of sensory processing sensitivity among SLTs and the association with burnout scores.
In line with our objectives, our results confirm the higher prevalence of BO among French and Belgian SLTs. Indeed, with over 77% of participants reporting moderate to high burnout, this profession seems to be particularly affected. This rate is even higher than the one (48.6%) reported by the CARPIMKO survey [9]. This could be the consequence of the COVID-19 crisis since the latter is known to have had strong and long-term effects on health and professional quality of life, especially for healthcare workers [54]. But it could equally be due to the burnout scale that we used, which was different from the Maslach Burnout Inventory used in the CARPIMKO survey. This is in line with the sparse results obtained for SLTs in other countries, such as in Italy [51] and in Iran [52] and other studies using MBI in which healthcare professionals obtained above-average scores for all dimensions of burnout (e.g., [55,56]). We thus confirmed the higher prevalence of burnout risk in this population in comparison with the prevalence in the French working population in general. Estimations indicate that around 7% of the 480,000 employees are suffering from work-related psychological distress, i.e., just over 30,000 people [57], and vary from 12% [58] to 34% of the French working population [4]. While these results confirm the vulnerability of speech and language therapists facing burnout, they also prompt questions about the reasons behind such scores. Some answers can already be found in the problematic working conditions of SLTs and healthcare workers in general [10,11,12].
The literature seems to confirm that gender has no influence on BO, e.g., [14,59]. However, differences sometimes appear in the BO forms, where males sometimes report higher depersonalisation [60] or “underchallenged” burnout [56] scores than females. Unfortunately, our study is based on an all-female sample. This is an obvious limitation, directly linked to the type of population studied, to which we must add the tools we used. Indeed, by using the ProQOL, and not the MBI, we were unable to identify the dimensions of the BO for which the women who responded obtained particularly high scores.
We found that the youngest SLTs had the lowest BO scores, although their scores still showed moderate BO. Moreover, the biggest difference related to age appears between respondents aged 20–29 years and those aged 30–39 years, and not with the oldest respondents. This difference is expressed in the sense of a lower BO score for the youngest respondents, which is contradictory to the literature, which states the youngest are more vulnerable to BO [13,14]. However, it should be noted that even though the 30–39-year-olds had higher BO scores, both the 20–29 and 30–39-years-old groups obtained scores revealing a moderate BO; this significant difference reveals a nuance in intensity, but not in categorical nature, of the BO prevalence. This difference between our own results and the literature could be explained by the fact that, since 2013, speech and language therapists have been taking their diploma in 5 years instead of 3. The new diploma incorporates elements relating to quality of working life and analysis of practice, whereas the old diploma was more technical. As a result, the 30–39 age group is mainly made up of speech and language therapists who earned the certificate in 3 years, unlike the youngest group, and may have fewer theoretical and methodological tools to deal with the difficulty of their professional practice. This could explain that they reported higher BO scores than the 20–29-years old group. The oldest SLTs who have also obtained their 3-year diploma could benefit, in line with the literature, from the experience that would enable them to cope better with difficult situations than the younger groups who have also obtained their 3-year diploma.
The proportion of SLTs reporting higher BO scores was also found to be significantly higher among those reporting a higher degree of sensitivity than others and who could be considered as highly sensitive [23]. However, we need to be very cautious about results based on categorical approaches, especially when categories were carried out on populations other than the studied one. Many limits are mentioned towards categorial approach, like the stigmatisation it could induce; but it also has some benefits like favouring “a methodologically differentiated treatment of mental disorders [even if HSPS is not a mental disorder], without prejudging a priori and generically their ontological status” [61]. The use of these categories in this study has only one purpose: to succeed in identifying—whether such a group exists—if the sensory processing sensitivity characteristic leads to more burnout specifically for this group. By confirming the association between SPS and BO scores, this study corroborates recent results obtained with other highly sensitive healthcare professionals [21]. Although the scores obtained by speech therapists mainly indicate a situation of moderate exhaustion rather than high BO, it is clear that the higher their sensitivity, the higher their burnout scores.
Looking specifically at the role and relationship between sensory processing sensitivity components and the different scores of the ProQOL scale, several elements must be underlined. The EOE and AES components played opposing roles in BO. In other words, the more an SLT feels overwhelmed by internal and external stimuli, and the greater awareness they have of the details of their environment, the greater the risk of developing a form of burnout. More specifically, the EOE component generates a higher level of emotional exhaustion (a component of burnout) [21]. Thus, sensory stimuli experienced more intensely (EOE) generate emotional exhaustion that could in turn lead to the expression of a form of BO. The effects of EOE have been widely documented, and they are a strong argument for considering this component as one of the vulnerability factors of high sensory processing sensitivity. Indeed, this component generally tends to increase the level of distress linked to general anxiety and depression [30,41]. Similarly, an increased awareness of details relating to the LST (low sensory threshold) component leads to greater fatigue in the most sensitive individuals, as their attention is more often on high alert. This component appears to play a vulnerabilising role, as does EOE.

5. Conclusions and Limitations

While high sensitivity due to the predominance of one or both of these components seems to have rather negative consequences for highly sensitive individuals, there is less consensus in the literature about the effects of the aesthetic component. It is sometimes presented as a protective factor [20], and sometimes as a risk factor [21]. Our results seem to be consistent with those of Meyerson and colleagues, who emphasised its protective role against BO. It could even play a protective role against depression, to which this component is sometimes negatively correlated [41].
From a perspective applied to the quality of professional life of speech language therapists, possible courses of action could be envisaged to reduce burnout among these heath workers, particularly those with a high level of sensory processing sensitivity. We propose three avenues for consideration in relation to the training courses that could be offered to them. The first relates to coping strategies likely to provide better protection against the effects of burnout and/or over-sensitivity to stimuli likely to be experienced negatively. The second is to familiarise them with the tools and methods associated with the practice of mindfulness, so that they are better able to stay in the present moment and become aware of the stimuli they are experiencing. Finally, the third relates to the development of aesthetic sensitivity, which appears to protect against burnout and depression, and could help to increase the feeling of well-being.
The study has a number of limitations, some of which have already been mentioned, such as the fact that the sample was made up entirely of women. There may also have been some selection bias. We collected our data partly via social networks, and in particular on a Facebook page dedicated to the quality of life of speech therapists and the prevention of burnout. In making this selection, we took the risk of recruiting speech therapists who felt particularly affected by this syndrome. This recruitment may also have been biased by the presentation of the questionnaire, indicating that it concerned hypersensitivity. Thus, only speech and language therapists with an interest in the subject may have been more likely to have responded. This could also explain the high scores obtained on the hypersensitivity scale. Speech and language therapists who responded to our questionnaire may also have known about hypersensitivity, or even already considered themselves to be hypersensitive. It is therefore possible that they filled in the scales in this way. In addition, media exposure of the concept has led to a certain valorisation of hypersensitivity. For some people, being hypersensitive now has a positive connotation, especially as the concept is regularly discussed in the media.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.B. and C.M.; methodology, J.B. and C.M.; validation, J.B. and C.M.; formal analysis, J.B.; investigation, J.B. and C.M.; data curation, C.M.; writing—original draft preparation, J.B.; writing—review and editing, C.M.; supervision, J.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived for this study since none of the questionnaire used could represent any risk for the participants who could stop their participation at any moment. They were invited to respond online and were informed that their responses would be anonymous and confidential. Prior to completing the questionnaire, participants were informed of the objectives of the study and were explicitly asked for their consent to continue the study. To access the questionnaire, participants had to click on “accept and continue” after having read the consent form and given consent to participate.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

All the data used for this research are available upon request to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all the participants who answered the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Schaufeli, W.B.; Maslach, C.; Marek, T. (Eds.) Professional Burnout: Recent Developments in Theory and Research; Series in Applied Psychology: Social Issues and Questions; Taylor & Francis: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  2. Maslach, C.; Jackson, S.E. The measurement of experienced burnout. J. Organ. Behav. 1981, 2, 99–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Maslach, C.; Jackson, S.; Leiter, M. The Maslach Burnout Inventory Manual, 3rd ed.; Taylor & Francis: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  4. Empreinte Humaine. Enquête Opinion Way sur la Santé Psychologique des Salariés Français Entre Mai et Octobre 2021; Empreinte Humaine: Paris, France, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  5. Brunetto, Y.; Xerri, M.; Shriberg, A.; Farr-Wharton, R.; Shacklock, K.; Newman, S.; Dienger, J. The impact of workplace relationships on engagement, well-being, commitment and turnover for nurses in Australia and the USA. J. Adv. Nurs. 2013, 69, 2786–2799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Dzau, V.J.; Kirch, D.; Nasca, T. Preventing a Parallel Pandemic—A National Strategy to Protect Clinicians’ Well-Being. N. Engl. J. Med. 2020, 383, 513–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Romani, M.; Ashkar, K. Burnout among physicians. Libyan J. Med. 2014, 9, 23556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Canouï, P. La souffrance des soignants: Un risque humain, des enjeux éthiques. InfoKara 2003, 18, 101–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. CARPIMKO Survey. Etude sur la Pénibilité de L’exercice Professionnel des Praticiens de Santé Affiliés à la CARPIMKO. 2020. Available online: https://www.caducee.net/upld/2021/03/etude-carpimko-penibilite.pdf (accessed on 11 October 2023).
  10. Schaufeli, W.B.; Janczur, B. Burnout among nurses: A Polish-Dutch comparison. J. Cross. Cult. Psychol. 1994, 25, 95–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kelly, L. Burnout, compassion fatigue, and secondary trauma in nurses: Recognizing the occupational phenomenon and personal consequences of caregiving. Crit. Care Nurs. Q. 2020, 43, 73–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Das Merces, M.C.d.; Coelho, J.M.F.; Lua, I.; Silva, D.d.S.e.; Gomes, A.M.T.; Erdmann, A.L.; Oliveira, D.C.d.; Lago, S.B.; Santana, A.I.C.; da Silva, D.A.R.d.; et al. Prevalence and Factors Associated with Burnout Syndrome among Primary Health Care Nursing Professionals: A Cross-Sectional Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Ruiz-Fernández, M.D.; Ramos-Pichardo, J.D.; Ibáñez-Masero, O.; Cabrera-Troya, J.; Carmona-Rega, M.I.; Ortega-Galán, M. Compassion fatigue, burnout, compassion satisfaction and perceived stress in healthcare professionals during the COVID-19 health crisis in Spain. J. Clin. Nurs. 2020, 29, 4321–4330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Zhang, Y.-Y.; Zhang, C.; Han, X.-R.; Li, W.; Wang, Y.-L. Determinants of compassion satisfaction, compassion fatigue and burn out in nursing: A correlative meta-analysis. Medicine 2018, 97, e11086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kase, S.M.; Waldman, E.D.; Weintraub, A.S. A cross-sectional pilot study of compassion fatigue, burnout, and compassion satisfaction in pediatric palliative care providers in the United States. Palliat. Support. Care 2019, 17, 269–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Delbrouck, M. Je suis épuisé(e) par ma charge de travail. Que puis-je y faire? Le burn-out ou la souffrance des soignants. Imagin. Inconsc. 2010, 25, 157–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Vannotti, M. Lémpathie Dans la Relation Médecin–Patient. In Cahiers Critiques de Thérapie Familiale et de Pratiques de Réseau; 2002; Volume 2, pp. 213–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Touzé, P.A.; Steiner, D. L’adaptation française du PPRF, un outil identifiant les facteurs de personnalité susceptibles de prédire la performance au travail. L’Orientat. Sc. Prof. 2002, 31, 443–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Barbier, D. Le syndrome d’épuisement professionnel du soignant. Presse Médicale 2004, 33, 394–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Meyerson, J.; Gelkopf, M.; Eli, I.; Uziel, N. Burnout and professional quality of life among Israeli dentists: The role of sensory processing sensitivity. Int. Dent. J. 2020, 70, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Pérez-Chacón, M.; Chacón, A.; Borda-Mas, M.; Avargues-Navarro, M.L. Sensory Processing Sensitivity and Compassion Satisfaction as Risk/Protective Factors from Burnout and Compassion Fatigue in Healthcare and Education Professionals. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Dunn, W. The Sensations of Everyday Life: Empirical, Theoretical, and Pragmatic Considerations. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2001, 55, 608–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Lionetti, F.; Aron, A.; Aron, E.N.; Burns, G.L.; Jagiellowicz, J.; Pluess, M. Dandelions, tulips and orchids: Evidence for the existence of low-sensitive, medium-sensitive and high-sensitive individuals. Transl. Psychiatry 2018, 8, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Aron, A.; Aron, E.N. Self-expansion motivation and including other in the self. In Handbook of Personal Relationships: Theory, Research and Interventions; Duck, S., Ed.; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1997; pp. 251–270. [Google Scholar]
  25. Gere, D.R.; Capps, S.C.; Mitchell, D.W.; Grubbs, E. Sensory Sensitivities of Gifted Children. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2009, 63, 288–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Akiskal, H.S.; Akiskal, K.K. TEMPS: Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa, Paris and San Diego. J. Affect. Disord. 2005, 85, 1–2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Balestri, M.; Porcelli, S.; Souery, D.; Kasper, S.; Dikeos, D.; Ferentinos, P.; Janiri, L. Temperament and character influence on depression treatment outcome. J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 252, 464–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Acevedo, B.; Aron, E.; Pospos, S.; Jessen, D. The functional highly sensitive brain: A review of the brain circuits underlying sensory processing sensitivity and seemingly related disorders. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2018, 373, 20170161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Aron, E.N.; Aron, A.; Jagiellowicz, J. Sensory processing sensitivity: A review in the light of the evolution of biological responsivity. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2012, 16, 262–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Liss, M.; Mailloux, J.; Erchull, M.J. The relationships between sensory processing sensitivity, alexithymia, autism, depression, and anxiety. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2008, 45, 255–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Booth, C.; Standage, H.; Fox, E. Sensory-processing sensitivity moderates the association between childhood experiences and adult life satisfaction. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 87, 24–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Boterberg, S.; Warreyn, P. Making sense of it all: The impact of sensory processing sensitivity on daily functioning of children. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2016, 92, 80–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Meredith, P.J.; Bailey, K.J.; Strong, J.; Rappel, G. Adult attachment, sensory processing, and distress in healthy adults. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2016, 70, 7001250010p1–7001250010p8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Yano, K.; Oishi, K. The relationships among daily exercise, sensory-processing sensitivity, and depressive tendency in Japanese university students. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2018, 127, 49–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Andresen, M.; Goldmann, P.; Volodina, A. Do overwhelmed expatriates intend to leave? The effects of sensory processing sensitivity, stress, and social capital on expatriates’ turnover intention. Eur. Manag. Rev. 2018, 15, 315–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Bakker, K.; Moulding, R. Sensory-processing sensitivity, dispositional mindfulness and negative psychological symptoms. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 53, 341–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Benham, G. The highly sensitive person: Stress and physical symptom reports. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2006, 40, 1433–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Smolewska, K.A.; McCabe, S.B.; Woody, E.Z. A psychometric evaluation of the Highly Sensitive Person Scale: The components of sensory-processing sensitivity and their relation to the BIS/BAS and “Big Five”. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2006, 40, 1269–1279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Bordarie, J.; Aguerre, C.; Bolteau, L. Validation and study of psychometric properties of a French version of the Sensory-Processing Sensitivy scale (HSPS-FR). Eur. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 2022, 72, 100781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Meyer, B.; Ajchenbrenner, M.; Bowles, D.P. Sensory sensitivity, attachment experiences, and rejection responses among adults with borderline and avoidant features. J. Personal. Disord. 2005, 19, 641–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Ahadi, B.; Basharpoor, S. Relationship between sensory processing sensitivity, personality dimensions and mental health. J. Appl. Sci. 2010, 10, 570–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Konrad, S.; Herzberg, P.Y. Psychometric properties and validation of a german high sensitive person scale (HSPS-G). Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2017, 35, 364–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Sobocko, K.; Zelenski, J.M. Trait sensory-processing sensitivity and subjective well-being: Distinctive associations for different aspects of sensitivity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 83, 44–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Bordarie, J.; Aguerre, C.; Bolteau, L. A longitudinal approach of lockdown effects on quality of life and the expression of anxiety-depressive disorders according to sensory processing sensitivity. Psychol. Health Med. 2021, 27, 2288–2299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Rothenberg, M.; Fisher, K.; Elias, A.; Helton, S.; Williams, S.; Pena, S.; Gregory, A. Quality of life and compassion satisfaction/fatigue and burnout in child welfare workers: A study of the child welfare workers in community based care organizations in central Florida. Soc. Work Christ. 2008, 36, 36–54. [Google Scholar]
  46. Harr, C. Promoting workplace health by diminishing the negative impact of compassion fatigue and increasing compassion satisfaction. Soc. Work Christ. 2013, 40, 71–88. [Google Scholar]
  47. Rogers, E.; Polonijo, A.N.; Carpiano, R.M. Getting by with a little help from friends and colleagues: Testing how residents’ social support networks affect loneliness and burnout. Can. Fam. Physician 2016, 62, e677–e683. Available online: https://www.cfp.ca/content/62/11/e677.short (accessed on 11 October 2023). [PubMed]
  48. Shapiro, J.; Zhang, B.; Warm, E.J. Residency as a social network: Burnout, loneliness, and social network centrality. J. Grad. Med. Educ. 2015, 7, 617–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Redfearn, R.A.; van Ittersum, K.W.; Stenmark, C.K. The impact of sensory processing sensitivity on stress and burnout in nurses. Int. J. Stress Manag. 2020, 27, 370–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Brito-Marcelino, A.; Oliva-Costa, E.F.; Sarmento, S.C.P.; Carvalho, A.A. Burnout syndrome in speech-language pathologists and audiologists : A review. Rev. Bras. Med. Trab. 2020, 18, 217–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Bruschini, M.; Carli, A.; Burla, F. Burnout and work-related stress in Italian rehabilitation professionals: A comparison of physiotherapists, speech therapists and occupational therapists. Work 2018, 59, 121–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Kasbi, F.; Kaviani, S.; Mokhlessin, M.; Monshizadeh, L.; Noruzi, R.; Kia, N.S. Job Burnout Among Iranian Speech and Language Pathologists. Middle East J. Rehabil. Health 2018, 5, e64374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Stamm, B.H. Professional Quality of Life Measure: Compassion, Satisfaction, and Fatigue Version 5 (ProQOL). 2009. Available online: https://img1.wsimg.com/blobby/go/dfc1e1a0-a1db-4456-9391-18746725179b/downloads/French.pdf?ver=1693419364561 (accessed on 11 October 2023).
  54. Petrino, R.; Riesgo, L.G.-C.; Yilmaz, B. Burnout in emergency medicine professionals after 2 years of the COVID-19 pandemic: A threat to the healthcare system? Eur. J. Emerg. Med. 2022, 29, 279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Fiabane, E.; Giorgi, I.; Sguazzin, C.; Argentero, P. Work engagement and occupational stress in nurses and other healthcare workers: The role of organisational and personal factors. J. Clin. Nurs. 2013, 22, 2614–2624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Montero-Marín, J.; García-Campayo, J.; Fajó-Pascual, M.; Carrasco, J.M.; Gascón, S.; Gili, M.; Mayoral-Cleries, F. Sociodemographic and occupational risk factors associated with the development of different burnout types: The cross-sectional University of Zaragoza study. BMC Psychiatry 2011, 11, 1–13. Available online: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/11/49 (accessed on 11 October 2023). [CrossRef]
  57. Khireddine-Medouni, I.; Lemaître, A.; Homère, J.; Plaine, J.; Garras, L.; Riol, M.-C.; Valenty, M. Augmentation des taux de prévalence de la souffrance psychique en lien avec le travail chez les salariés actifs en France entre 2007 et 2012, à partir du programme MCP. Arch. Mal. Prof. L’environnement 2016, 77, 438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Technologia. Etude Clinique et Organisationnelle Permettant de Définir et de Quantifier ce Qu’on Appelle Communément Burn-Out. 2014. Available online: http://www.cftcaev.com/medias/files/burn-out-etude-clinique-et-organisationnelle-janvier-20141.pdf (accessed on 11 October 2023).
  59. Gandi, J.C.; Wai, P.S.; Karick, H.; Dagona, Z.K. The role of stress and level of burnout in job performance among nurses. Ment. Health Fam. Med. 2011, 8, 181. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  60. Brake, H.T.; Bloemendal, E.; Hoogstraten, J. Gender differences in burnout among Dutch dentists. Community Dent. Oral Epidemiol. 2003, 31, 321–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Demazeux, S. Les catégories psychiatriques sont-elles dépassées? Philonsorbonne 2008, 2, 67–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the sample according to respondents’ country of residence (%).
Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the sample according to respondents’ country of residence (%).
FranceBelgium
Age
20–2919.9635.85
30–3932.4641.51
40–4927.6216.98
50 and +19.965.66
Total100.00100.00
Duration of the formation before certification
3 years8.8775.47
4 years72.7810.38
5 years18.3514.15
Total100.00100.00
Number of years of professional practice
Less than 5 years18.5529.24
5 to 10 years 21.5726.42
11 to 20 years 30.2432.08
More than 20 years 29.6412.26
Total100.00100.00
Professional practice
Private practice 5.047.55
Medical-social structure81.6583.96
Hospital 3.231.89
Mixed 10.086.60
Total100.00100.00
Table 2. Descriptive results according to thresholds (low, medium, high) fixed for the scales.
Table 2. Descriptive results according to thresholds (low, medium, high) fixed for the scales.
Low ScoresMedium ScoresHigh Scores
BON136.00464.002.00
%22.5977.080.33
Mean19.0130.2744.00
SD2.704.631.41
Median20.0030.0044.00
IQR4.007.001.00
SPSN138.00160.00304.00
%22.9226.5850.50
Mean96.03125.97156.12
SD12.407.4112.71
Median98.00126.00155.00
IQR17.7513.0019.00
Table 3. Characteristics of the sample, descriptive data, with means, SD, median and IQR, Kruskall–Wall and Mann–Whitney tests for BO scores and SPS scores.
Table 3. Characteristics of the sample, descriptive data, with means, SD, median and IQR, Kruskall–Wall and Mann–Whitney tests for BO scores and SPS scores.
Variables BO SPS
N%MeanSDMedianIQR MeanSDMedianIQR
Gender
Female602100.0027.776.4228.009.00 134.3326.95138.0040.75
N%meanSDmedianIQRWpmeanSDmedianIQRWp
Country of training
Belgium10617.6128.916.3129.009.003.0020.083 135.8128.11138.0043.5027.2590.550
France49682.3927.536.4228.009.00134.0126.72138.0039.00
Rank-Biserial Correlation = 0.107Rank-Biserial Correlation = 0.037
SE Rank-Biserial Correlation = 0.062SE Rank-Biserial Correlation = 0.062
N%meanSDmedianIQRχ2 statpmeanSDmedianIQRχ2
stat
p
Age
20–2913722.7626.576.2726.009.008.570 0.036 138.6227.45140.0037.0012.917 0.005
30–3920534.0528.656.3829.009.00136.1926.28139.0038.00
40–4915525.7527.646.4729.009.00133.1227.04138.0042.00
50 and +10517.4427.846.4527.008.00126.8826.20129.0041.00
Duration of the formation before certification
3 years12420.6028.776.3528.509.005.870 0.053 129.3528.96131.0045.005.315 0.070
4 years37261.8027.746.4328.009.00135.1226.56139.0039.00
5 years10617.6026.736.3427.008.00137.3725.35138.0036.75
Number of years of professional practice
Less than 512320.4326.596.3126.009.009.161 0.027 136.8927.36138.0039.0011.642 0.009
5 to 1013522.4327.796.4528.009.00136.8025.88139.0035.50
11 to 2018430.5628.676.3829.009.00136.2826.90139.0038.50
More than 2016026.5827.646.4328.009.00128.0426.84130.5044.00
Professional practice
Private practice49482.0627.836.3926.008.001.930 0.587 134.1726.44138.0034.000.409 0.938
Medical-social335.4826.705.9228.009.00137.5823.87138.0038.00
Hospital182.9926.567.1026.509.00134.3329.40138.5045.75
Mixed579.4728.286.7929.0012.00133.8132.41137.0052.00
Table 4. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison—age categories and number of years of professional practice.
Table 4. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison—age categories and number of years of professional practice.
BO Scores
Dunn’s Test for Age CategorieszWiWjp
20–2930–39 −2.927267.766323.8730.003
20–2940–49−1.692267.766302.2450.091
20–2950 and +−1.463267.766300.7330.143
30–39 40–491.170323.873302.2450.242
30–39 50 and +1.110323.873300.7330.267
40–4950 and +0.069302.245300.7330.945
Dunn’s test for duration before certificationzWiWjp
3 years4 years1.119323.649303.4970.263
3 years5 years2.396323.649268.5800.017
4 years5 years1.825303.497268.5800.068
Dunn’s test for number of years of professional practicezWiWjp
Less than 55 to 10 −1.719265.902303.1370.086
Less than 511 to 20 −3.014265.902326.8910.003
Less than 5More than 20 −1.554265.902298.2840.120
5 to 10 11 to 20 −1.207303.137326.8910.228
5 to 10 More than 20 0.239303.137298.2840.811
11 to 20 More than 20 1.523326.891298.2840.128
SPS scores
Dunn’s test for age categorieszWiWjp
20–2930–39 1.034331.062311.2170.301
20–2940–491.755331.062295.2810.079
20–2950 and +3.455331.062253.138<0.001
30–39 40–490.861311.217295.2810.389
30–39 50 and +2.783311.217253.1380.005
40–4950 and +1.917295.281253.1380.055
Dunn’s test for duration before certificationzWiWjp
3 years4 years−1.947271.121306.2330.052
3 years5 years−2.143271.121320.4290.032
4 years5 years−0.741306.233320.4290.458
Dunn’s test for number of years of professional practicezWiWjp
Less than 55 to 10 0.251320.630315.1890.802
Less than 511 to 20 0.356320.630313.4270.722
Less than 5More than 20 2.834320.630261.5280.005
5 to 10 11 to 20 0.089315.189313.4270.929
5 to 10 More than 20 2.640315.189261.5280.008
11 to 20 More than 20 2.761313.427261.5280.006
Table 5. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison for the three groups on burnout scores according to sensory processing sensitivity degree (hypo, medium, and highly sensitive).
Table 5. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison for the three groups on burnout scores according to sensory processing sensitivity degree (hypo, medium, and highly sensitive).
Factor χ2 Statp
SPS-categories68.859<0.001
zWiWjp
Highly sensitiveHypo sensitive8.287352.275204.493<0.001
Highly sensitiveMedium sensitive3.747352.275288.697<0.001
Hypo sensitiveMedium sensitive−4.172204.493288.697<0.001
Table 6. Coefficients of linear regression for BO with SPS components.
Table 6. Coefficients of linear regression for BO with SPS components.
Model UnstandardisedSEStandardisedtp
H₀(Intercept)27.7740.262 106.150<0.001
H₁(Intercept)21.1351.660 12.728<0.001
EOE0.1680.0330.3215.178<0.001
LST0.1010.0350.1752.8770.004
AES−0.2140.056−0.176−3.857<0.001
CHA−0.0320.101−0.015−0.3200.749
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Bordarie, J.; Mourtialon, C. Study of the Relationship between Sensory Processing Sensitivity and Burnout Syndrome among Speech and Language Therapists. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 7132. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20237132

AMA Style

Bordarie J, Mourtialon C. Study of the Relationship between Sensory Processing Sensitivity and Burnout Syndrome among Speech and Language Therapists. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(23):7132. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20237132

Chicago/Turabian Style

Bordarie, Jimmy, and Caroline Mourtialon. 2023. "Study of the Relationship between Sensory Processing Sensitivity and Burnout Syndrome among Speech and Language Therapists" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 23: 7132. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20237132

APA Style

Bordarie, J., & Mourtialon, C. (2023). Study of the Relationship between Sensory Processing Sensitivity and Burnout Syndrome among Speech and Language Therapists. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(23), 7132. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20237132

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop