Early Postnatal Protein-Calorie Malnutrition and Cognition: A Review of Human and Animal Studies
Abstract
:1. Introduction
Review Methodology
2. Human Studies
3. Animal Studies
4. Discussion
References
- De Onis, M; Blössner, M. WHO Global Database on Child Growth and Malnutrition; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 1997; p. 74. [Google Scholar]
- Galler, JR; Ramsey, FC; Forde, V; Salt, P; Archer, E. Long-term effects of early kwashiorkor compared with marasmus. II. Intelectual performance. J. Pediatr. Gastroenterol. Nutr 1987, 6, 847–854. [Google Scholar]
- Galler, JR; Ramsey, FC; Morley, DS; Archer, E; Salt, P. The long-term effects of early kwashiorkor compared with marasmus. IV. Performance on the national high school entrance examination. Pediatr. Res 1990, 28, 235–239. [Google Scholar]
- Agarwal, DK; Awasthy, A; Upadhyay, SK; Singh, P; Kumar, J; Agarwal, KN. Growth, behavior, development and intelligence in rural children between 1–3 years of life. Indian Pediatr 1992, 29, 467–480. [Google Scholar]
- Kar, BR; Rao, SL; Chandramouli, BA. Cognitive development in children with chronic protein energy malnutrition. Behav Brain Funct 2008, 4, 31:1–31:12. [Google Scholar]
- Mendez, MA; Adair, LS. Severity and timing of stunting in the first two years of life affect performance on cognitive tests in late childhood. J. Nutr 1999, 129, 1555–1562. [Google Scholar]
- Berkman, DS; Lescano, AG; Gilman, RH; Lopez, SL; Black, MM. Effects of stunting, diarrhoeal disease, and parasitic infection during infancy on cognition in late childhood: A follow-up study. Lancet 2002, 359, 564–571. [Google Scholar]
- Miranda, MC; Nóbrega, FJ; Sato, K; Pompéia, S; Sinnes, EG; Bueno, OFA. Neuropsychology and malnutrition: A study with 7 to 10 years-old children in a poor community. Rev. Bras. Saúde Matern. Infant 2007, 7, 45–54. [Google Scholar]
- Chavez, A; Martinez, C; Yaschine, T. Nutrition, behavioral development, and mother-child interaction in young rural children. Fed. Proc 1975, 34, 1574–1582. [Google Scholar]
- Pollitt, E; Gorman, KS; Engle, PL; Martorell, R; Rivera, J; Wachs, TD; Scrimshaw, NS. Early supplementary feeding and cognition: Effects over two decades. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev 1993, 58, 1–99. [Google Scholar]
- Li, H; Barnhart, HX; Stein, AD; Martorell, R. Effects of early childhood supplementation on the educational achievement of women. Pediatrics 2003, 112, 1156–1162. [Google Scholar]
- Whaley, SE; Sigman, M; Neumann, C; Bwibo, N; Guthrie, D; Weiss, RE; Alberz, S; Murphy, PS. The impact of dietary intervention on the cognitive development of Kenyan school children. J. Nutr 2003, 133, 3965S–3971S. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman, HE; Klein, RE; Kagan, J; Yarbrough, C. Relations between nutrition and cognition in rural Guatemala. Am. J. Public Health 1977, 67, 233–239. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman, HE; Klein, RE; Townsend, JW; Lechtig, A. Nutrition and cognitive development among rural Guatemalan children. Am. J. Public Health 1980, 70, 1277–1285. [Google Scholar]
- Klein, RE; Kagan, J; Freeman, HE; Yarbrough, C; Habicht, J. Is big smart? The relation of growth to cognition. J. Health Soc. Behav 1972, 13, 219–225. [Google Scholar]
- McKay, H; Sinisterra, L; McKay, A; Gomez, H; Lloreda, P. Improving cognitive ability in chronically deprived children. Science 1978, 200, 270–278. [Google Scholar]
- Waber, DP; Vuori-Christiansen, L; Ortiz, N; Clement, JR; Christiansen, NE; Mora, JO; Reed, RB; Herrera, MG. Nutritional supplementation, maternal education, and cognitive development of infants at risk of malnutrition. Am. J. Clin. Nuir 1981, 34, 797–803. [Google Scholar]
- Grantham-McGregor, SM; Powell, CA; Walker, SP; Himes, JH. Nutritional supplementation, psychosocial stimulation, and mental development of stunted children: The Jamaican study. Lancet 1991, 338, 1–5. [Google Scholar]
- Grantham-McGregor, SM; Walker, SP; Chang, SM; Powell, CA. Effects of early childhood supplementation with and without stimulation on later development in stunted Jamaican children. Am. J. Clin. Nutr 1997, 66, 247–253. [Google Scholar]
- Walker, SP; Grantham-McGregor, SM; Powell, CA; Chang, SM. Effects of growth restriction in early childhood on growth, IQ, and cognition at age 11 to 12 years and the benefits of nutritional supplementation and psychosocial stimulation. J. Pediatr 2000, 137, 36–41. [Google Scholar]
- Walker, SP; Chang, SM; Powell, CA; Grantham-McGregor, SM. Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth-stunted Jamaican children: Prospective cohort study. Lancet 2005, 366, 1804–1807. [Google Scholar]
- Brioni, JD; Orsingher, OA. Operant behavior and reactivity to the anticonflict effect of diazepam in perinatally undernourished rats. Physiol. Behav 1988, 44, 193–198. [Google Scholar]
- Tonkiss, J; Shultz, P; Galler, JR. An analysis of spatial navigation in prenatally protein malnourished rats. Physiol. Behav 1994, 55, 217–224. [Google Scholar]
- De Oliveira, LM; Almeida, SS. Effects of malnutrition and environment on the acquisition and extinction of avoidance behavior in rats. Physiol. Behav 1985, 34, 141–145. [Google Scholar]
- Almeida, SS; de Oliveira, LM; Graeff, FG. Decreased reactivity to anxiolytics caused by early protein malnutrition in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav 1990, 36, 997–1000. [Google Scholar]
- Almeida, SS; de Oliveira, LM; Graeff, FG. Early life protein malnutrition changes exploration of the elevated plus-maze and reactivity to anxiolytics. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1991, 103, 513–518. [Google Scholar]
- Rocinholi, LF; Almeida, SS; De-Oliveira, LM. Response threshold to aversive stimuli in stimulated early protein-malnourished rats. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res 1997, 30, 407–413. [Google Scholar]
- Hernandes, AS; Almeida, SS. Postnatal protein malnutrition affects inhibitory avoidance and risk assessment behaviors in two models of anxiety in rats. Nutr. Neurosci 2003, 6, 213–219. [Google Scholar]
- Barnes, RH; Cunnold, SR; Zimmermann, RR; Simmons, H; MacLeod, RB; Krook, L. Influence of nutritional deprivations in early life on learning behavior of rats as measured by performance in a water maze. J. Nutr 1966, 89, 399–410. [Google Scholar]
- Tonkiss, J; Galler, JR. Prenatal protein malnutrition and working memory performance in adult rats. Behav. Brain. Res 1990, 40, 95–107. [Google Scholar]
- Fukuda, MTH; Françolin-Silva, AL; Almeida, SS. Early postnatal protein malnutrition affects learning and memory in the distal but not in the proximal cue version of the Morris water maze. Behav. Brain Res 2002, 133, 271–277. [Google Scholar]
- Valadares, CT; De Sousa Almeida, S. Early protein malnutrition changes learning and memory in spaced but not in condensed trials in the Morris water-maze. Nutr. Neurosci 2005, 8, 39–47. [Google Scholar]
- Hemb, M; Cammarota, M; Nunes, ML. Effects of early malnutrition, isolation and seizures on memory and spatial learning in the developing rat. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci 2010, 28, 303–307. [Google Scholar]
- Valadares, CT; Fukuda, MT; Françolin-Silva, AL; Hernandes, AS; Almeida, SS. Effects of postnatal protein malnutrition on learning and memory procedures. Nutr. Neurosci 2010, 13, 274–282. [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, Y; Li, N; Yang, Z. Perinatal food restriction impaired spatial learning and memory behavior and decreased the density of nitric oxide synthase neurons in the hippocampus of adult male rat offspring. Toxicol. Lett 2010, 193, 167–172. [Google Scholar]
- Crnic, LS; Chase, HP. Models of infantile malnutrition in rats: Effects on milk. J. Nutr 1978, 108, 1755–1760. [Google Scholar]
- Pine, AP; Jessop, NS; Oldham, JD. Maternal protein reserves and their influence on lactational performance in rats: The effect of dietary protein restriction and stage of lactation on milk composition. Br. J. Nutr 1994, 72, 815–830. [Google Scholar]
- Passos, MCF; Ramos, CF; Moura, EG. Short and long term effects of malnutrition in rats during lactation on the body weight of offspring. Nutr. Res 2000, 20, 1603–1612. [Google Scholar]
- Hernandes, AS; Françolin-Silva, AL; Valadares, CT; Fukuda, MTH; Almeida, SS. Effects of different malnutrition techniques on the behavior of rats tested in the elevated T-maze. Behav. Brain Res 2005, 162, 240–245. [Google Scholar]
- Smart, JL; Preece, J. Maternal behavior of undernourished mother rats. Anim. Behav 1973, 21, 613–619. [Google Scholar]
- Crnic, LS. Models of infantile malnutrition in rats: Effects on maternal behavior. Dev. Psychobiol 1980, 13, 615–628. [Google Scholar]
- Riul, TR; Carvalho, AF; Almeida, PS; De-Oliveira, LM; Almeida, SS. Ethological analysis of mother-pup interactions and other behavioral reactions in rats: Effects of malnutrition and tactile stimulation of the pups. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res 1999, 32, 975–983. [Google Scholar]
- Lima, JG; Oliveira, LM; Lachat, JJ; Dal-Bo, CM; Almeida, SS. Comparison of the effects of lab chow and casein diets based on body and brain development of rats. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res 1993, 26, 1069–1076. [Google Scholar]
- Rocinholi, LF; de Oliveira, LM; Colafêmina, JF. Malnutrition and environmental stimulation in rats: Wave latencies of the brainstem auditory evoked potentials. Nutr Neurosci 2001, 4, 199–212. [Google Scholar]
- Camargo, LM; Nascimento, AB; Almeida, SS. Differential response to gepirone but not to chlordiazepoxide in malnourished rats subjected to learned helplessness. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res 2005, 41, 54–59. [Google Scholar]
- Fukuda, MT; Françolin-Silva, AL; Hernandes, AS; Valadares, CT; Almeida, SS. Effects of early protein malnutrition and scopolamine on learning and memory in the Morris water maze. Nutr. Neurosci 2007, 10, 251–259. [Google Scholar]
- Castro, CA; Rudy, JW. Impaired short-term memory resulting from postnatal undernutrition is attenuated by physostigmine. Behav. Neural Biol 1993, 60, 282–285. [Google Scholar]
- Huang, LT; Lai, MC; Wang, CL; Wang, CA; Yang, CH; Hsieh, CS; Liou, CW; Yang, SN. Long-term effects of early-life malnutrition and status epilepticus: Assessment by spatial navigation and CREB(Serine-133) phosphorylation. Dev. Brain Res 2003, 145, 213–218. [Google Scholar]
- Martínez, Y; Díaz-Cintra, S; León-Jacinto, U; Aguilar-Vázquez, A; Medina, AC; Quirarte, GL; Prado-Alcalá, RA. Effects of postnatal malnutrition and senescence on learning, long-term memory, and extinction in the rat. Behav. Brain Res 2009, 203, 48–53. [Google Scholar]
- Lukoyanov, NV; Andrade, JP. Behavioral effects of protein deprivation and rehabilitation in adult rats: Relevance to morphological alterations in the hippocampal formation. Behav. Brain Res 2000, 112, 85–97. [Google Scholar]
- Cintra, L; Díaz-Cintra, S; Galvan, A; Kemper, T; Morgane, PJ. Effects of protein undernutrition on the dentate gyrus in rats of three age groups. Brain Res 1990, 532, 271–277. [Google Scholar]
- Bedi, KS. Effects of undernutrition during early life on granule cell numbers in the rat dentate gyrus. J. Comp. Neurol 1991, 311, 425–433. [Google Scholar]
- Wolf, C; Almli, CR; Finger, S; Ryan, S; Morgane, PJ. Behavioral effects of severe and moderate early malnutrition. Physiol. Behav 1986, 38, 725–730. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell, LF; Bedi, KS. The effects of undernutrition during early life on spatial learning. Physiol. Behav 1989, 45, 883–890. [Google Scholar]
- Bedi, KS. Spatial learning ability of rats undernourished during early postnatal life. Physiol. Behav 1992, 51, 1001–1007. [Google Scholar]
- Castro, CA; Tracy, M; Rudy, JW. Early life undernutrition impairs the development of the learning and short-term memory process mediating performance in a conditional-spatial discrimination task. Behav. Brain Res 1989, 32, 255–264. [Google Scholar]
- Jordan, TC; Cane, SE; Howells, KF. Deficits in spatial memory performance induced by early undernutrition. Dev. Psychobiol 1981, 14, 317–325. [Google Scholar]
- Levitsky, DA; Barnes, RH. Nutritional and environmental interactions in the behavioral development of the rat: Long-term effects. Science 1972, 176, 68–71. [Google Scholar]
- Smart, JL. “Malnutrition, learning and behavior”: 25 years on from the MIT symposium. Proc. Nutr. Soc 1993, 52, 189–199. [Google Scholar]
- Strupp, BJ; Levitsky, DA. Enduring effects of early maulnutriton: A theoretical reappraisal. J. Nutr 1995, 125, 2221S–2232S. [Google Scholar]
- Morgane, PJ; Austin-Lafrance, RJ; Bronzino, JD; Tonkiss, J; Díaz-Cintra, S; Cintra, L; Kemper, T; Galler, JR. Prenatal Malnutrition and development of the brain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 1993, 17, 91–128. [Google Scholar]
- Morgane, PJ; Mokler, DJ; Galler, JR. Effects of prenatal protein malnutrition on the hippocampal formation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 2002, 26, 471–483. [Google Scholar]
- Almeida, SS; Tonkiss, J; Galler, JR. Malnutrition and reactivity to drugs acting in the central nervous system. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 1996, 20, 389–402. [Google Scholar]
- Georgieff, MK. Nutrition and the developing brain: Nutrient priorities and measurement. Am. J. Clin. Nutr 2007, 85, 614–620. [Google Scholar]
- Dobbing, J. Vulnerable periods in developing brain. In Applied Neurochemistry; Davison, N, Dobbing, J, Eds.; Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 1968; pp. 287–316. [Google Scholar]
- Morris, RGM; Garrud, P; Rawlins, JNP; O’Keefe, J. Place navigation impaired in rats with hipocampal lesions. Nature 1982, 297, 681–683. [Google Scholar]
- O’Keefe, JB; Nadel, R. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map; Clarendon: Oxford, UK, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- D’Hooge, R; De Deyn, PP. Applications of the Morris water maze in the study of learning and memory. Brain Res. Rev 2001, 36, 60–90. [Google Scholar]
- Ruben, RJ. A time frame of critical/sensitive periods of language development. Acta Otolaryngol. (Stockh) 1997, 117, 202–205. [Google Scholar]
- Connolly, K. Learning and the concept of critical periods in infancy. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol 1972, 14, 705–714. [Google Scholar]
- Santos, D; Assis, AMO; Bastos, ACS; Santos, LM; Santos, CAST; Strina, A; Prado, MS; Almeida-Filho, NM; Rodrigues, LC; Barreto, M. Determinants of cognitive function in childhood: A cohort study in a middle income context. BMC Public Health 2008, 8, 202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gou, G; Harris, KH. The mechanisms mediating the effects of poverty on children’s intellectual development. Demography 2000, 37, 431–447. [Google Scholar]
- McCulloch, A; Joshi, HE. Neighbourhood and family influences on the cognitive ability of children in the British National Child Development Study. Soc. Sci. Med 2001, 53, 579–591. [Google Scholar]
- Santos, LM; Santos, DN; Bastos, ACS; Assis, AMO; Prado, MS; Barreto, ML. Determinants of early cognitive development: Hierarchical analysis of a longitudinal study. Cad. Saúde Pública 2008, 24, 427–437. [Google Scholar]
- Meeks Gardner, JM; Grantham-Mcgregor, SM; Chang, SM. Behaviour of stunted children and the relationship to development. Proc. Nutr. Soc 1993, 52, 36A. [Google Scholar]
Study authors | Year | Nutritional supplementation | Psychosocial stimulation | Cognitive tasks | Statistical significance | Age of children | Country of study population |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Galler et al. [2] | 1987 | No | No | WISC-R1; Piagetian tests of conservation | Yes* | 11–18 years | Barbados |
Galler et al. [3] | 1990 | No | No | 11-plus examination; WISC-R1; Classroom behavior | Yes* | 11–18 years | Barbados |
Agarwal et al. [4] | 1992 | No | No | Gesell’s development schedule; Binet Kulsrestha Intelligence scale; Level of Cognitive Ability; Block-Sort test | Yes* | 1–3 years | India |
Kar et al. [5] | 2008 | No | No | NIMHANS neuropsychological battery for children | Yes* | 5–10 years | India |
Mendez et al. [6] | 1999 | No | No | Philippines Non-Verbal Intelligence test; Cognitive test; Level of schooling | Yes* | 8 and 11 years | Philippines |
Berkman et al. [7] | 2002 | No | No | WISC-R1 | Yes* | 9 years | Peru |
Miranda et al. [8] | 2007 | No | No | Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices–Special scale; Cancellation of letters and symbol tests; WCST2; Stanford-Binet Scale; The Bender Visual Motor Gestalt test; Rivermead Behavioral Memory test; Corsi Block and Digital Span test; BCPR3; Educational Performance test; Teacher Rating Behavior scale | Yes* | 7–10 years | Brazil |
Chavez et al. [9] | 1975 | Yes | No | Physical, mental, and social development | Yes# | 0–18 months | Mexico |
Pollitt et al. [10] | 1993 | Yes | No | Brazelton Neonatal Assessment; Composite Infant Scale; Psychoeducational and information-processing tests | Yes‡ | 0–7 years | Guatemala |
Whaley et al. [12] | 2003 | Yes | No | Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices; Verbal Meaning test; Arithmetic test | Yes‡ | 7 years | Kenya |
Freeman et al. [13] | 1977 | Yes | No | Language facility; Short-term memory for numbers; Perceptual analysis | Yes‡ | 3–4 years | Guatemala |
Freeman et al. [14] | 1980 | Yes | No | Language facility; Short-term memory for numbers; Perceptual analysis; Cognitive composite | Yes‡ | 3–7 years | Guatemala |
McKay et al. [16] | 1978 | Yes | Yes | Understanding complex commands; Figure tracing; Picture vocabulary; Intersensory perception; Colors, numbers and letters; Use of prepositions; Block construction; Cognitive maturity; Sentence completion; Memory for sentences; Knox cubes; Geometric drawings; Arithmetic; Mazes; Information; Vocabulary; Block design; Digit memory; Analogies and similarities; Matrices; Visual classification | Yes‡ | 3–7 years | Colombia |
Waber et al. [17] | 1981 | Yes | Yes | Griffiths test; Einstein scale | Yes‡ | 3 years | Colombia |
Grantham-McGregor et al. [18] | 1991 | Yes | Yes | Griffiths mental development scales | Yes# | 9–24 months | Jamaica |
Grantham-McGregor et al. [19] | 1997 | Yes | Yes | WRAT4; Stanford Binet test; PPVT5; Raven’s Progressive Matrices; Categorical fluency; Verbal analogies; Free call; French learning test; Digit span; Corsi blocks; Lafayette Grooved Pegboard test | Yes# | 7–8 years | Jamaica |
Walker et al. [20] | 2000 | Yes | Yes | Test of general intelligence; WISC-R1; test of visual reasoning ability; Raven’s Progressive Matrices; test of language comprehension; PPVT5; test of verbal analogies; Vocabulary test; test of auditory working memory; Digit span forwards and digit span backwards; test of visual-spatial memory; Corsi blocks; search test; Stroop test modified | Yes# ‡ | 11–12 years | Jamaica |
Walker et al. [21] | 2005 | Yes | Yes | WAIS6; Raven’s Progressive Matrices; Corsi blocks; Digit span forwards and digit span backwards; test of verbal analogies; PPVT5; group reading test 2-revised; WRAT7 | Yes# ‡ | 17–18 years | Jamaica |
Study authors | Year | Nutritional rehabilitation | Cognitive tasks | Statistical significance | Age of animals |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Brioni & Orsingher [22] | 1988 | After weaning | Operant Reinforcement Test | Yes | 70 days |
Tonkiss, Shultz & Galler [23] | 1994 | After birth | Morris Water Maze (a) Distal-cue test (b) Proximal-cue test | No | 16, 20, 70, 200–221 days |
De Oliveira & Almeida [24] | 1985 | After weaning | Avoidance Performance Test | Yes | 70 days |
Almeida, de Oliveira & Graeff [25] | 1990 | After the 50th day of life | Hypertonic Water Ingestion and dose effect curves for (a) diazepam, (b) ipsapirone, (c) ritanserin and (d) isamoltane | Yes (a and b) No (c and d) | 91 days |
Almeida, de Oliveira & Graeff [26] | 1991 | After the 50th day of life | Elevated Plus-maze Test and Reactivity to Anxiolytic Drugs | Yes | 70 days |
Rocinholi, Almeida & De-Oliveira [27] | 1997 | After weaning | Shock Threshold Test (a) Tail-flick Test (b) | Yes (a) No (b) | 70 days |
Hernandes & Almeida [28] | 2003 | After the 50th day of life | Elevated T-maze (ETM) and Stretched Attend Posture (SAP) | Yes | 70 days |
Barnes et al. [29] | 1966 | After weaning (a) or after the 77th day of life (b) | Y Water Maze—Visual discrimination performance | Yes (b) No (a) | 180–270 days |
Tonkiss & Galler [30] | 1990 | Postnatal life | Elevated T-maze | Yes | 90 or 160 days |
Fukuda, Françolin-Silva & Almeida [31] | 2002 | After the 50th day of life | Morris Water Maze (a) Distal-cue test (b) Proximal-cue test | Yes (a) No (b) | 70–77 and 102 days |
Valadares & De Sousa Almeida [32] | 2005 | After weaning | Morris Water Maze (a) Spaced trials (b) Intermediate trials (c) Condensed trials | Yes (a) No (b and c) | 70–77 and 102 days |
Hemb, Cammarota & Nunes [33] | 2010 | --- | Morris Water Maze (a) Undernutrition (b) Seizures | Yes | 21–25 days |
Valadares et al. [34] | 2010 | After weaning | Morris Water Maze (a) Recognition Memory of Objects (b) Water T-maze (c) | Yes (a and b) No (c) | 70 days |
Zhang, Li & Yang [35] | 2010 | After weaning | Morris Water Maze | Yes | 70 days |
Crnic & Chase [36] | 1978 | --- | Milk Composition | Yes | --- |
Pine, Jessop & Oldham [37] | 1994 | --- | Milk Composition | Yes | --- |
Passos, Ramos & Moura [38] | 2000 | --- | Milk Composition | Yes | --- |
Hernandes et al. [39] | 2005 | After weaning | Elevated T-maze | Yes | 70 days |
Smart & Preece [40] | 1973 | --- | Maternal Behavior | Yes | --- |
Crnic [41] | 1980 | --- | Maternal Behavior | Yes | --- |
Riul et al. [42] | 1999 | --- | Mother-pup Interaction | Yes | --- |
Lima et al. [43] | 1993 | After weaning | Lab Chow and Experimental Casein Diets Ingestion | Yes | 49 days |
Rocinholi, de Oliveira & Colafêmina [44] | 2001 | After weaning half the malnourished group | Brainstem Auditory Evoked Potentials | Yes | 14, 18, 22, 32, and 42 days. |
Camargo, Nascimento & Almeida [45] | 2005 | After weaning | Open Field—Social play behavior | Yes | 26, 36, 46, 56, 66 and 76 days |
Fukuda et al. [46] | 2007 | After the 50th day of life | Morris Water Maze after treatment with scopolamine (amnesic drug) | Yes | 70 days |
Castro & Rudy [47] | 1993 | After weaning | Conditional-spatial Discrimination Task | 90 days | |
Huang et al. [48] | 2003 | After weaning | Morris Water Maze | No | 80 days |
Martínez et al. [49] | 2009 | After weaning | Active Avoidance Task | Yes | 90 or 660 days |
Lukoyanov & Andrade [50] | 2000 | From the 8th to the 10th month* | Open Field (a) Passive Avoidance Task (b) Morris Water Maze (c) | Yes (a and c) No (b) | 10 months |
Cintra et al. [51] | 1990 | --- | Neuroanatomical Study | Yes | 30, 90 and 220 days |
Bedi [52] | 1991 | After the 30th day of life | Neuroanatomical Study | Yes | 70 and 212 days |
Wolf et al. [53] | 1986 | After weaning | Open Field (a) 8-arm Radial Maze (b) | Yes (a and b) | 90 days |
Campbell & Bedi. [54] | 1989 | After the 30th or the 60th day of life | Morris Water Maze | No | 70 days |
Bedi [55] | 1992 | After the 30th day of life | Morris Water Maze | Yes | 35–65 or 170–200 days |
Castro, Tracy & Rudy [56] | 1989 | After weaning | Conditional-spatial Discrimination Task-Win-shift version | Yes | 23, 30, 40, and 90 days |
Jordan, Cane & Howells [57] | 1981 | After weaning | 8 and 16-arm Radial Maze | Yes | 90 days |
Levitsky & Barnes [58] | 1972 | After the 7th week of life | Social Behavior | Yes | Birth–17 weeks |
© 2011 by the authors; licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open-access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
Share and Cite
Laus, M.F.; Duarte Manhas Ferreira Vales, L.; Braga Costa, T.M.; Sousa Almeida, S. Early Postnatal Protein-Calorie Malnutrition and Cognition: A Review of Human and Animal Studies. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2011, 8, 590-612. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph8020590
Laus MF, Duarte Manhas Ferreira Vales L, Braga Costa TM, Sousa Almeida S. Early Postnatal Protein-Calorie Malnutrition and Cognition: A Review of Human and Animal Studies. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2011; 8(2):590-612. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph8020590
Chicago/Turabian StyleLaus, Maria Fernanda, Lucas Duarte Manhas Ferreira Vales, Telma Maria Braga Costa, and Sebastião Sousa Almeida. 2011. "Early Postnatal Protein-Calorie Malnutrition and Cognition: A Review of Human and Animal Studies" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 8, no. 2: 590-612. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph8020590
APA StyleLaus, M. F., Duarte Manhas Ferreira Vales, L., Braga Costa, T. M., & Sousa Almeida, S. (2011). Early Postnatal Protein-Calorie Malnutrition and Cognition: A Review of Human and Animal Studies. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 8(2), 590-612. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph8020590