1. Introduction
Human action has been widely deemed as the main contributor to environmental problems [
1,
2,
3]. Environmental degradation has been emerging as a global challenge faced by humankind in the last decades. Various issues such as air pollution, exhausting resources, biodiversity decrease, diminishing rainforests and worsening of water quality have been caused by environment problems. Scholars argue most of the environment problems are caused by human behavior [
4,
5]. The United Nations (UN) initiated “Sustainable Development Goals” (SDG) to support countries and regions all over the world to develop in a sustainable way, and 17 goals including affordable and clean energy, clean water and sanitation, sustainable cities and communities, climate action and responsible consumption and production have been proposed as common aims for everyone.
An energy crisis is also threatening sustainability development. The price of crude oil has surged dramatically by 65% in 2021 to USD 83 per barrel, and the price of gasoline also witnessed a significant rise in most regions and countries. China has become the country with the highest import of crude oil since 2018 [
6], with more than 70% of the crude oil consumed in China being imported [
7]. Transportation has drawn significant notice for consuming a majority of crude oil and emitting pollutive substances. According to statistics [
8], transportation accounts for 16% of the total CO2 emitted annually, being the third largest resource globally. Transition to cleaner-energy transportation has been promoted by governments, NGOs, industry and consumers. The advantages of electric vehicles are prominent. One of the most important points that differs electric vehicles (battery electric vehicles, plug-in hybrid electric vehicles and hybrid electric vehicles) from conventional vehicles is that electric vehicles are more environmentally friendly: no need for petrol or diesel and low emission from their tailpipe. Governments in the world such as China, U.S., India and Japan, as well as the European Union, have been setting plans and enacting policies to guarantee fast transformation into transportation electrification.
Electric vehicles are a unique eco-innovative product for their high-involvement characteristic. Unlike curtailment behaviors such as car usage deduction and recycling, purchasing an electric vehicle could be more complex because it is more expensive, requires more information before making a decision, reflects the image of the consumer and has social values [
9]. Two stems of motivational factors have been identified in the literature that influence the intention to adopt an electric vehicle, namely the functional and utilitarian attributes of electric vehicles and the motives for green environmental development [
10]. The fact is, even though consumers have stronger propensity to protect the environment, low adoption of electric vehicles leads to discussion of key factors and antecedents of intention to adopt an electric vehicle. While functional factors such as cost and access to charging facilities have been found to influence the decision-making process, normative factors have emerged as a key antecedent [
11,
12,
13]. EV sales in China skyrocketed in the last several years as government incentives kept declining, and it is challenging for rational-based frameworks to explain this scenario because EV sales surged as the costs were becoming higher. However, the literature has omitted how values, beliefs and norms could influence the intention to adopt an EV in China so far.
Adequate literature on adoption intention and behavior of electric vehicles has been published focusing on a large coverage of antecedents in the last two decades. Nevertheless, there are still gaps remaining. First, the diversity of frameworks adopted needs to be enriched. The majority of existing research utilizes rational theories such as the Theory of Planned Behavior [
10,
14,
15] and Theory of Reasoned Action [
16,
17], and the Technology Acceptance Model has also been used by [
18,
19]. Since governments are cutting down the incentives to buy EVs and the sales are still increasing significantly, rational theories might encounter difficulty explaining the current phenomenon. On the other hand, even moral factors have been found to be important to consumers’ adoption of EVs, and limited studies have employed moral-based theories, for example, the Value–Belief–Norm theory or Norm Activation Theory. Second, as the diffusion of EVs is accelerating, the perception of consumers might vary accordingly. One important aspect is the social influence effect. According to diffusion of innovation [
20], the diffusion of innovative product is influenced by communication over time among individuals in a social system. He categorized consumers and their share of percentage as follows: innovators (1.5%), early adopters (13.5%), early majority (34%), late majority (34%) and laggard (15%). As per statistics revealed by CAAM [
21], the market share of sale volume of EVs increased dramatically to 16.11% in 2021. Referring to the categorization proposed by diffusion of innovation, China is transferring from the stage of early adopters to early majority. Factors affecting social influence could have critical impact during the diffusion of EVs, and culture is one of them [
22].
Culture has been shown to have influence on consumption behaviors [
23], and the influence is vital in the context of attitudes and behaviors of sustainable consumption [
24]. Furthermore, study on the influence of culture on how consumers make decisions on the purchase of high-involvement products is crucial with the emerging power of consumption, trend of globalization and technology advancement [
9]. Stern et al. [
25] highlighted that environmental behavior is influenced by how individuals perceive the organization structure of society. Specifically, collectivism has emerged as one of the most important culture orientations to influence pro-environmental behavior, because it distinguishes people’s tendency towards a certain type of behavior from a self-construal level. Since pro-environmental behavior is usually altruistic, it considers the trade-off between personal benefit and greater benefit. People that receive effects from collectivism usually prioritize in-group benefits over personal benefits and value in-group harmony. They are more inclined to sacrifice personal benefits for the greater good for other group members. Stern et al. [
25] also found that individualistic people are less likely to perform environmentally friendly behavior, when personal norms are the only antecedent.
Research on the role of collectivism on consumers’ intention to adopt electric vehicles is scant, and scholars have been calling for further study on the topic. Previous studies found mixed results: Dogan and Ozmen [
26] and Novotny et al. [
27] found a positive relationship between collectivism and intention to adopt electric vehicles, while Ashmore et al. [
28] and Barbarossa and Pastore [
29] asserted the effect is negative. Further study on the relationship between collectivism and intention to adopt electric vehicles is needed as suggested by Afroz et al. [
30] and Adnan et al. [
31]. Afroz et al. [
30] inferred that the collectivist nature of consumers in Malaysia could be positively linked with electric vehicle diffusion, while He and Zhan [
12] assumed individualistic culture could promote the translation from personal norms into intention to adopt electric vehicles in China. However, these are all speculations without empirical research. Saleem et al. [
32] highlighted the collectivistic tendency of consumers could be influential to the interpretation process from personal norms to intention to adopt electric vehicles. Collectivists have a higher tendency to ascribe responsibility for their behaviors and receive influence of moral judgement during the decision-making process [
33,
34]. Adnan et al. [
31] related the collectivistic nature of Malaysian consumers to the relationship between personal norms and the intention to adopt electric vehicles and assume collectivism might negatively influence the relationship. However, as most previous studies found a positive relationship between collectivism and intention to adopt electric vehicles, it is worth studying how collectivism could moderate the relationship between personal norms and intention to adopt electric vehicles.
To sum up, in order to shed light on the mechanism of electric vehicle acceptance of Chinese consumers using normative-based theory and clarify the effect of collectivism as a factor of social influence, this study aims to examine the intention to adopt electric vehicles in China by adding collectivism as a moderator to the Value–Belief–Norm framework.
4. Results and Findings
In social science studies, common method variance has been viewed as a problem affecting the credibility of research results and should be avoided. This study used both procedural and statistical remedies to avoid common method variance [
134]. For the procedural remedy, all participants were informed the data would be completely confidential, and there are no right or wrong answers. For the statistical remedy, both Harman’s single factor test and common method factor analysis proposed by Liang [
135] were conducted to detect if there is common method variance in the data. Harman’s single factor test conducts exploratory factor analysis by importing all variables and checking if the variance accounting for of any single factor exceeds a standard, for example 50% [
134]. If the variance is higher than 50%, it could be suggested that the data are subject to common method variance; if the result is lower than 50%, the conclusion could be made that the data are not influenced by common method variance. IBM SPSS 27 was used for this study to conduct Harman’s single factor test with the collected data. The result after data processing was 22.184%, below the criterion set for 50%. Following Liang et al. [
134,
135], common method factor analysis was conducted by changing all indicators into single-indicator second-order constructs and relating the constructs with a method factor consisting of all the indicators of the principal constructs (
Figure 3). The average substantive and method variance are 0.781 and 0.003, and the ratio is 28.5:1 (
Appendix A). Even though many social science studies adopt Harman’s single factor analysis to detect common method variance, common method factor analysis paves another innovative and insightful way. In this study, both ways returned negative results, suggesting that common method variance is not a major concern in this study.
4.1. Demographic Distribution of Respondents
Respondents’ profiles have been presented in
Table 3. In terms of gender, there are 167 male respondents (38.4%) and 268 female respondents (61.6%). The majority of respondents were ages from 18–30 (251 respondents, 57.7%), followed by 31–40 (71, 16.3%), 41–50 (48, 11%), 51–60 (54, 12.4%) and above 60 (11, 2.5%). Moreover, 10 people hold a diploma from junior high school (2.3%), 9 people hold a diploma from senior high school (2.1%), 159 people hold a graduate diploma (36.6%) and 251 people hold a postgraduate diploma (57.7%), and there are also 6 respondents who chose others (1.4%). From a personal income perspective, 240 respondents have an average personal monthly income below CNY 5000 (55.2%), while 128 respondents were from CNY 5000–10,000 (29.4%), 59 respondents were from CNY 10,000–20,000 (13.6%) and 8 respondents were above CNY 20,000 (1.8%).
Regarding household car ownership, 234 respondents have one car in their family (53.8%), 95 respondents have two cars in their family (21.8%), 87 respondents do not have any car in their family (20.0%) and 19 respondents have three or more cars in their family (4.4%). Regarding marital status, 252 respondents are married (57.9%) and 183 respondents are single (42.1%). Regarding number of kids, 265 respondents do not have any children (60.9%), 130 respondents have one child (29.9%), 36 respondents have two children (8.3%) and 4 respondents have three or more children (0.9%). Regarding location of residence, 302 respondents live in an urban area (69.4%), while 49 respondents live in a rural area (11.3%) and 84 respondents live in a village (19.3%).
4.2. Measurement Model Assessment
For measurement model assessment, reliability and validity of measurements of each construct are examined. Composite reliability is used to evaluate the internal consistency reliability of the constructs [
136]. For validity assessment, convergent validity is examined by assessing outer loadings and average variance extracted (AVE), while discriminant validity is examined using Fornell–Larcker criterion analysis, cross-loadings and Heterotrait–Monotrait (HTMT) ratio. The results are presented in
Table 4 and
Table 5.
From the results demonstrated in
Table 4 and
Table 5, it could be concluded that all constructs have satisfactory reliability (Cronbach’s alpha and composite reliability above 0.7). In terms of convergent validity, Hair et al. [
133] provided a rule of thumb suggesting that items with outer loadings higher than 0.7 could be satisfactory. Outer loadings between 0.4 and 0.7 should be considered for elimination. The majority of items have satisfactory outer loadings higher than 0.7, while several items have outer loadings between 0.4 and 0.7. Although they could be considered eliminated according to rule of thumb, Hair et al. [
132] argued that if the elimination of indicators that have outer loadings between 0.4 and 0.7 could not increase the AVE of a construct, it could be retained. All constructs have an AVE higher than 0.5, except for NEP (0.47). Fornell and Larcker [
137] advised that the validity of a construct is still adequate and acceptable if the composite reliability of the construct is higher than 0.6.
For discriminant validity, Fornell–Larcker criterion analysis is to compare the square root of AVE of a construct with its correlations with other constructs, and all constructs meet the criterion for Fornell–Larcker analysis with the highest value of square root of AVE compared with correlations with other constructs. The rule of thumb for HTMT ratio analysis is if the value is higher than 0.85, the discriminant validity is unacceptable; otherwise, it could be concluded the validity is discriminant [
128]. As shown in
Table 4, no value is higher than 0.85. Different from the Fornell-–Larcker criterion analysis, which compares correlations between constructs, cross-loadings analysis focuses on each indicator of all constructs and examines if the loadings of any indicator to the other construct is higher than to its own construct [
138]. After calculation by running the PLS algorithm, the results of cross-loadings analysis of all indicators in this study are presented (see
Supplementary Files). It could be concluded that all indicators have higher loadings to their own constructs. All constructs have satisfying reliability and validity results after measurement model assessment, and the next step in structural model assessment will be conducted.
4.3. Structural Model Assessment
4.3.1. Collinearity Assessment
The aim of structural model assessment is to examine the model’s ability to predict the relationships between the constructs of Hair et al. [
132]. The procedure has been advised as five steps, which are (1) assess structural model for collinearity issues, (2) assess the significance and relevance of the structural model relationships, (3) assess the level of R
2, (4) assess the effect sizes
f2 and (5) assess the predictive relevance Q
2 and the q2 effect sizes [
132] (p. 169) (see
Figure 4). Moreover, this study would conduct mediation and moderation analysis to test relevant proposed hypotheses.
Collinearity assessment is necessary since the path coefficients between constructs might be biased when significant collinearity exists among constructs. This is due to the OLS regression nature of path coefficients estimation in PLS-SEM [
132]. SmartPLS 3 (Oststeinbek, Germany) is utilized to assess the collinearity among constructs to obtain the variance inflation factor (VIF) of constructs that point to the same construct. VIF indicates to what extend there is an error variance for the unique effect of a predictor. Using SmartPLS 3, the VIF values of all constructs have been analyzed and shown in
Table 6.
The rule of thumb to examine the value of VIF is, if the VIF is lower than five, the collinearity issue is avoided [
139]. It could be concluded that there is no collinearity issue for the structural model assessment.
4.3.2. Path Coefficients between Constructs
Structural model path coefficients are standardized estimates to test structural model relationships. To test the significance of results, it is advised to use bootstrapping and examine the t-statistics and
p-value. Hair et al. [
126] suggested having 5000 bootstrap subsamples for final results preparation. Thus, this study set the subsamples number at 5000 in bootstrapping. After running bootstrapping in SmartPLS, the results are provided in
Table 7 and
Figure 4.
Results show that biospheric values (β = 0.206, p < 0.01) and altruistic values (β = 0.124, p < 0.05) have positive significant effects on the New Ecological Paradigm, while egoistic values (β= −0.097, p > 0.05) have a negative, insignificant effect. The New Ecological Paradigm (β = 0.446, p < 0.01) has a positive significant effect on awareness of consequence, and awareness of consequence (β = 0.492, p < 0.01) has a positive significant effect on ascription of responsibility. Ascription of responsibility (β = 0.648, p < 0.01) has a positive significant effect on personal norms. Personal norms (β = 0.437, p < 0.01) have a positive significant effect on intention to adopt EV.
4.3.3. Coefficient of Determination of Endogenous Constructs
Coefficient of determination (R
2) explains the extent of how the variance of the dependent variable could be attributed to any independent variables [
130]. More specifically, the aim to conduct coefficient of determination is to measure the predictive accuracy of the research framework. Hair et al. [
139] also highlighted that an R
2 value above 0.20 is considered high in consumer behavior research. Using SmartPLS 3, the R
2 is calculated by running the PLS algorithm. The R
2 of endogenous constructs in this study has been shown in
Table 8. Comparing against the rule of thumb, the R
2 of awareness of consequence (0.20), ascription of responsibility (0.242) and personal norms (0.42) could be categorized as high, while the R
2 values of intention to adopt EV (0.191) and the New Ecological Paradigm (0.101) are not as high as other endogenous constructs, even though awareness of consequence (0.191) is very close to 0.2.
4.3.4. Effect Size of Exogenous Constructs
Effect size (
f2) examines how a specific independent variable contributes to the variance in overall the R
2 value. As suggested by Cohen [
140], the
f2 values of 0.02, 0.15 and 0.35, respectively, are considered small, medium and large.
Table 8 illustrates the effect size of all exogenous latent variables. The highest
f2 value for all endogenous latent variables is the relationship between ascription of responsibility and personal norms, which is 0.723 (large). The other three relationships that have
f2 values which could be categorized as a medium effect are the relationship between awareness of consequence and ascription of responsibility (0.319), the relationship between the New Ecological Paradigm and awareness of consequence (0.250) and the relationship between personal norms and intention to adopt EV (0.236). Other relationships are considered as neither large nor medium. The results are shown in
Table 9.
4.3.5. Blindfolding and Predictive Relevance (Q2) of Constructs
The last step in structural model assessment is predictive relevance (Q
2) assessment. The Stone–Geisser test [
141,
142] is used for predictive relevance evaluation. The rule of thumb for Q
2 value evaluation provided by Hair et al. [
139] argues that if the Q
2 value of an endogenous reflective construct is greater than zero, it means that the exogenous constructs have predictive relevance for an endogenous construct.
Table 10 indicates the Q
2 value for this study. All endogenous latent variables in this study have a Q
2 value larger than zero, so it can be concluded that all exogenous constructs in this research framework have predictive relevance for endogenous construct.
In summary, after conducting measurement model and structural model assessment, the results and findings suggest that the measurement model shows great measurement reliability and validity, while the structural model assessment demonstrates that the exogenous and endogenous constructs are well-related.
4.4. Moderation Analysis of Collectivism
4.4.1. Bootstrapping
A moderating effect occurs when a moderating variable changes the strength or even the direction of the relationship between two constructs [
126]. Moderating effect is usually presented in PLS-SEM as an interaction term by multiplying the exogenous variable and moderator variable, which is called a two-way interaction [
126]. Hair et al. [
126] suggested it is generally recommended to use a two-stage approach since it demonstrates high statistical power and could be applied to both formative and reflective constructs. Thus, this study would use a two-stage approach for the interaction term creation. By running bootstrapping in SmartPLS 3, the path coefficient of the interaction term,
t-value and
p-value and effect size are obtained. Kenny [
143] depicted that 0.005, 0.01 and 0.025 would be large enough to be evaluated as small, medium and large.
Table 11 and
Figure 5 illustrate the results of moderating analysis. The path coefficient of PN*INT→INT is 0.187 (
p < 0.01), and the effect size (
f2) is large (0.046). It could be concluded that collectivism significantly moderates the relationship between personal norms and intention to adopt EV.
Figure 6 demonstrates the slope slot of the moderation effect of collectivism. To look into more detail at this graph, it indicates that when the personal norms of respondents are low (1 SD below median), there is little difference of intention to adopt EV between respondents with low and high levels of collectivism. However, on the other hand, when the personal norms of respondents are high (1 SD above median), high level of collectivism refers to a higher intention to adopt EV than respondents with low collectivism. However, for moderation effect analysis, it is critical to discover the influence of observed and unobserved heterogeneity in moderator variables [
126], and failure to consider the effects of heterogeneity could lead to threat to the validity of the moderation effect results.
4.4.2. Multigroup Analysis on High Collectivism vs. Low Collectivism
Multigroup analysis is frequently adopted to assess the heterogeneity in moderator variables. Since the aim of this study is to examine if there is a difference when comparing the path coefficient between personal norms and intention to adopt EV for respondents with high collectivism and low collectivism, two groups of respondents that are “High collectivism” and “Low collectivism” are identified and categorized based on the average value of collectivism. The dichotomization method has been frequently referred to for multigroup analysis to categorize samples into two groups when the moderator variable is a latent variable [
132]. In order to achieve a more meaningful difference between groups, the polar extremes approach was adopted for multigroup analysis for this study. Referring to the moderator variable, which is collectivism (mean = 6.07, SD = 0.79, min = 4, max = 7), samples with average values of collectivism higher than 6.8 (mean + 1SD) are categorized as “High collectivism” (91 samples), while samples with average values of collectivism lower than 5.4 (mean − 1SD) are categorized as “Low collectivism” (86 samples). Samples with an average value of collectivism between 5.4 and 6.8 are neglected for multigroup analysis.
The permutation test “randomly exchanges (i.e., permutes) observations between the groups and re-estimates the model for each permutation [
144]. Computing the differences between the group-specific path coefficients per permutation enables testing whether these also differ in the population” [
126] (p. 294). As suggested by Hair et al. [
126], permutation is more suitable for nonparametric multigroup analysis and yields better statistical properties; thus, this study would adopt permutation as the approach for multigroup analysis.
Similar group sample size of groups is a requirement for the premutation test [
126]. Moreover, the sample size should meet the criterion for PLS-SEM analysis execution. In this study, there are 91 samples and 86 samples in two groups, and only one direct relationship is to be examined. According to the ten-times rule of thumb, the sample size meets the requirements for PLS-SEM and premutation test conduction. Before conducting multigroup analysis, measurement model invariance was tested.
Since this study focuses on how the level of collectivism of respondents moderates the relationship between personal norms and intention to adopt EV, only relevant constructs will be considered for the measurement invariance test.
Table 12 indicates the sample size and measurement results for two groups. For better statistical performance, items with low outer loadings are deleted (INT4, PN1, PN4, PN5, PN7). The statistics of Cronbach’s alpha, composite reliability and AVE of personal norms and intention to adopt EV are all above the criterion and hence of reliability and validity for further structural model assessment (Cronbach’s alpha value > 0.7, composite reliability > 0.8 and AVE > 0.6).
The permutation test is conducted by selecting two groups (high and low in collectivism) with 5000 permutations, and a two-tailed test type is chosen with significance level set to 0.05. By applying the same measurement and setting to two groups, step 1 configural invariance is established. According to results provided in MICOM section under the quality criteria (see
Table 13), compositional invariance is achieved with both permutation
p-values higher than 0.05. Step 3 equality of a composite’s mean value and variance across groups is not established, because both the mean and variance of original difference do not fall between 2.5% and 97.5%, and permutation
p-value is less than 0.05 for intention to adopt EV (see
Table 14). According to Hair et al. [
126], partial measurement invariance is confirmed, and the study could proceed to conduct multigroup analysis.
The results of the permutation test for difference between high and low collectivism groups are illustrated in
Table 15 by assessing path coefficients in the final results. As there is difference and the difference is significant (permutation
p-value < 0.01), it could be concluded that there is difference between the moderation effect of collectivism among respondents with high and low levels of collectivism. Furthermore, as the difference of the path coefficient is positive (high collectivism-low collectivism), it demonstrates that the higher the level of collectivism, the stronger the moderation effect.
In summary, by conducting moderation effect analysis, this study confirms that collectivism significantly moderates the relationship between personal norms and intention to adopt EV through bootstrapping. Moreover, by conducting the premutation test, it is demonstrated that the higher the level of collectivism, the stronger the moderation effect collectivism has on the relationship between personal norms and intention to adopt EV.
4.5. Summary of Results
After conducting data analysis, the results of the proposed hypotheses are revealed in
Table 16. All hypotheses are accepted except for H3, which relates to egoistic values.
5. Discussion
5.1. Theoretical Implications
This article focuses on the influence of normative factors (value, belief and norm) and collectivism on Chinese consumers’ intention to adopt electric vehicles. Due to the emergence of vehicle emission pollution and the energy crisis, it has become more critical to figure out what the factors are affecting the decision-making process of consumers. Even though many studies have approached this problem from rational perspectives, limited knowledge is known from a moral perspective. Moreover, as a collectivistic country, despite the well-known influence of culture orientation, articles have rarely analyzed the effect of collectivism and how it could be utilized to accelerate the adoption of electric vehicles. From a theoretical perspective, this study empirically tested the VBN model for electric vehicle adoption in the context of China. The VBN model has been tested with various pro-environmental products, and only limited studies investigate the intention to adopt electric vehicles. However, there has been no record of a VBN-based study of electric vehicle adoption in China. This study addresses the gap by adopting the VBN framework and adding collectivism as a moderator variable to examine the intention to adopt electric vehicles of Chinese consumers. As study results have disclosed, the VBN framework is suitable for the mechanism explanation, with all relationships significant except between egoistic values and NEP. This result depicts that even if previous literature argued that normative factors and moral-based frameworks are not suitable for the prediction of purchase intention of high-involvement products such as electric vehicles, the VBN framework has ability to explain the complex decision-making mechanism.
In terms of values, both biospheric values and altruistic values positively influence NEP, while the effect of egoistic values is insignificant. This is in line with previous study results. Numerous research has proved that the relationship between biospheric values and NEP and altruistic values and NEP is positive [
32,
98], while past studies have returned contradictive results on the relationship between egoistic value and NEP, with Saleem et al. [
32] suggesting a positive relationship and Steg et al. [
98] indicating a negative relationship. That could explain the insignificant relationship found in this study, because both positive and negative evidence has been found before. It could be summarized that values, especially biospheric values and altruistic values, positively influence NEP. NEP is found to positively influence AC, which is in line with Chen [
145] and Saleem et al. [
32]. This suggests that an individual with higher NEP would be more aware of negative consequences and the results of behaviors that would lead to environmental degradation. AC positively influences AR, supporting the results of Saleem et al. [
32] and Chen [
145]. It could be suggested that an individual with higher AC would derive higher AR when considering adoption of electric vehicles. Furthermore, AR positively influences PN, and in the end, PN positively influences the intention to adopt electric vehicles, which is similar to many past studies.
The study also contributes to the sustainability development perspective. Being an environmentally friendly product, EVs reduce greenhouse gas emission and fossil fuel consumption. This is in line with Sustainable Development Goals (SDG) initiated by United Nations purposing “affordable and clean energy” and “climate action”. Personal norms as the factor directly influencing the intention to adopt EVs are critical for sustainability development and are affected by ascription of responsibility. Promotions and education should focus on how to stimulate the moral obligation of consumers, noticing that everyone is responsible for sustainability development and SDG, and emphasizing that the importance of the group benefit gained from sustainability development from a collectivistic perspective increases the tendency of an individual to act sustainably. Awareness of consequence influences ascription of responsibility directly, which means the consequences of actions that undermine the development of sustainability should be introduced to the public to increase ascription of responsibility. The New Ecological Paradigm influences awareness of consequence directly and is affected by biospheric values and altruistic values, which means an equal and friendly relationship between humans and the environment should be supported and how people concerned for the environment and wellbeing of others are decisive. All constructs included in the causal framework of VBN should be noticed and enhanced for the development of sustainability.
The positive moderation effect of collectivism on the relationship between PN and intention of electric vehicle adoption is significant. Although several studies have focused on the moderation effect of collectivism, this specific relationship has rarely been studied in China. To add, by using multigroup analysis, it is suggested that the higher the collectivism, the stronger the moderation effect. Since PN is the moral obligation perceived by the individual, collectivism as a critical culture orientation positively moderates the relationship because it involves how an individual perceives the relationship between individuals and society. As collectivists tend to prioritize the greater benefit to groups rather than personal interests, individuals might consider less about the costs and risks of electric vehicle adoption and focus more on the benefit electric vehicle adoption could have, for example, on reducing emission pollution and crude oil consumption. Another point is that since adopting electric vehicles has been promoted by the government as a pro-environmental and pro-social behavior, it could be deemed as “right” and ethical to do so. Collectivists would have higher efficacy because they believe other collectivists would follow the conduction of ethical behaviors [
146]. He and Zhan [
12] inferred the collectivist tendency of the Chinese consumer might hinder the translation of personal norms into intention to adopt electric vehicles, which is contradictive to the results of the current study. It could be caused by the neglection of the ethical consideration of acceptance of electric vehicles, and the authors speculated that collectivism would undermine the interpretation of any kind of personal norms into intention/behavior. However, the assumption needs to be further discussed depending on the nature of behavior.
The positive effect of collectivism on EV adoption could also be obtained by real-world statistics. Take the two biggest auto markets in the world, for example, China (annual car sale volume 20.5 million in 2021, no. 1 in the world) and the U.S. (annual car sale volume 15.1 million in 2021, no. 2 in the world). China is one of the most collectivistic countries with a collectivism index of 80, and the U.S. is the most individualistic country with a collectivism index of 9 [
64]. The market share of EVs in China in 2021 is 16.1% while only 4.2% in the U.S [
147]. Even if both the governments of China and the U.S. have been comprehensively supporting the diffusion of EVs, the penetration of EVs is significantly different. The significant difference of EV market share could act as evidence from the real world supporting the importance and the moderation effect of collectivism on the intention to adopt EVs.
To view things in a wider perspective, the theoretical findings of this study are useful to other countries and regions. Even though the VBN framework has been used to examine various pro-environmental products, only very limited studies have focused on EVs. This study revealed positive results supporting the feasibility of using the VBN framework for a study of consumers’ intention to adopt EVs. Future research is encouraged to employ VBN and other normative-based frameworks to investigate consumers’ intention to adopt EV and other high-innovative pro-environmental products in other regions. The moderation effect of collectivism on the relationship between personal norms and intention to adopt EVs could provide insight for studies on the effects of social influence and culture on intention to adopt EVs. It could be inferred that the differences of EV penetration among counties all over the world could be attributed to cultural reasons. For the translation from personal norms to intention to adopt EVs, higher collectivistic tendencies have a positive influence and lower collectivistic tendencies and lower individualistic tendencies negatively affect the translation. Considering the existing literature revealing the substantive effect of culture orientations including collectivism and individualism, further studies should focus on how cultural orientations are affecting the decision-making mechanism of consumers.
5.2. Managerial Implications
There are several managerial implications for policymakers and marketers in the automotive industry. Since the results of current study and past literature have demonstrated personal norms to be a direct influencing factor on consumers’ electric vehicle adoption intention, it is evident that policies and market interventions should be tailored to improve consumers’ personal norms. Norms should be promoted and emphasized to achieve higher personal norms among the public. Moreover, as AR is the antecedent of PN, it should also be strengthened to let more consumers know that citizens have a responsibility to preserve the environment by accepting electric vehicles. Other variables, such as values, NEP and AC, even though they do not directly influence PN, have indirect effects. Thus, policies and market interventions should pay attention to those aspects at the same time. Wang et al. [
148] argued that government, industry practitioners and electric vehicle sellers should let consumers know that it is everyone’s obligation to act in an environmentally friendly way.
The collectivist nature of Chinese consumers should not be omitted when designing marketing messages and policies. Dogan and Ozmen [
26] suggested policymakers should create high environmentalism norms in collectivistic society to promote electric vehicles, and educational courses should be provided to the public. By identifying the influence of collectivism, marketers could design communication messages that activate the collective self and emphasis on group benefit, with the aim to strengthen the moral obligation consumers would feel. The implications also remind marketers and policymakers in other countries and regions to take cultural orientation of consumers into consideration. To strengthen the propensity to adopt EV of consumers who have moral obligation, market communication and policies emphasizing collectivism could be helpful.
5.3. Limitations and Future Directions
The current study has several limitations. Compared with the large population and variations of policies in different regions in China, the sample size is limited; hence, the results lack generalizability. Similarly, the effects of demographic factors are not considered. Future studies could either focus on a specific area/population or extend the sample size to obtain better generalizability. Furthermore, this study focuses on the moderator effect of collectivism. There are many different culture orientations besides collectivism, and other culture orientations might also play roles in the diffusion of electric vehicles. Upcoming research could take more relevant cultural orientation into consideration. In addition, with the fast-changing pace of the economy, the behavior of consumers keeps developing. This study adopts a quantitative approach and is cross-sectional and hence might neglect the latest findings in the change in consumers perceptions. Qualitative approaches and research methods, such as interviews and case studies, and quantitative approaches, such as longitudinal studies, should be conducted to gain more insight regarding consumer behavioral changes.