Next Article in Journal
Analysis of Malfunctions in Selected Parking Systems in the Czech Republic
Next Article in Special Issue
Investors’ Moral and Financial Concerns—Ethical and Financial Divestment in the Fossil Fuel Industry
Previous Article in Journal
Impact of Transport Development on the Accessibility of Selected Functional Elements: The Case of the Suburban Zielonki Municipality within the Krakow Metropolitan Area
Previous Article in Special Issue
Environmental Externalities of Secondhand Markets—Based on a Dutch Auctioning Company
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Climate Change Beliefs, Personal Environmental Norms and Environmentally Conscious Behaviour Intention

Department of Management & Marketing, Swinburne University of Technology, Hawthorn, VIC 3122, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2022, 14(3), 1824; https://doi.org/10.3390/su14031824
Submission received: 15 December 2021 / Revised: 1 February 2022 / Accepted: 4 February 2022 / Published: 5 February 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Feature Papers in Economic and Business Aspects of Sustainability)

Abstract

:
Purpose—The purpose of this study is to examine the relationship between climate change beliefs, personal environmental norms and environmentally conscious behaviour. This study also investigates how the relationship between climate change beliefs and environmentally conscious behaviour is mediated by environmental identity. Design/methodology/approach—A survey conducted online involving 564 Australians informs the findings. Data analysis is performed using AMOS, a structural equation modelling package. Findings—This study finds strong positive relationships between climate change beliefs, personal environmental norms and environmentally conscious behaviour. The relationship between climate change beliefs and environmentally conscious behaviour is partially mediated by environmental identity. In addition, this study also finds that the relationships between personnel environmental norms, and environmental identity and environmentally conscious behaviour are partially mediated by climate change beliefs. Further, both personal environmental norms and climate change beliefs play stronger roles than environmental identity in environmentally conscious behaviour. Originality—This study engages in a scholarly conversation which claims the predictability of personal environmental norms in environmentally conscious behaviour. It adds value by establishing boundary conditions to some conversations in the field of study that claim environmental identity can be a better predictor of environmentally conscious behaviour. Research implications and limitations—This study postulates an integrated framework of value, beliefs and norms and the norm activation model to investigate environmentally conscious behaviour. This study findings are limited to a survey which involved an Australian sample. Practical implications—This study provides valuable implications for environmentally conscious businesses and policy makers. This study stresses the importance of highlighting climate change beliefs to enhance increased environmentally conscious behaviour engagement. It is, however, strongly recommended to focus on personal environmental norms as well because they play a stronger role in environmentally conscious behaviour engagement than climate change beliefs and strengthen climate change beliefs. This is important especially when conversations on the adverse effects of climate change and strategies to combat them are clouded by some political debates.

1. Introduction

According to the world’s largest survey of public opinion on climate change, 65% of those aged 18–35, 66% of those aged 36–59 and 58% of those over 60 widely hold climate change beliefs (CCB) [1]. At the 2021 United Nations Climate Change Conference (COP26), several countries pledged to cut carbon emissions by 50–52% below 2005 levels by the year 2030 [2]. Nevertheless, Australia, which has often been listed among the countries with higher per-capita CO2 and consumption-related carbon footprints [3], agreed only to continue its existing pledge of cutting carbon emissions by 26–28% below 2005 levels [4]. Sparking some heated debates, the Australian government still relies heavily on consumers and companies to drive its pathway to reach net zero carbon emissions [5].
Nearly two-thirds of global greenhouse gas emissions are directly or indirectly linked to human daily activities [6]. Therefore, undoubtedly, consumers should play a significant role in climate actions through minimising consumption-related carbon footprints [7,8]. However, they should be provided considerable support at the grassroot level to deal with climate change as an everyday problem among other challenges [9]. Some controversial consumer research claims that Australians still believe climate change is a political debate as opposed to an everyday problem [10,11]. Therefore, climate change may not be an issue they think about on an everyday basis.
Environmentally conscious behaviour (ECB) involves exerting an effort to minimise or avoid the adverse effects of behaviour on environmental well-being [12]. Beliefs about timing, human cause, seriousness and threat of climate change are defined as climate change beliefs (CCB) [13]. Many studies find that CCB influence ECB [14,15,16]. According to systematic reviews of previous literature, the findings are, however, subject to the complexities of understanding and believing climate change as a real problem [17,18,19].
Environmental identity (EI) has the highest predictive value in ECB above and beyond attitudes, values and beliefs [20]. Environmental identity (EI) is defined as a sense of connection to some part of the non-human natural environment [21]. It is a sense of identity that transcends an individual and places him or herself as part of a living ecosystem. For example, upon knowing about and believing the adverse effects of climate change, an individual could become an environmentally conscious person, constructing an environmental identity. How individuals position and identify themselves as environmentally conscious people also plays a significant role in ECB [22]. It can therefore be assumed that CCB can influence the EI of individuals, which then in turn can influence ECB.
Although the positive (direct) relationship between EI and ECB has been well established in previous studies [20,23], the predictive value of EI in the context of CCB has yet to be confirmed. The relationship is in fact often reported to be unclear as there could be so many pathways and intersecting relationships between the two constructs. For example, leading studies in the field often argued that personal norms, feelings of moral obligation [24,25] as well as personal environmental norms (PEN) are related to self-identity [26]. They are also well established as significant factors influencing behaviour [27,28,29]. However, as shown in recent literature, while PEN continues to be reported as the strongest predictor of ECB [30], some other studies report that a PEN does not always predict ECB [31]. Given that not many studies re-establish the connection [26], if any, between PEN and EI, further investigation seems necessary. To this end, the present study borrowed two theoretical underpinnings—values, beliefs and norms (VBN) theory [32] and the norm activation model (NAM) [25]. They are classic theories of environmental behaviour. However, integrating the two theories and extending the parameters of the construct of environmental behaviour, especially in the context of CCB, the present study intends to postulate that PEN, CCB and EI can explain ECB. It is expected that more investigations into the relationship between ECB and CCB could provide valuable insights to cut the carbon emissions in Australia with a bottom-up approach.
Based on this background, the research problem of the investigation is articulated as: what is the relationship between PEN and ECB if CCB informs EI? This paper is organised into six sections. Following this introduction section, the literature review section engages with the relevant theoretical underpinnings, and the existing scholarly conversations on the key constructs before presenting the hypotheses of the investigation. The third section details the methodological approach adopted followed by the findings of this study in the fourth section. After discussing the findings in light of previous research in the fifth discussion section, the sixth section provides concluding remarks along with the theoretical and practical implications of the investigation.

2. Literature Review

As highlighted in the introduction section, there is a research gap in understanding individuals’ beliefs about climate change and how those beliefs influence ECB. This section critically reviews relevant theoretical underpinnings, and previous literature on the key constructs. Hypotheses and the conceptual model of this study are presented at the end of the section.

Theoretical Framework and Hypotheses

Individual behaviour is usually guided by a set of internal standards [33]. They are referred to as personal norms, an individual sense of a moral obligation [24,25]. These moral obligations could be context specific and flexible; however, they usually play a significant role in guiding behaviour. For example, we may have moral obligations not to harm nature, its natural settings and rhythms through our behaviour.
Personal environmental norms are beliefs and responsibility of obligation to act in a certain way concerning the environment [34,35] and a strong predictor of ECB [30,36,37,38] as well as climate change mitigation [39].
As mentioned in the introduction, this study is informed by two theoretical underpinnings that widely guide environmental studies: values, beliefs and norms (VBN) theory [32] and the norm activation model (NAM) [25]. VBN theory proposes that individuals’ values, beliefs and norms influence the individuals’ ECB [32,33]. Researchers [26] who adopt Schwartz’s measurement scale of PEN [25,26] in their work on ECB find a significant effect of PEN on ECB. This finding is also confirmed by more recent studies [12,34,35,36]. Extending this understanding, some researchers [35] find that PEN plays a more significant role than social norms when it comes to ECB such as choosing eco-friendly travel options. On the contrary, a systematic review [37] finds that social norms have a significant effect on ECB. Some other studies [38] also find that when it comes to certain ECB such as choosing an environmentally friendly travel mode, PEN plays no significant role. Nevertheless, some other groups of researchers report that PEN is a more powerful predictor of ECB than social norms [39,40,41].
The influence of personal norms on ECB can differ among countries [42]. Conducting nine focus groups of Australians, a study [43] reports individuals’ engagement in ECB depends on four main categories of belief: awareness and acceptance of environmental need; consequences of personal action; responsibility for personal action; and acceptance of policy initiatives. According to a more recent study [44], these categories can also be related to how Australians believe in climate change, understanding the importance of personal actions in mitigating the adverse effects of climate change and their personal environmental norms.
The NAM [24,25] proposes three types of antecedents of behaviour—awareness of consequences, ascription of responsibility and personal norms. It is postulated that awareness of consequences affects personal norms through ascribed responsibility. Further, the relationship between personal norms and behaviour is moderated by both awareness of consequences and ascribed responsibility. Beliefs about timing, human cause, seriousness and threat of climate change are defined as CCB [45]. To this end, this study intends to extend the theoretical parameters of both VBN and the NAB by integrating CCB and EI.
Previous research establishes the relationship between values, personal norms and climate change actions [46]. Attitudinal surveys into climate change are also common. A European study found that personal norms influence individual climate actions [47]. Overall, many studies establish the association between personal norms and climate change actions and other ECB [48,49,50,51]. Except for a few studies establishing the connection between social norms and CCB [9], no compelling evidence on the association between PEN and CCB can be found in previous literature. Therefore, the association is considered an important area of focus in the present study. As mentioned earlier, one of the few investigations [9] on travel mode finds that utility needs (vehicle ownership), other factors (infrastructure), and social norms support individuals to use conventional travel modes rejecting scientific evidence on climate change. Another study finds that social norms and CCB mediate the relationship between a sense of community and ECB [52]. A meta review finds that unlike the established associations such as values and worldviews, CCB are less strongly associated with environmental knowledge [14]. Given that personal norms are found be a stronger predictor of ECB [30,53], it is important to investigate the relationship between PEN and CCB.
Research shows that CCB have a positive relationship with ECB [50,54] to different extents [55]. CCB explains how individuals support fossil fuel taxes in Europe [56], and public acceptance of climate change actions in China, Germany and the US [57], Colorado [58] and the Czech Republic [31]. Moreover, CCB have a positive effect on ECB among Australians [59,60].
In previous literature, PEN is often understood in relation to self-identity, which defines individual behaviour. The construct of self-identity can be understood as a perception of oneself within a certain social context. Self-identity is defined as “the self as reflexively understood by an individual in terms of her or his biography” [61], p.53. This means self-identity cannot be seen as a passive entity, rather it is an entity subject to time and space. EI is also defined as a sense of connection to some part of the non-human natural environment [21]. It can be argued that individuals are in the process of continuously and reflexively building a coherent and rewarding sense of environmental identity based on their outer environmental challenges such as climate change. Individuals not only feel responsible but empowered when dealing with complex environmental problems such as climate change problems [12]. Thus, it is argued that consumer role and identity are continuously reconstructed along with how they deal with environmental problems
As mentioned in the introduction section, environmental identity is found to have a significant relationship with ECB [22,38]. While some studies find that social identity plays a significant role in climate actions and ECB [62,63], some other studies find that (individual) environmental identity plays a more significant role in ECB than (collective) environmental identity [22]. For example, a study [64] finds that consumers’ self-identification with environmentally friendly traits is a major predictor of ECB. More often in the literature, individual factors (anxiety and psychological characteristics) appear to be stronger predictors of ECB in the context of CCB [55,65]. Although some studies find no mediation effect of politicalised environmental identity and environmental activism [66], several other studies confirm a mediation effect of environmental identity on ecotourism [67], identification with nature [68] or valuing nature [69].
The relationship between environmental values and environmental self-identity is moderated by personal norms [70]. Although self-monitoring does not play a moderating role in the relationships between social environmental norms and personal environmental norms, self-monitoring has a significant moderating effect on the relationships between personal environmental norms and ECB [71]. Environmental identity partially mediates the relationship between internalization (i.e., the importance of morality to one’s self-identity) and ECB [72]. According to another study, there is a full mediation effect of the environmentally identity of the relationship between PEN and ECB [73]. Therefore, in this study, we examined the mediation effect of EI on ECB. Overall, following the review of the theoretical frameworks and previous literature, the below hypotheses are derived:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Personal environmental norms (PEN) positively affect environmentally conscious behaviour (ECB).
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
Personal environmental norms (PEN) positively affect climate change beliefs (CCB).
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
Climate change beliefs (CCB) positively affect environmentally conscious behaviour (ECB).
Hypothesis 4 (H4).
Environmental identity (EI) has a significant relationship with environmentally conscious behaviour (ECB).
Hypothesis 5 (H5).
Climate change beliefs have a significant relationship (CCB) with environmental identity (EI).
Hypothesis 6 (H6).
Personal environmental norms (PEN) have a significant relationship with environmental identity (EI).
The conceptual framework of this study is shown in Figure 1.

3. Methodology

To test the conceptual model, an internet survey method was used to investigate the relationship between independent variables and dependent variables. The survey was designed using the Qualtrics platform and data were collected from July 2019 to August 2019, using the Dynata survey panel, which is an Australian research panel company. Individuals who had registered with Dynata received an invitation email from Dynata, inviting them to learn more about our study. Following the accompanying link took the participants to an online participant information sheet, which outlined the purpose of this study and provided a voluntary link to the questionnaire. Participants were able to check a box showing their consent to be involved in the survey before they could proceed to the questionnaire. In total, 682 respondents completed the survey. The sample is nationally (Australia) representative sample.
The questionnaire was estimated to take approximately 10–15 min for participants to fully answer all the questions. The questionnaire contained a variety of questions. These included sociodemographic questions, constructs that measured EI, PEN, CCB and ECB. The constructs that were used in this study were built primarily based on the measurements developed in previous studies including, 24 items measuring EI [21], 5 items measuring CCB [13], six items measuring ECB intentions [26] and seven items measuring PEN [24,25,26].
ECB and PEN were measured using a five-point Likert scale, with 5 being “strongly agree” and 1 being “strongly disagree”. Environmental identity was measured using a five-point scale, with 1 being “does not describe me” and 5 being “describes me extremely well”. CCB was measured using 5 individual questions. All the scales with their measurement are present in Table 1.
After cleaning the data, 118 responses were deleted from 682 responses collected due to poor response, resulting in a total of 564 usable responses to analyse. A response was considered a poor response due to one or both of the following reasons: the responses were totally random, for example straight lining answers, and the time that participants spent to complete the survey was less than two minutes.
SPSS V27 was used for cleaning and editing the data, and IBM AMOS 28 was used to conduct a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to check the quality of the measurement model. Next, via AMOS, structural equation modelling (SEM) was used to test the path model.

4. Results

Table 2 summarizes the sociodemographic background of participants of this study. The cleaned sample (n = 564) consisted of approximately an even age ratio. A total of 53.5% of the participants were female (44.9% male), and 55.5% were married (23.8% single). Regarding the education of the participants, more than 95% of participants had at least a high school education. In terms of annual income, 43.8% of the participants earned less than $19,999, 24.3% earned $20,000–$39,000, 14.9% earned $40,000–$59,999, 8.32% earned $60,000–$79,999, and 8.8% earned more than $80,000 per annum.

5. Measurement Model

Following previous studies [74], we used a two-stage structural equation modelling (SEM) approach with AMOS 28 to test the measurement model before analysing the structural model. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to test the measurement model. The initial CFA revealed several poor loaded items (standardized regression weights smaller than 0.5). These items were also cross-loaded with other items in the model. Based on the CFA analysis, some minor problems in convergent and divergent validity were observed, thus leading to the use of modification indices so as to improve the model fit adjustment. Following previous studies [75], modification indices with higher absolute values were chosen. The analysis of the indices showed that some modifications in the model specification could be made to improve the global fit indices. Moreover, all the constructs in this study are of a reflective scale and all the items in the constructs are interchangeable. Hence, overall, one item from PEN, two items from ECB, and six items from EI were removed. These modifications helped us to achieve acceptable model fit (χ2 (489) = 1761.645; χ2/df = 3.603; CFI = 0.909; RMSEA = 0.068) and the high alpha values in Table 3 show that this did not affect the measurement validly. The measurement validity of the scales is still consistent with the theoretical constructs of this study. The results of CFA suggest that the complete measurement model represents a good fit to the data. Table 3 shows the standardised factor loadings (SFLs) for each item from final CFA.
Following previous studies [76], the composite reliability (CR) and the average variance extracted (AVE) of each latent variable were calculated. All the estimates are greater than the threshold, thus providing support for internal consistency. In addition, discriminant validity was evaluated by using two different methods. First, all the inter-construct correlations were significantly less than 1 at the p = 0.001 and p = 0.1 levels, providing evidence for discriminant validity [77]. Second, discriminant validity was established, as the square root of the AVE estimates for each construct were greater than the correlation with all other constructs, providing further support for discriminant validity [76]. Moreover, the hetero-trait–mono-trait (HTMT) ratio was tested and it met the criteria for discriminant validity. Table 4 and Table 5 present a summary of this section.

6. Structural Model Results

In this study, we used a latent SEM analysis with maximum likelihood estimation to test the relationships between the constructs. The structural model contains both direct and indirect effects. The model fit of the structural model was excellent (χ2 (520) = 1856.902; χ2/df = 3.571; CFI = 0.906; RMSEA = 0.068). The analysis revealed support for all our hypotheses, as summarized in Table 6.
A mediation analysis using bootstrap 5000 and Gaskin’s (2020) plugin in AMOS software was utilized to test the mediating role of CCB and EI. Based on the results, which are reported in Table 7, all the mediations are significant. As the direct relationship between the DVs and IVs was also significant, we can conclude that we have partial mediation.

7. Discussion

This study investigates how personal environmental norms and climate change beliefs influence environmentally conscious behaviour. In consultation with existing debates in the literature, this study also investigates how the relationship between climate change beliefs and environmentally conscious behaviour is mediated by environmental identity. This section discusses the findings of the investigation in light of the findings of previous studies and the chosen theoretical insights.
This study borrows theoretical underpinnings from VBN theory [32] and NAM [24,25]. Strengthening VBN theory and disconfirming previous research [38], this study finds that PEN positively affect ECB (H1) and that they are positively related to CCB (H2). VBN postulates that values influence ECB via pro-environmental beliefs and personal norms. The present study’s findings confirm that PEN influence ECB both directly as well as via CCB. Establishing a partial mediation, CCB mediates the relationship between PEN and ECB (see Table 7).
As postulated in the NAM and confirming previous research [78], in the context of climate change, PEN strengthen the beliefs about timing, human cause, seriousness and threat of climate change and influence ECB. The NAM postulated that altruism and behaviour are influenced by moral values that are triggered by awareness of consequence, ascription of responsibility, and personal norms. Therefore, this study proposes that an integrated framework of these theoretical models could better explain ECB in the context of CCB. To this end, as shown in Figure 1, a conceptual model of the present study, the present study postulates that PEN, CCB and EI can explain ECB. This theoretical contribution of this study can guide future research in ECB.
The influence of individual factors (values, attitudes) on ECB are well established. However, previous research provides no compelling evidence on the association between PEN and CCB except for the association between social norms and CCB [9]. To this end, this study makes a significant contribution by establishing the association between PEN and CCB (H2). Social norms are values and standards of behaviour accepted by a group of people. In ECB literature, some studies report that social norms [79,80], social identity [81] and social interactions [82] influence ECB. While a meta-analysis of ECB confirms that PEN is the third powerful predictor of ECB next to attitudes and behaviour control, empirical evidence on the association between PEN and ECB, especially in the context of CCB, is rare. Presenting empirical evidence on the association, the present study makes a significant contribution to the literature.
Interestingly, this study shows than PEN is a stronger predictor of ECB than CCB (see Table 7). Previous research conducted in Australia finds that CCB have a positive effect on ECB [59,60]. Nevertheless, confirming some other research [30,53], the present study finds that PEN also have a stronger predictability than CCB in ECB. The finding sets a significant boundary condition to the existing findings in the field of study. Further, the finding may imply that the complexity of understanding climate change and the adverse effects of the political debates in Australia surrounding the effectiveness of strategies of reducing carbon emissions still influence ECB; therefore, CCB themselves do not make a significant influence on ECB like PEN do. Therefore, actions on climate change should focus both on PEN and CCB—especially, it should be noted that PEN strengthen CCB.
As detailed in the literature review section of this paper, a growing body of literature shows that EI plays a significant role in ECB [22]. Moreover, some studies report that EI is the strongest predictor of ECB [20,83]. While the present study confirms the positive relationship between EI and ECB (H4), this study also shows that both PEN (H1) and CCB (H3) apparently play a stronger role than EI in ECB. Between the two constructs, however, PEN influence EI stronger (H6) than CCB, although CCB and EI are also positively related (H5). This finding confirms previous research findings [30], by re-establishing the stronger influence of PEN in ECB. Moreover, some research in the field of environmental activism [66] finds no evidence of a mediation effect on EI, the present study finds a partial mediation between PEN and ECB and CCB and ECB.
This study also makes significant practical implications for environmentally conscious businesses and policy makers. There is a possibility that the impact of PEN can be stronger when individuals believe in CCB, although they may not have strongly established EI. Therefore, it is important to strengthen CCB to enhance individuals’ engagement in ECB. Further, although EI can play a role in ECB, influence of PEN on EI is stronger than CCB. Therefore, caution should be exercised when promoting climate change as a promotional theme with a view to strengthen EI (e.g., strengthening consumer identity through climate change-conscious product branding strategies); instead, focusing on PEN is strongly recommended especially because PEN strengthen CCB.
A few limitations of this study should be paid attention to. This study utilized a sample of respondents in Australia, a developed country where climate change is predominantly a political debate despite the economic, environmental and social issues it creates for all Australians. A study involving respondents from a more environmentally conscious country, a developing country, or a poor country where other social issues are more prominent may yield different results from the present study.

8. Conclusions

This study showed the strong positive relationships between personal environmental norms, climate change beliefs and environmentally conscious behaviour. The key findings of this study are: (1) personal environmental norms positively affect environmentally conscious behaviour, (2) personal environmental norms and climate change beliefs are positively related, (3) environmental identity and environmentally conscious behaviour are positively related, and (4) climate change beliefs play a stronger role than environmental identity in environmentally conscious behaviour. Further, this study also found that personal environmental norms influence environmental identity stronger than climate change beliefs. This study also showed how the relationship between climate change beliefs and environmentally conscious behaviour is partially mediated by environmental identity. While this study made a significant theoretical contribution by proposing an integrated theoretical framework of values, beliefs and norms theory and the norm activation model, it also discussed several practical implications for environmentally conscious businesses and policy makers. Climate change-related natural disasters (e.g., floods and bushfires) occur differently in different geographic locations in Australia. Therefore, a study which utilizes a clustering sampling technique to represent climate change beliefs among Australians in different geographic locations can be recommended as a future investigation. The findings of the present study are informed by an online survey. Some of the complexities of understanding climate change can also be better captured through a qualitative investigation.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.R.P.; methodology, H.K. and L.W.J.; software, H.K.; formal analysis, H.K. and C.R.P.; investigation, C.R.P.; data curation, H.K.; writing—original draft preparation, C.R.P.; writing—review and editing, L.W.J. and H.K.; project administration, C.R.P.; All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. United Nations Development Programme. World’s Largest Survey of Public Opinion on Climate Change: A Majority of People Call for Wide-Ranging Action. 2021. Available online: https://www.undp.org/press-releases/worlds-largest-survey-public-opinion-climate-change-majority-people-call-wide (accessed on 14 December 2021).
  2. United Nations. COP26. 2021. Available online: https://ukcop26.org (accessed on 14 December 2021).
  3. The Guardian. The Ultimate Climate Change FAQ. 2021. Available online: https://www.theguardian.com/australia-news/2021/jan/28/australia-needs-to-cut-emissions-by-at-least-50-by-2030-to-meet-paris-goals-experts-say (accessed on 14 December 2021).
  4. BBC News. Australia Resists Calls for Tougher Climate Targets. 2021. Available online: https://www.bbc.com/news/world-australia-56854558 (accessed on 14 December 2021).
  5. NBC News. Australia’s Dismal Climate Record Comes under COP26 Spotlight. 2021. Available online: https://www.cnbc.com/2021/11/26/australias-dismal-climate-record-comes-under-cop26-spotlight.html (accessed on 14 December 2021).
  6. Wang, T.; Shen, B.; Springer, C.H.; Hou, J. What prevents us from taking low-carbon actions? A comprehensive review of influencing factors affecting low-carbon behaviors. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 2021, 71, 101844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Feng, K.; Hubacek, K.; Song, K. Household carbon inequality in the US. J. Clean Prod. 2021, 278, 123994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Scrucca, F.; Barberio, G.; Fantin, V.; Porta, P.L.; Barbanera, M. Carbon footprint: Concept, methodology and calculation. In Carbon Footprint Case Studies; Springer: Singapore, 2021; pp. 1–31. [Google Scholar]
  9. Thøgersen, J. Consumer behavior and climate change: Consumers need considerable assistance. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2021, 42, 9–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Barber, G.; Klassen, A. Climate change, the Australian Greens, and dynamics of party competition across five national elections in Australia. Aust. J. Polit. Sci. 2021, 56, 56–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Copland, S. Anti-politics and global climate inaction: The case of the Australian carbon tax. Crit. Sociol. 2020, 46, 623–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Connolly, J.; Prothero, A. Green Consumption: Life-politics, risk and contradictions. J. Consum. Cult. 2008, 8, 117–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. McCright, A.M.; Dunlap, R.E.; Xiao, C. Perceived scientific agreement and support for government action on climate change in the USA. Clim. Change 2013, 119, 511–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Hornsey, M.J.; Harris, E.A.; Bain, P.G.; Fielding, K.S. Meta-analyses of the determinants and outcomes of belief in climate change. Nat. Clim. Change 2016, 6, 622–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Perera, L.C.R.; Hewege, C.R. Climate change risk perceptions and environmentally conscious behaviour among young environmentalists in Australia. Young Consum. 2013, 14, 139–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Zawadzki, S.J.; Bouman, T.; Steg, L.; Bojarskich, V.; Druen, P.B. Translating climate beliefs into action in a changing political landscape. Clim. Change 2020, 161, 21–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Goodman, M.K.; Doyle, J.; Farrell, N. Practising everyday climate cultures: Understanding the cultural politics of climate change. Clim. Change 2020, 163, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Hügel, S.; Davies, A.R. Public participation, engagement, and climate change adaptation: A re-view of the research literature. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Clim. Change 2020, 11, e645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Rousell, D.; Cutter-Mackenzie-Knowles, A. A systematic review of climate change education: Giving children and young people a ‘voice’and a ‘hand’in redressing climate change. Child Geogr. 2020, 18, 191–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Schwartz, D.; Loewenstein, G.; Agüero-Gaete, L. Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour through green identity labelling. Nat. Sustain. 2020, 3, 746–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Clayton, S.; Optow, S. Environmental identity: A conceptual and an operational definition. In Identity and the Natural Environment: The Psychological Significance of Nature; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2003; pp. 45–65. [Google Scholar]
  22. Wang, X.; van der, W.E.; Bouman, T.; Harder, M.K.; Steg, L. I am vs. we are: How biospheric values and environmental identity of individuals and groups can influence pro-environmental behaviour. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 8956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Ateş, H. Understanding students’ and science educators’ eco-labeled food purchase behaviors: Ex-tension of theory of planned behavior with self-identity, personal norm, willingness to pay, and eco-label knowledge. Ecol. Food Nutr. 2021, 60, 454–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Schwartz, S.H. Normative explanations of helping behavior: A critique, proposal, and empirical test. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1973, 9, 349–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Schwartz, S.H. Normative influences on altruism. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1977, 10, 221–279. [Google Scholar]
  26. Minton, A.P.; Rose, R.L. The effects of environmental concern on environmentally friendly con-sumer behavior: An exploratory study. J. Bus. Res. 1997, 40, 37–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Berkowitz, L. Social norms, feelings, and other factors affecting helping and altruism. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1972; Volume 6, pp. 63–108. [Google Scholar]
  28. Fishbein, M.; Ajzen, I. Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and re-search. Philos. Rhetor. 1975, 10, 177–189. [Google Scholar]
  29. Triandis, H.C. Theoretical framework for evaluation of cross-cultural training effectiveness. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 1977, 1, 19–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Esfandiar, K.; Dowling, R.; Pearce, J.; Goh, E. Personal norms and the adoption of pro-environmental binning behaviour in national parks: An integrated structural model ap-proach. J. Sustain. Tour. 2020, 28, 10–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Krkoška Lorencová, E.; Loučková, B.; Vačkářů, D. Perception of climate change risk and adaptation in the Czech Republic. Climate 2019, 7, 61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T.; Abel, T.; Guagnano, G.A.; Kalof, L. A value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: The case of environmentalism. Hum. Ecol. Rev. 1999, 6, 81–98. [Google Scholar]
  33. Stern, P.C. Psychology and the science of human-environment interactions. Am. Psychol. 2000, 55, 523–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. De Groot, J.I.M.; Bondy, K.; Schuitema, G. Listen to others or yourself? The role of personal norms on the effectiveness of social norm interventions to change pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2021, 78, 101688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Doran, R.; Larsen, S. The relative importance of social and personal norms in explaining intentions to choose eco-friendly travel options. Int. J. Tour. Res. 2016, 18, 159–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Reinders, M.J.; Onwezen, M.C.; Meeusen, M.J.G. Can bio-based attributes upgrade a brand? How partial and full use of bio-based materials affects the purchase intention of brands. J. Clean Prod. 2017, 162, 1169–1179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Farrow, K.; Grolleau, G.; Ibanez, L. Social norms and pro-environmental behavior: A review of the evidence. Ecol. Econ. 2017, 140, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Zavareh, M.F.; Mehdizadeh, M.; Nordfjærn, T. Active travel as a pro-environmental behaviour: An integrated framework. Transp. Res. Part D Transp. Environ. 2020, 84, 102356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Gao, L.; Wang, S.; Li, J.; Li, H. Application of the extended theory of planned behavior to understand individual’s energy saving behavior in workplaces. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2017, 127, 107–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Lobo, A.; Greenland, S. The influence of cultural values on green purchase behaviour. Mark Intell. Plan. 2017, 35, 377–396. [Google Scholar]
  41. Prakash, G.; Pathak, P. Intention to buy eco-friendly packaged products among young consumers of India: A study on developing nation. J. Clean Prod. 2017, 141, 385–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Robles-Avila, S.E. The role of environmental personal norms in the VBN framework: Testing the differences between the United States and Mexico regarding the disposal of potentially harmful products. J. Int. Consum. Mark. 2021, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Blamey, R. Contingent valuation and the activation of environmental norms. Ecol. Econ. 1998, 24, 47–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Brosch, T. Affect and emotions as drivers of climate change perception and action: A review. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2021, 42, 15–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. McCright, A.M.; Dunlap, R.E.; Xiao, C. Increasing influence of party identification on perceived scientific agreement and support for government action on climate change in the United States, 2006–2012. Weather Clim. Soc. 2014, 6, 194–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Kwon, S.-A.; Kim, S.; Lee, J.E. Analyzing the determinants of individual action on climate change by specifying the roles of six values in South Korea. Sustainability 2019, 11, 1834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Reichl, J.; Cohen, J.J.; Klöckner, C.A.; Kollmann, A.; Azarova, V. The drivers of individual climate actions in Europe. Glob. Environ. Change 2021, 71, 102390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Brick, C.; Bosshard, A.; Whitmarsh, L. Motivation and climate change: A review. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2021, 42, 82–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Whitmarsh, L.; Seyfang, G.; O’Neill, S. Public engagement with carbon and climate change: To what extent is the public carbon capable’? Global Environ. Change 2011, 21, 56–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Cunningham, G.; McCullough, B.P.; Hohensee, S. Physical activity and climate change attitudes. Clim. Change 2020, 159, 61–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Ünal, A.B.; Steg, L.; Gorsira, M. Values versus environmental knowledge as triggers of a process of activation of personal norms for eco-driving. Environ. Behav. 2018, 50, 1092–1118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Smith, C.J.; Dupré, K.E.; McEvoy, A.; Kenny, S. Community perceptions and pro-environmental behavior: The mediating roles of social norms and climate change risk. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 2021, 53, 200–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Sharma, R.; Gupta, A. Pro-environmental behaviour among tourists visiting national parks: Ap-plication of value-belief-norm theory in an emerging economy context. Asia Pac. J. Tour Res. 2020, 25, 829–840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Jessani, Z.; Harris, P.B. Personality, politics, and denial: Tolerance of ambiguity, political orien-tation and disbelief in climate change. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2018, 131, 121–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Stollberg, J.; Jonas, E. Existential threat as a challenge for individual and collective engagement: Climate change and the motivation to act. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2021, 42, 145–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Fairbrother, M.; Sevä, I.J.; Kulin, J. Political trust and the relationship between climate change beliefs and support for fossil fuel taxes: Evidence from a survey of 23 European countries. Glob. Environ. Change 2019, 59, 102003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Schwirplies, C. Citizens’ acceptance of climate change adaptation and mitigation: A survey in China, Germany, and the US. Ecol. Econ. 2018, 145, 308–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Shepard, S.; Boudet, H.; Zanocco, C.M.; Cramer, L.A.; Tilt, B. Community climate change beliefs, awareness, and actions in the wake of the September 2013 flooding in Boulder County, Col-orado. J. Environ. Stud. Sci. 2018, 8, 312–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Beattie, G.; Han, Y.; La Nauze, A. Conservation Spillovers: The Effect of Rooftop Solar on Climate Change Beliefs. Environ. Resour. Econ. 2019, 74, 1425–1451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Muroi, S.K.; Bertone, E. From thoughts to actions: The importance of climate change education in enhancing students’ self-efficacy. Aust. J. Environ. Educ. 2019, 35, 123–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Giddens, A. Modernity and Self-Identity: Self and Society in the Late Modern age; Stanford Univ Press: Redwood City, CA, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  62. Mackay, C.M.L.; Cristoffanini, F.; Wright, J.D.; Neufeld, S.D.; Ogawa, H.F.; Schmitt, M.T. Connection to nature and environmental activism: Politicized environmental identity mediates a relationship between identification with nature and observed environmental activist behaviour. Curr. Res. Ecol. Soc. Psychol. 2021, 2, 100009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Masson, T.; Fritsche, I. We need climate change mitigation and climate change mitigation needs the ‘We’: A state-of-the-art review of social identity effects motivating climate change action. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2021, 42, 89–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Khare, A. Antecedents to green buying behaviour: A study on consumers in an emerging economy. Mark. Intell. Plan. 2015, 33, 309–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Klas, A.; Clarke, E.J.R. The role of psychological variables in developing effective climate change message frames. In Research Handbook on Communicating Climate Change; Edward Elgar Publishing: Cheltenham, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  66. Schmitt, M.T.; Mackay, C.M.L.; Droogendyk, L.M.; Payne, D. What predicts environmental ac-tivism? The roles of identification with nature and politicized environmental identity. J. Environ. Psychol. 2019, 61, 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Teeroovengadum, V. Environmental identity and ecotourism behaviours: Examination of the direct and indirect effects. Tour. Rev. 2019, 74, 280–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Mackay, C.M.L.; Schmitt, M.T.; Lutz, A.E.; Mendel, J. Recent developments in the social identity approach to the psychology of climate change. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2021, 42, 95–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Diessner, R.; Genthôs, R.; Praest, K.; Pohling, R. Identifying with nature mediates the influence of valuing nature’s beauty on proenvironmental behaviors. Ecopsychology 2018, 10, 97–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Balundė, A.; Perlaviciute, G.; Truskauskaitė-Kunevičienė, I. Sustainability in youth: Environmental considerations in adolescence and their relationship to pro-environmental behavior. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 2985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Park, S.-Y.; Sohn, S.H. Exploring the normative influences of social norms on individual envi-ronmental behavior. J. Glob. Sch. Mark. Sci. 2012, 22, 183–194. [Google Scholar]
  72. Abbas, M.; Bashir, F. Having a green identity: Does pro-environmental self-identity mediate the effects of moral identity on ethical consumption and pro-environmental behaviour? Stud. Psychol. 2020, 41, 612–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Gatersleben, B.; Murtagh, N.; Abrahamse, W. Values, identity and pro-environmental behaviour. J. Acad. Soc. Sci. 2014, 9, 374–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Anderson, J.C.; Gerbing, D.W. Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recom-mended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 1988, 103, 411–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Duarte, P.; e Silva, S.C.; Ferreira, M.B. How convenient is it? Delivering online shopping con-venience to enhance customer satisfaction and encourage e-WOM. J. Retail Consum. Serv. 2018, 44, 161–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Fornell, C.; Larcker, D.F. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 18, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Bagozzi, R.P.; Yi, Y. On the evaluation of structural equation models. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 1988, 16, 74–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Zhang, L.; Ruiz-Menjivar, J.; Luo, B.; Liang, Z.; Swisher, M.E. Predicting climate change mitigation and adaptation behaviors in agricultural production: A comparison of the theory of planned behavior and the Value-Belief-Norm Theory. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 68, 101408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Culiberg, B.; Elgaaied-Gambier, L. Going green to fit in–understanding the impact of social norms on pro-environmental behaviour, a cross-cultural approach. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2016, 40, 179–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Perry, G.L.; Richardson, S.J.; Harré, N.; Hodges, D.; Lyver, P.O.B.; Maseyk, F.J.; Taylor, R.; Todd, J.H.; Tylianakis, J.M.; Yletyinen, J.; et al. Evaluating the role of social norms in fostering pro-environmental behaviors. Front. Environ. Sci. 2021, 9, 160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Fritsche, I.; Barth, M.; Jugert, P.; Masson, T.; Reese, G. A social identity model of pro-environmental action (SIMPEA). Psychol. Rev. 2018, 125, 245–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Li, Q.; Wu, M. Tourists’ pro-environmental behaviour in travel destinations: Benchmarking the power of social interaction and individual attitude. J. Sustain. Tour. 2020, 28, 1371–1389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Whitmarsh, L.; O’Neill, S. Green identity, green living? The role of pro-environmental self-identity in determining consistency across diverse pro-environmental behaviours. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 305–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Conceptual model of this study.
Figure 1. Conceptual model of this study.
Sustainability 14 01824 g001
Table 1. Measures of the scales in the model.
Table 1. Measures of the scales in the model.
VariableSurvey Items Scale
ECBI would be willing to sign a petition to support an environmental cause. (ECB_1) 5-point scale: (1) Strongly Disagree … (5) Strongly Agree
I would consider joining a group or club which is concerned with the environment. (ECB_2)
I would be willing to pay more taxes to support greater government control of pollution. (ECB_3)
I would be willing to pay more each month for electricity if it meant cleaner air. (ECB_4)
I would be willing to stop buying products from companies guilty of polluting the environment even though it might be inconvenient for me. (ECB_5)
I would be willing to make personal sacrifices for the sake of slowing down pollution even though the immediate results may not seem significant. (ECB_6)
PENBuy environmentally friendly products for your household? (PEN_1)
Recycle household waste? (PEN_2)
Pay attention to advertisements about products which are safe for the environment? (PEN_3)
Buy products made with recycled ingredient? (PEN_4)
Buy larger size products in order to reduce waste? (PEN_5)
Do whatever you can to help improve the environment? (PEN_6)
Buy products made by companies known for being environmentally responsible? (PEN_7)
CCB Which of the following statements reflects your view of when the effects of global warming will begin to happen: (CCB_1) 5-point scale: (1) ‘they have already begun to happen’, (0.75) ‘they will start happening within a few years’, (0.5) ‘they will start happening within your lifetime,’ (0.25) ‘they will not happen within your lifetime, but they will affect future generations’, (0) or they will never happen’
From what you have heard or read, do you believe increases in the Earth’s temperature over the last century are due more to: (CCB_2) (1) ‘the effects of pollution from human activities’ or (0) natural changes in the environment that are not due to human activities’
I’m going to read you a list of environmental problems. How much do you personally worry about Global arming? (CCB_3) 4-point scale: (1) ‘a great deal’, (0.67)
‘a fair amount’, (0.33) ‘only a little’,
or (0) ‘not at all’
‘Do you think that global warming will pose a serious threat to you or your way of life in your lifetime? (CCB_4) 0 = no, 0.5 = Maybe, 1 = yes
‘Thinking about what is said in the news, in your view is the seriousness of global warming (CCB_5) (0) ‘generally exaggerated’, (0.5) ‘generally correct’, or is it (1) ‘generally underestimated’
EI I spend a lot of time in natural settings (woods, mountains, desert, lakes, and ocean). (EI_1) 5-point scale: (1) Does not describe me … (5) Describes me extremely well
Engaging in environmental behaviours is important to me. (EI_2)
I think of myself as a part of nature, not separate from it. (EI_3)
If I had enough time or money, I would certainly devote some of it to working for environmental causes. (EI_4)
When I am upset or stressed, I can feel better by spending some time outdoors “communing with nature”. (EI_5)
Living near wildlife is important to me; I would not want to live in a city all the time. (EI_6)
I have a lot in common with environmentalists as a group. (EI_7)
I believe that some of today’s social problems could be cured by returning to a more rural lifestyle in which people live in harmony with the land. (EI_8)
I feel that I have a lot in common with other species. (EI_9)
I like to garden. (EI_10)
Being a part of the ecosystem is an important part of who I am. (EI_11)
I feel that I have roots to a particular geographical location that had a significant impact on my development. (EI_12)
Behaving responsibly toward the earth—living a sustainable lifestyle—is part of my moral code. (EI_13)
Learning about the natural world should be an important part of every child’s upbringing. (EI_14)
In general, being part of the natural world is an important part of my self—image. (EI_15)
I would rather live in a small room or house with a nice view than a bigger room or house with a view of other buildings. (EI_16)
I really enjoy camping and hiking outdoors. (EI_17)
Sometimes I feel like parts of nature—certain trees, or storms, or mountains—have a personality of their own. (EI_18)
I would feel that an important part of my life was missing if I was not able to get out and enjoy nature from time to time. (EI_19)
I take pride in the fact that I could survive outdoors on my own for a few days. (EI_20)
I have never seen a work of art that is as beautiful as a work of nature, like a sunset or a mountain range. (EI_21)
My own interests usually seem to coincide with the position advocated by environmentalists. (EI_22)
I feel that I receive spiritual sustenance from experiences with nature. (EI_23)
I keep mementos from the outdoors in my room, like shells or rocks or feathers. (EI_24)
Table 2. Respondents’ sociodemographic background.
Table 2. Respondents’ sociodemographic background.
Variables Percent (%) Percent (%)
Age Income
18–246.7 $0–$25,00019.3
25–3415.4 $25,001–$50,00024.3
35–4415.4 $50,001–$75,00019.1
44–5419 $75,001–$100,00010.8
55–6415.2 $100,001–$125,0005.9
65 and above21.1 $125,001–$150,0004.1
Marital $150,001–$175,0001.1
Single23.8 $175,001–$200,0001.6
Married55.5 $200,001+1.8
Separated2.8Education
Divorced8.7 Less than High School3.7
Widowed4.4 High School/GED28.2
Never Married3 Some College23
2 Year College Degree7.4
Gender 4 Year College Degree23
Male44.9 Master’s degree9.2
Female53.5 Doctoral Degree1.4
Professional Degree (JD, MD)2.1
Table 3. Standardised factor loadings and Cronbach’s α values.
Table 3. Standardised factor loadings and Cronbach’s α values.
VariableITEMSSFLsαVariableITEMSSFLsα
EI 0.959ECB 0.89
EI20.766 ECB_20.768
EI30.812 ECB_30.872
EI40.771 ECB_40.845
EI60.718 ECB_60.801
EI80.761 PEN 0.924
EI90.795 PEN_10.873
EI100.61 PEN_30.805
EI110.865 PEN_40.869
EI120.758 PEN_50.684
EI150.854 PEN_60.785
EI170.643 PEN_70.905
EI180.795 CCB 0.864
EI190.725 CCB_10.743
EI200.676 CCB_20.694
EI210.64 CCB_30.812
EI220.819 CCB_40.735
EI230.813 CCB_50.745
EI240.704
Table 4. Means, standard deviations, correlations, AVEs and discriminant validity.
Table 4. Means, standard deviations, correlations, AVEs and discriminant validity.
ConstructsCRAVEEIPENCCB ECB
EI0.9590.570.755
PEN0.9260.6780.517 ***0.823
CCB 0.8630.5580.418 ***0.461 ***0.747
ECB0.8930.6770.521 ***0.734 ***0.622 ***0.823
Significant at *** p < 0.001.
Table 5. HTMT criterion for discriminant validity.
Table 5. HTMT criterion for discriminant validity.
ConstructsEIPENCCB ECB
EI
PEN0.515
CCB0.3880.448
ECB0.5250.7570.619
Table 6. Result of structural equation analyses.
Table 6. Result of structural equation analyses.
Hypothesised Relationshipsβ SEt-ValueResult
H1PEN→ECB0.5230.03911.769 ***Supported
H2PEN→CCB0.4610.0279.795 ***Supported
H3CCB→ECB0.3350.0658.007 ***Supported
H4EI→ECB0.110.0462.932 ***Supported
H5CCB→EI0.2280.0594.864 ***Supported
H6PEN→EI0.4130.0338.807 ***Supported
Significant at *** p < 0.001 (two-tailed test); β, standardised path coefficients; SE, standard error.
Table 7. Mediation analysis.
Table 7. Mediation analysis.
Indirect PathStandardised EstimateLowerUpperResult
PEN --> CCB --> EI0.105 ***0.0470.11Supported
PEN --> CCB --> ECB0.155 ***0.1040.174Supported
PEN --> EI --> ECB0.046 **0.0160.07Supported
CCB --> EI --> ECB0.025 **0.0160.074Supported
Significant at *** p < 0.001; ** p < 0.01 (two-tailed test).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Perera, C.R.; Kalantari, H.; Johnson, L.W. Climate Change Beliefs, Personal Environmental Norms and Environmentally Conscious Behaviour Intention. Sustainability 2022, 14, 1824. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14031824

AMA Style

Perera CR, Kalantari H, Johnson LW. Climate Change Beliefs, Personal Environmental Norms and Environmentally Conscious Behaviour Intention. Sustainability. 2022; 14(3):1824. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14031824

Chicago/Turabian Style

Perera, Chamila R., Hassan Kalantari, and Lester W. Johnson. 2022. "Climate Change Beliefs, Personal Environmental Norms and Environmentally Conscious Behaviour Intention" Sustainability 14, no. 3: 1824. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14031824

APA Style

Perera, C. R., Kalantari, H., & Johnson, L. W. (2022). Climate Change Beliefs, Personal Environmental Norms and Environmentally Conscious Behaviour Intention. Sustainability, 14(3), 1824. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14031824

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop