Closing the Gap: The Tripartite Structure of Sustainability as a Tool for Sustainable Education—A Systematic Literature Review
Abstract
:1. How Can We Close the Gap?
2. Theory
2.1. Ego Development Theory, Domain Theory and Research on Automatisms
- People’s stage of development influences what they notice and can become aware of and, therefore, what they can describe, articulate, cultivate, influence, and change.
- All stage descriptions are idealizations that no human being fits entirely.
- As healthy development unfolds, autonomy, freedom, tolerance for difference, and ambiguity, as well as flexibility, self-awareness, and skill in interacting with the environment, increase while defenses decrease.
- Later stages are reached only by journeying through the earlier stages. Once a stage has been traversed, it remains a part of the individual’s response repertoire, even when more complex, later stages are adopted as primary lenses to look at experience.
- A person who has reached a later stage can understand earlier world views, but a person at an earlier stage cannot understand the later ones.
2.2. The Tripartite Structure of Sustainability (TSS)
- The reason to focus on the self in one’s action is (implicitly) to maintain one’s own security, power, or face (see Schwartz’s values of self-enhancement [35]). The goal is to optimize one’s own opportunities. The underlying paradigm is mechanistic, rational, and competitive. There is hardly width in awareness; the focus is narrow and restricted to the person themselves.
- In the social focus, the action of a person is informed by the (implicit) desire to belong to a group and to uphold this group’s well-being. Awareness is as whole is needed to include close “others”, usually from their direct social environment or, for example, their own party or nation. Individuals orient themselves towards the values they interpret as underlying this group’s actions.
- The term “self-transcendent” originates from the Schwartz value scale and includes values such as benevolence, humility, and universalism. The concept of a self-transcendent focus suggests that individuals see themselves as part of something larger than their own self or group. They possess a broad awareness that encompasses not only all humans across time but may also extend to the non-human world. Their actions are motivated by a desire to promote the well-being of this broader community. This perspective encourages a sense of connectedness and responsibility towards the welfare of others, transcending personal or immediate group interests.
- Stable: An action is performed primarily due to the values and attitudes of a person or group, which is the deep leverage point of intent according to Meadow’s model [13]. The underpinning values, goals, and world views of actors shape the emergent direction to which a system is oriented towards.
- Situational: An action is performed primarily due to an occurrence on the outside, that is usually manipulable, closely interwoven with the action and recognized as important. In Meadow’s model, this situational activation is a shallow parameter similar to materials or feedback, defined as interactions between elements within a system that drive internal dynamics.
- Automated: An action is performed primarily because of a programmed system inside a person, like psychological mechanisms, biases, emotions, habits, or scripts. The mechanism itself goes mostly unrecognized, but is similar for all humans. The mechanism itself is not changeable, whereas the trigger and outplay are inter- and intra-individually different. Situational activation and automatisms are closely interwoven because situations outside a person are “picked up” by mechanisms inside—like aroused emotions of a student because of very strict recycling regulations. Regarding the higher cost of a sustainable product, a person might react to the higher price with loss aversion. A higher cost is seen as a situational factor and loss aversion as an automatism in this case. In Meadow’s model, automatisms can be seen as deep leverage points in the human system, such as a design or structure in the background that manages feedback and parameters.
2.3. Research Questions
- Is the TSS as a model able to fathom all empirical valid factors influencing the inner–outer gap that we find in the literature?
- Is the TSS and the directional assumption it contains supported (by the direction of the factors’ effects—sustainable or unsustainable)?
3. Methods
- Empirical data were generated within the reviewed article itself in an empirical design that meets scientific standards.
- A factor that was not tested in the research itself was shown to be supported by at least three empirical studies. To find out whether the cited research was empirical, we read the titles and abstracts of the cited studies. We considered three articles to be sufficient to eliminate the risk of false-positive factors (because of, for example, problematic research designs or a misunderstanding of citations).
- The cited empirical source was a meta-analysis.
4. Results
5. Discussion
- Bayesian reasoning: anticipating the likelihood of future events via an internal model constructed from past situations [44].
- Inductive inference: extracting meaningful mental representations from sparse data, which may be too sparse to derive a valid representation [44].
- Availability bias: the inclination to “draw on readily available information that is easily accessible in memory and springs to mind quickly, […], especially personal anecdotes of family/friends, customer testimonials, and recent, frequent, vivid, salient, emotive or concrete examples” [8] (p.1388).
- Occam’s razor: preference for simple over complex explanations [44].
- Individuals act sustainably if they have a stable self-transcendent focus and not too many situational or automated factors point towards a focus on the self/an unsustainable or unethical surrounding.
- When individuals have a stable social focus, they act sustainably if the social surrounding they are orientated towards behaves ethically/sustainably and if not too many situational or automated factors point towards a focus on the self.
- With a stable focus on the self, individuals may (inconsistently and for a short amount of time) act sustainably if the situational surroundings (like laws, rewards, etc.) are manipulated to steer their automatisms accordingly.
6. Conclusions
7. Consequences for Educators
- Avoid what attracts individuals into focusing only on themselves (stable, situational, or automated).
- When creating educational surroundings, focus on establishing ethical and sustainable environments (situational factor). Also, support social automatisms and stable social factors that point towards sustainability.
- Try to support the development of self-transcendent foci in individuals (stable, situational, or automated), but bear in mind that later paradigms cannot be understood in earlier stages of ego development: everyone would have to be met where they are in terms of their inner development.
- As can be seen in Table 1, we think that a part of what draws people into a focus on the self are the self-enhancement values of achievement, power, face, and hedonism and also the emotion fear. In our opinion, the European school system that was spread around the world as a result of colonization supports these values and this emotion in students [55]. To change this seems as important as letting students become aware of the automatisms that lead to a focus on the self. What poses a problem is the fact that automatisms usually go unrecognized (see the discussion on exceptions). They therefore elude both research and reflection. More research is needed on automatisms and what gives them direction toward a focus. Neuroimaging might be a way to perform research on automatisms, as some interesting findings show [56,57]. Also, creative ways must be found to work with them in education to make people aware of their automatisms. Only then, we believe, can individuals work on escaping the pull of automatisms that lead to self-focus and unsustainable behaviors such as fear, loss aversion, or the desire for extrinsic rewards. It might have an effect if educators make students sensitive to the un- or subconscious parts of their decision-making process and help them to become aware of automatisms like habits and scripts and their influence on themselves and the world. For the educational handling of automatisms, neuroeducational findings and techniques also offer promising avenues. Although a gap or transfer deficit in translating neuroscientific insights into educational practice still exists, transdisciplinary collaborations between neuroscience and educational research and practitioners appear promising [58,59,60]. Neuroeducational insights highlight, for example, the importance of promoting executive functions (e.g., working memory, inhibition, cognitive flexibility) to decelerate, make conscious, and regulate automated decision-making processes: Executive functions (EFs) “enable the individual to evaluate or judge their position in relation to others, to the group and the social system and to act according to this evaluation. Therefore, the EFs are not only relevant for cognitive performance but also for self-regulation and behavior, and for social and emotional functioning. They are indispensable for personal growth over the period of childhood and adolescence” [58], p. 10.
- As all educational institutions are at the same time social surroundings of the students who work and live in it, educational stakeholders have a lot of influence if they create a sustainable social environment in their institutions. We consider ethic-orientated contexts and leaders, positive role models, information about positive social norms, trustable information and informants, the provision of examples, and everything else that was found in Table 1 as helpful instruments. At the same time, educators should avoid what was found to hinder sustainable behavior having a social focus like the Boomerang or the Free-Riding Effect.
- When trying to help individuals towards a stable self-transcendent focus/the new paradigm, it has to be kept in mind that this is a later stage of the developmental process (see “theory” section). In our opinion, reaching the goal of the new paradigm in sustainability (see instruction) would necessitate prioritizing vertical development against the (also needed) acquisition of competencies in education. Ego development research does not see competencies as contributing to the transition into a new paradigm [27]. There are some notions that are important when focusing on vertical development: Chandler and colleagues showed that educational programs are only useful if they are designed for the next stage a person will reach or the stage after [61]. Designs for earlier, actual or later stages do not help in development. So, firstly, supporting vertical development would necessitate knowing the center of gravity a person has. Their stage could be measured with the robust Washington Sentence Stem Test [24]. Leverage points into the next center of gravity seem to be consciously and systematically dealing with the thinking, doing and being of the next stage [61], which would have to be considered in the design. Two additional assumptions of ego development theory play a role and would need to be reflected upon [27] (p. 2f):
- “While vertical development can be invited and the environment optimally structured towards growth, it cannot be forced. People have the right to be who they are at any station in life”.
- “Development occurs through the interplay between person and environment, not just by one or the other. It is a potential and can be encouraged and facilitated by appropriate support and challenge, but it cannot be guaranteed”.
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Armstrong McKay, D.I.; Staal, A.; Abrams, J.F.; Winkelmann, R.; Sakschewski, B.; Loriani, S.; Fetzer, I.; Cornell, S.E.; Rockström, J.; Lenton, T.M. Exceeding 1.5 °C global warming could trigger multiple climate tipping points. Science 2022, 377, eabn7950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Steffen, W.; Rockström, J.; Richardson, K.; Lenton, T.M.; Folke, C.; Liverman, D.; Summerhayes, C.P.; Barnosky, A.D.; Cornell, S.E.; Crucifix, M.; et al. Trajectories of the Earth System in the Anthropocene. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, 8252–8259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dewey, J. America’s Public Philosopher: Essays on Social Justice, Economics, Education, and the Future of Democracy; Columbia University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2021; ISBN 9780231552882. [Google Scholar]
- Meyer, B.E. The Tripartite Structure of Sustainability: A new educational approach to bridge the gap to wise and sustainable action. Front. Educ. 2023, 8, 1224303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, M. Understanding Pro-Environmental Behavior: Models and Messages. In Strategic Communication for Sustainable Organizations; Allen, M., Ed.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 105–137. ISBN 978-3-319-18004-5. [Google Scholar]
- Binder, M.; Blankenberg, A.-K. Green lifestyles and subjective well-being: More about self-image than actual behavior? J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2017, 137, 304–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flynn, R.; Bellaby, P.; Ricci, M. The ‘Value-Action Gap’ in Public Attitudes towards Sustaina-ble Energy: The Case of Hydrogen Energy. Sociol. Rev. 2009, 57, 159–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frederiks, E.R.; Stenner, K.; Hobman, E.V. Household energy use: Applying behavioural eco-nomics to understand consumer decision-making and behaviour. Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 2015, 41, 1385–1394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gao, J.; Wang, J.; Wang, J. The Impact of Pro-environmental Preference on Consumers’ Perceived Well-being: The Mediating Role of Self-Determination Need Satisfaction. Sustainability 2020, 12, 436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, D.; Zhao, L.; Ma, S.; Shao, S.; Zhang, L. What influences an individual’s pro-environmental behavior? A literature review. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2019, 146, 28–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moser, S.; Kleinhückelkotten, S. Good Intents, but Low Impacts: Diverging Importance of Motivational and Socioeconomic Determinants Explaining Pro-Environmental Behavior, Energy Use, and Carbon Footprint. Environ. Behav. 2018, 50, 626–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Osunmuyiwa, O.O.; Payne, S.R.; Vigneswara Ilavarasan, P.; Peacock, A.D.; Jenkins, D.P. I cannot live without air conditioning! The role of identity, values and situational factors on cooling consumption patterns in India. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 2020, 69, 101634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meadows, D. Leverage Points: Places to Intervene in a System. Available online: https://donellameadows.org/archives/leverage-points-places-to-intervene-in-a-system/ (accessed on 12 March 2024).
- Woiwode, C.; Schäpke, N.; Bina, O.; Veciana, S.; Kunze, I.; Parodi, O.; Schweizer-Ries, P.; Wamsler, C. Inner transformation to sustainability as a deep leverage point: Fostering new avenues for change through dialogue and reflection. Sustain. Sci. 2021, 16, 841–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Artmann, M. Human-nature resonance in times of social-ecological crisis—A relational account for sustainability transformation. Ecosyst. People 2023, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Riechers, M.; Balázsi, Á.; García-Llorente, M.; Loos, J. Human-nature connectedness as leverage point. Ecosyst. People 2021, 17, 215–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wamsler, C. Mind the gap: The role of mindfulness in adapting to increasing risk and climate change. Sustain. Sci. 2018, 13, 1121–1135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Taylor, B.; Wright, J.; LeVasseur, T. Dark green humility: Religious, psychological, and affective attributes of proenvironmental behaviors. J. Environ. Stud. Sci. 2020, 10, 41–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wamsler, C.; Osberg, G.; Osika, W.; Herndersson, H.; Mundaca, L. Linking internal and ex-ternal transformation for sustainability and climate action: Towards a new research and policy agenda. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2021, 71, 102373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ives, C.D.; Freeth, R.; Fischer, J. Inside-out sustainability: The neglect of inner worlds. Ambio 2020, 49, 208–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Böhme, J.; Walsh, Z.; Wamsler, C. Sustainable lifestyles: Towards a relational approach. Sustain. Sci. 2022, 17, 2063–2076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cook-Greuter, S. Postautonomous Ego Development: A Study of Its Nature and Measurement. Doctoral Dissertation, Harvard University, New Haven, CT, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Bennett, N.; Lemoine, G.J. What a difference a word makes: Understanding threats to performance in a VUCA world. Bus. Horiz. 2014, 57, 311–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murray, T. Sentence Completion Assessments for Ego Development, Meaning-Making, and Wisdom Maturity, Including STAGES. 2017, p. 17. Available online: https://integralleadershipreview.com/15642-sentence-completion-assessments-for-ego-development-meaning-making-and-wisdom-maturity-including-stages-overview-and-summary-this-article-began-as-a-series-of-short-white-papers-providing-various/ (accessed on 20 April 2024).
- Binder, T. Ego Development for Effective Coaching and Consulting: Including a Comprehensive Overview of Ego Development Theory, Its Validation, Critique and Empirical Foundations; Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht: Göttingen, Germany, 2023; ISBN 9783647400051. [Google Scholar]
- Loevinger, J. Ego Development: Conceptions and Theories; Jossey-Bass: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Cook-Greuter, S. Nine Levels of Increasing Embrace in Ego Development: A Full-Spectrum Theory of Vertical Growth and Meaning Making; Prepublication version. Adapted and expanded from S. Cook-Greuter (1985). A detailed description of the successive stages of ego-development. Revised December 2013; 2013; pp. 1–95. Available online: https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Nine-Levels-Of-Increasing-Embrace-In-Ego-%3A-A-Theory-Cook-Greuter/cc0e81e8aaf82e6ec4faccbc3ed9889fe0cd2bb7 (accessed on 20 April 2024).
- Schubarth, W.; Speck, K. Employability und Praxisbezüge im wissenschaftlichen Studium, 2009/September. Available online: https://www.hrk-nexus.de/uploads/media/2013_HRK__10-07-2013_Schubarth_Speck_final.pdf (accessed on 20 April 2024).
- Bursik, K.; Martin, T.A. Ego Development and Adolescent Academic Achievement. J. Res. Adolesc. 2006, 16, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ivanova, E.; Rimanoczy, I. Revolutionizing Sustainability Education: Stories and Tools of Mind-Set Transformation; Routledge: Abingdon, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2022; ISBN 9781000533965. [Google Scholar]
- Gigerenzer, G. Moral Intuition = Fast and Frugal Heuristics? In Moral Psychology; Sinnott-Armstrong, W., Ed.; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2007; pp. 1–26. ISBN 9780262303019. [Google Scholar]
- Narvaez, D.; Lapsley, D.K. The psychological foundations of everyday morality and moral expertise. In Character Psychology and Character Education; Lapsley, D.K., Power, C., Eds.; University of Notre Dame Press: Notre Dame, IN, USA, 2005; pp. 140–165. [Google Scholar]
- Sunstein, C.R. Moral heuristics. Behav. Brain Sci. 2005, 28, 531–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Krettenauer, T. The Gappiness of the “Gappiness Problem”. Hum. Dev. 2019, 62, 142–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, S.H.; Cieciuch, J.; Vecchione, M.; Davidov, E.; Fischer, R.; Beierlein, C.; Ramos, A.; Verkasalo, M.; Lönnqvist, J.-E.; Demirutku, K.; et al. Refining the theory of basic individual values. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 103, 663–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Sham-seer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Page, M.J.; Moher, D.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: Updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Klöckner, C.A. A comprehensive model of the psychology of environmental behaviour—A meta-analysis. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2013, 23, 1028–1038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hertz, S.G.; Krettenauer, T. Does Moral Identity Effectively Predict Moral Behavior? A Meta-Analysis. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2016, 20, 129–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayring, P. Qualitative Content Analysis. Forum Qual. Sozialforschung 2000, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landis, J.R.; Koch, G.G. The Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical Data. Biometrics 1977, 33, 159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verplanken, B.; Wood, W. Interventions to Break and Create Consumer Habits. J. Public. Policy Mark. 2006, 25, 90–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wood, W.; Tam, L.; Witt, M.G. Changing circumstances, disrupting habits. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 88, 918–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Suomala, J. The Consumer Contextual Decision-Making Model. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 570430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Klöckner, C.A.; Blöbaum, A. A comprehensive action determination model: Toward a broad-er understanding of ecological behaviour using the example of travel mode choice. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 574–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kollmuss, A.; Agyeman, J. Mind the Gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 2002, 8, 239–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, J.-M.; Kim, H.-J.; Rha, J.-Y. Shopping for Society? Consumers’ Value Conflicts in Socially Responsible Consumption Affected by Retail Regulation. Sustainability 2017, 9, 1968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newton, P.; Meyer, D. Exploring the Attitudes-Action Gap in Household Resource Consumption: Does “Environmental Lifestyle” Segmentation Align with Consumer Behaviour? Sustainability 2013, 5, 1211–1233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jennings, P.L.; Mitchell, M.S.; Hannah, S.T. The moral self: A review and integration of the literature. J. Organiz. Behav. 2015, 36, S104–S168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nucci, L. Culture, universals, and the personal. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 1997, 1997, 5–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turiel, E. The Development of Morality. In Handbook of Child Psychology; Damon, W., Lerner, R.M., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 2007; ISBN 9780471272878. [Google Scholar]
- Babutsidze, Z.; Chai, A. Look at me Saving the Planet! The Imitation of Visible Green Behav-ior and its Impact on the Climate Value-Action Gap. Ecol. Econ. 2018, 146, 290–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horcea-Milcu, A.-I. Values as leverage points for sustainability transformation: Two path-ways for transformation research. Curr. Opin. Environ. Sustain. 2022, 57, 101205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anable, J.; Lane, B.; Kelay, T. An Evidence Base Review of Attitudes to Climate Change and Transport Behaviour; Department for Transport: London, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, S.; Schellhammer, B. Indigegogy: An Invitation to Learning in a Relational Way; Wbg Academic: Darmstadt, Germany, 2021; ISBN 9783534406302. [Google Scholar]
- Stephens, J.M. Bridging the Divide: The Role of Motivation and Self-Regulation in Explaining the Judgment-Action Gap Related to Academic Dishonesty. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Genevsky, A.; Yoon, C.; Knutson, B. When Brain Beats Behavior: Neuroforecasting Crowd-funding Outcomes. J. Neurosci. 2017, 37, 8625–8634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jelle, J.; Jolles, D. Why and How to Improve Neuroscientific Literacy in Educational Professionals. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 752151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ansari, D.; König, J.; Leask, M.; Tokuhama-Espinosa, T. Developmental cognitive neuroscience: Implications for teachers’ pedagogical knowledge. In Pedagogical Knowledge and the Changing Nature of the Teaching Profession, Educational Research and Innovation; Guerriero, S., Ed.; OECD Publishing: Paris, France, 2017; pp. 195–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lotfi, S.; Zerdani, I.; Elouafi, L. Chapter 7 Learning from a Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. In New Science of Learning; Saleh, I.M., Khine, M.S., Eds.; Brill: Leiden, The Netherlands, 2023; pp. 101–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chandler, H.M.; Alexander, C.N.; Heaton, D.P. The Transcendental Meditation Program and Postconventional Self-Development: A 10-year Longitudinal Study. J. Soc. Behav. Personal. 2005, 17, 93–121. [Google Scholar]
Focus on the Self | Social Focus | Self-Transcendent Focus | No Specific Focus | |
---|---|---|---|---|
stable | Preconventional stage, self-enhancement values (achievement, power, face, hedonism); selfishness; “Enviro-Sceptic” cluster; “Market Liberalist” cluster; extrinsic religiosity, amplifying subjective well-being through a green self-image (11) | Socially responsible families; love for children and grandchildren; social norms; pressure to keep up with resource-consuming trends; demonstrating morality to others; mistrust (6) | Post-conventional stage; self-transcendence values (benevolence, universalism, humility); new environmental paradigm; feeling of connection with nature; emotional bond between a child and nature; seeing forest “as life”; moral identity; “Committed Greens” cluster; “Ethical Conformist” cluster; intrinsic religiosity; “willingness to sacrifice for the environment”; unselfishness; moral attentiveness; self-conscious moral orientation (17) | “Ambivalent Bystander” cluster; “Internally Conflicted” cluster; “Material Greens” cluster (3) |
situational | Consumed time; monetary costs; practicability; encouraging measurements (4) | Ethics-orientated social contexts; ethical leaders; unethical social contexts; unethical leaders; role models; ecological behavior in neighborhoods; information about positive social norms; trust felt towards the source of the information; high information quality; competence-based trust; integrity-based trust; enabling measurements; engaging measurements; exemplifying; intrinsic (social) rewards (15) | awareness of consequences; ascription of responsibility; immediate information given about individual consumption; information labels on products; reminders (like phone calls or posters); “discretionary time” (6) | perceived behavioral control (1) |
automated | Fear; pressure because of suddenness or unexpectedness; status quo bias; loss aversion; satisficing; risk aversion; sunk-cost effects; desire for extrinsic rewards; temporal and spatial discounting (9) | Sympathy; empathy; tendency to conform to social norms; “Boomerang Effect”; Free-Riding Effect and Social Loafing (5) | Moral judgement disposition; moral sensitivity (2) | Bayesian reasoning; Occam’s razor; inductive inference; availability bias (4) |
Focus on Self | ? | Self-Transcendent Focus | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Lee et al. [47] | “Market liberalist”: personal values, shop in various retail formats to maximize personal benefits, reject regulation | “Internally conflicted”: No clear position, clash between personal and social values. Considerable shopping in hypermarkets despite moral obligations, inconvenient with regulations | “Ambivalent bystander”: pursue personal and social values, discrepancy between stated values and actual behavior, agree with regulations | “Ethical conformist”: social values as a consumer, use small local stores, welcome regulations |
Newton and Meyer [48] | “Enviro-sceptics”: see environment as not too important and are not willing to make sacrifices for it | “Material Greens”: tend to view the environment as important, but act only pro-environmentally if it does not cost money or time | “Committed Greens”: view the environment as important, purchase green-labeled products, decline plastic bags and volunteer time for green projects |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Meyer, B.E.; Gaertner, E.; Elting, C. Closing the Gap: The Tripartite Structure of Sustainability as a Tool for Sustainable Education—A Systematic Literature Review. Sustainability 2024, 16, 3622. https://doi.org/10.3390/su16093622
Meyer BE, Gaertner E, Elting C. Closing the Gap: The Tripartite Structure of Sustainability as a Tool for Sustainable Education—A Systematic Literature Review. Sustainability. 2024; 16(9):3622. https://doi.org/10.3390/su16093622
Chicago/Turabian StyleMeyer, Barbara E., Elena Gaertner, and Christian Elting. 2024. "Closing the Gap: The Tripartite Structure of Sustainability as a Tool for Sustainable Education—A Systematic Literature Review" Sustainability 16, no. 9: 3622. https://doi.org/10.3390/su16093622
APA StyleMeyer, B. E., Gaertner, E., & Elting, C. (2024). Closing the Gap: The Tripartite Structure of Sustainability as a Tool for Sustainable Education—A Systematic Literature Review. Sustainability, 16(9), 3622. https://doi.org/10.3390/su16093622