3.1. Group Dynamics
In this first results section we provide a brief discussion of the dynamics in each of the three group interviews, to set the scene and make some observations about the conduct of the conversations themselves. Group interview 1 (GI1, 42 min) consisted of four men, Trevor, Sebastian, Sam and Bill (we note that throughout this paper pseudonyms have been used). Sam was very engaged and talkative, often the first to respond to questions, and stepping in if there was a pause in the conversation. He related the conversation to wider issues, for example which had received media coverage. Trevor was a more reserved member of the group, but always responded thoughtfully when brought into the conversation by someone else. He seemed particularly empathetic towards others, and came up with perhaps less “obvious” aspects of situations. Sebastian would also tend to wait to be brought into conversations. Particular issues seemed to be of importance to him, such as food waste, and he sometimes expressed frustration at why things were/weren’t done in certain ways. Finally Bill often used humour as a way to lighten the conversation (discussed below), and tended to give very quick and brief responses, often focussing on particularly visible aspects of situations (e.g., pollution)—discussed more in
Section 3.4. He also prompted others to give their thoughts. As a group, all participants contributed, and gave each other time to respond to questions.
Group interview 2 (GI2, 33 min) consisted of both women, Sally and Antonia, as well as Ben. Ben was perhaps the quietest participant of the three groups. He did contribute, but was less forthcoming with his opinions or feelings. Interestingly, Sam and Ben, two younger men, were perhaps the most resigned in their attitudes—“it’s just the way it is” being an indicative comment. Antonia often related the questions to specific personal experiences she had had, and was willing to respond to questions, but sometimes openly saying she didn’t have any particular opinion. Sally was the most confident of the group, and seemed happy to give her thoughts, or explain why she didn’t have opinions about some things. She often followed up and clarified what others in the group had said.
Group interview 3 (GI3, 43.5 min) consisted of James and Alan. Probably due to its small size, this group had the most direct “conversation” between participants. Alan had much to say, often reflecting issues back fairly swiftly to how they would impact on him, or relating his personal experiences to the topics. James also related the topic of conversation to wider/global issues, as Sam had done. James was also particularly conscious of many aspects of issues such as climate change, and arguably expressed the most personal concern of all the participants. Although the smallest group, this was the longest interview. In terms of gender dynamics in the three groups, the interviewer did not feel there was a significant difference between the all-male groups (GI1 and GI3) and the mixed group (GI2), in terms of participants feeling comfortable contributing to the conversation, and giving each other respect and space to do so. However one could speculate that Ben might have been more talkative in a group with others of his own age and gender. In all the interviews the interviewer did provide a thread for the conversation and used follow-up prompts to encourage participants to elaborate on responses.
An interesting theme to emerge from the analysis was the use of humour within the conversations. For example in GI1, responses to the question “what can you do to reduce the amount of energy used in your home?” included “don’t make the tea!” (Bill) or “go round to everybody else’s!” (Sam), alongside less jocular responses. Humour served the purposes of enabling participants to talk more easily about the issues, give an initial reaction to potentially difficult questions, and reassure each other about their own behaviour. The everyday humour of these conversations seemed to be a potentially useful tool to keep conversation flowing in a positive way, and one which could be borne in mind when developing public engagement materials. Overall, we found participants to be happy to talk about and engage with these issues, when the conversation was brought to them and they were provided with prompts for topics of discussion or encouragement to follow-up on their initial comments. Opportunities for discussion which are easy to attend, within a familiar environment, and/or have some small incentive, may therefore be fruitful in furthering public engagement.
3.2. Interest in and Engagement with Environment and Energy Issues
In this section we cover two areas. Firstly the impression the interviews gave of participants’ interest in the topics under discussion. This includes their views on whether the issues were important or not, in terms of whether one should act “pro-environmentally”, together with the results of the accompanying questionnaire. Secondly, we discuss the levels of knowledge displayed by the participants regarding relevant issues.
As discussed in
Section 3.1., all participants engaged with the conversations, and described their feelings, opinions and personal relevant experiences. They often expressed their own strong motivations, and desires to “do the right thing”, or what they had been taught was right, even (or sometimes particularly) when they saw others not doing this. James said sometimes he’d been “really angry at myself” (GI3) after realising he’d left something switched on by accident. Some said they “should” know more than they did. Pro-environmental motivations mentioned by participants were often related to personal experience, for example previously doing a lot of fishing and observing negative trends in environmental conditions, being a “country lad” and getting pleasure from birdsong. There were also pro-social motivations mentioned, including considerations for the wellbeing of future generations, and issues of how one could feed a growing population.
At the same time, participants sometimes did express disinterest, or ambivalence towards the issues being discussed:
“I think most people in this day and age, a lot...I’d say about sixty per cent, care about themselves more, before they care about the environment. And I’m one of them as well, to be honest wiv ya. I want to save the environment but the cost...”
—Alan, GI3
As can be seen from this quote, the desire to act pro-environmentally was sometimes seen to be constrained by other factors. That is, Alan did “want to” take positive action, however other aspects of a situation (such as cost) might take priority. When asked to reflect more broadly on the views of others, participants voiced the opinion that
some other people did care about the environment, but some did not, and that this might depend on things like occupation, age, where they lived
etc. Recognising our own mixed responses to situations and making the connection that others may be similarly affected by situational constraints may be helpful in resolving feelings of unfairness when others seem to take less action than ourselves, discussed in more detail in
Section 3.3.3. below.
Alan’s comments above, and participants’ comments about others in general, could be interpreted as expressions of a degree of
apathy. Ben also mentioned both the fact that damage had already occurred, and that it would not occur until some time in the future, in a disempowering light:
Ben: Yeah I do care about it but
RR: Yep.
Ben: It’s already messed up as it is isn’t it?
RR: Ok. What makes you say that? In what way?
Ben: Well I’m not gonna live long enough to see it really am I, but for like my kids and that, obviously want the environment to be like, good and that.
—GI2
Discussion of more affect-based reasons for inaction are arguably under-represented within mainstream debate (e.g., policy, pro-environmental advertising campaigns
etc.), although there is a growing body of research in this area [
25,
26,
27] and some increase in awareness of this issue. In using the framework of “the myth of apathy” Lertzman [
28] challenges the position that people “do not care” and instead highlights the range of emotions (e.g., denial, anxiety) which can underlie a seemingly disinterested exterior. This approach to apathy is reflected in related sociological studies that focus on how certain responses or emotional engagements may be socially produced [
26,
29].
A further measure of participants’ attitudes towards environmental issues was provided by our survey. All but one interviewee ran through our questionnaire measuring attitudes toward the environment and energy use with the interviewer, undertaken either one month prior to the interviews, or on the same day. Examples of questions included within the 7-item factor for environmental attitude were “I am interested in environmental issues” and “I believe that it is important to preserve the environment”. The survey’s Likert-scale ran from 1 (Disagree a lot) to 7 (Agree a lot). Adjusting the reverse scored questions appropriately, the mean for the 8 interview participant questionnaires was 5 out of a possible 7, indicating that the tenants had pro-environmental attitudes overall, although this was not extremely strong. The minimum value was 3.71 and the maximum value 7, and thus some participants tended towards non pro-environmental views whilst some held strongly pro-environmental views. As discussed in the introduction, those on low incomes, who do not own their own home, have been found in previous studies to be less engaged in environmental issues, however our results suggested that the majority still felt these issues to be of some interest and importance.
A secondary element which emerged from the interviews was around the level of knowledge our participants had (or felt others had) regarding environmental and energy issues. “Lack of knowledge” remains an attractive top-of-mind reason for why people do not act, and this was indeed a view which was voiced several times by the interview participants themselves. One participant expressed this view when explaining why people might use more hot water than perceived to be necessary:
Antonia: That’s wasting it. Using too much hot water when we don’t need to.
RR: Uh huh. But why do you think that is?
Antonia: I don’t know why...they’re probably not educated or they haven’t been taught it or just a lack of...no-one’s been taught it.
—GI2
However, whilst expressing these views, the participants at the same time actually displayed both a broad awareness and knowledge, as well as an understanding of how one could become more informed, if one chose. Within these (short) group interviews, which did not include any prompt material, diverse issues of relevance to our use of resources were spontaneously mentioned, including: the trend of increased use of solar and wind energy, the existence of Feed-in-Tariffs, the effects of the weather on recent wheat harvests in the UK, the use of electric vehicles, comparison of the length of time for hardwood
vs. softwood to grow, public opinion on genetically modified foods, the dilemmas posed by the use of pesticides
vs. feeding a growing population, and China’s increasing coal consumption. In fact, participants perhaps lacked confidence in their existing levels of knowledge, and maybe wanted someone else to “tell” them the real answers, or teach them, before having permission to act:
“What is the difference using energy saving [items from] ordinary? If we don’t know the difference or we’re not taught it...we can’t sort of improve anything.”
—Sally, GI2
A related strand of the interviews was around disempowerment, and that those in positions of power may put systems in place which then constrain our own actions, whereas ordinary individuals have limited power (although participants also recognised some level of responsibility which was shared between everyone). Implicit in this view of things is the view that we are not experts and depend on the information we get from others, although sometimes participants seemed to express a degree of scepticism about some of what they had heard.
There was also high awareness of the common subjects of energy saving campaigns: everyday actions one could do to save energy such as switching lights off, switching things off standby, turning the water off when brushing your teeth etc. These actions were front of mind for the participants when asked “what can you do to reduce the amount of energy used?”, whereas larger actions (e.g., driving less) took longer to be thought of, or were not mentioned at all. There has been an emphasis within environmental campaigns over past decades to concentrate on the “small steps” and these repeated messages evidently are picked up, although no specific campaigns were mentioned by name by participants.
Arguably, the knowledge the participants said they lacked would be available to them, if they were very determined to find it. Participants recognised a range of ways in which knowledge could be improved. Most notably, education (particularly in schools) was seen as an important route: “they learn this at schools nowadays, which I think is a good thing”, (James, GI3). However discussion of educational issues was tinged sometimes with a sentiment that, for those people who were beyond their schooldays, it would be much harder to obtain new knowledge:
“Even though they teach it at schools now...what about them people who you know have grown up and that and don’t know nothing about it?”
—Antonia, GI2
However, James saw other routes for knowledge acquisition for adults: “I love debating to be honest with you. You learn a lot as well just from talking”. Here was a clear expression of a desire for knowledge, and the recognition of the personal benefit which can come from being more informed.
A common view, amongst academics, policy makers and also the general public, is that many other members of the general public lack “awareness”, in some sense, of either the existence of any “problems” regarding our current use of resources (such as resource limits, climate change
etc.) or what can be done at a societal or individual level to address these issues (both policy and behavioural “solutions”). Moreover, it can be tempting to assume that this lack of knowledge is the primary factor in why these issues persist. In some ways this is a “no fault” view of others—it’s not that people don’t care, it’s that they don’t know. It is also true that there is a level of knowledge that is necessary in order to make informed behavioural choices. However, this “information deficit” model has been shown to be severely limited regarding issues with a scientific basis (such as health, climate change
etc.). That is, provision of information which “should” (“rationally” speaking) lead to a change in behaviour, may not do so, in part due to more fundamental worldviews which underlie our stated attitudes [
12].
We therefore hypothesise that, if the reality is that it is impossible to reach a point of perfect knowledge about any issue, including environmentally related issues, then feeling empowered that one is able to learn more as and when needed may be the more useful position. We found our participants to be both interested in the issues, keen to behave in sustainable ways at least some of the time, and fairly knowledgeable already, in the sense of having a broad awareness of a wide range of relevant issues, but feeling they perhaps lacked the level of knowledge needed to act in some areas. A key conclusion is that a change of emphasis from solely increasing people’s knowledge to increasing people’s confidence in their existing knowledge, and their own ability to gain knowledge and become more informed, may be effective in enabling active behavioural choices.
3.3. Barriers to Action
One sub-theme of this paper is a focus on the many layered nature of the barriers to pro-environmental behaviour, as identified by the interview participants themselves. In our reading of these discussions, we recognised an awareness amongst participants of how some of the barriers often cited are in fact more nuanced and multi-faceted than they seem at first sight. Many “front of mind” explanations given by participants for why they or others act in certain ways were revisited and expanded upon elsewhere within the same discussion. In this section, we highlight in turn three of the commonly mentioned personal barriers to action (habit, self-interest and lack of agency), where several different perspectives were explicitly voiced by participants.
3.3.1. “Habit”
The explanation of “habit” for the participants’ own behaviour, or that of others, was repeated many times, in many forms. It was often referred to in the same breath as explanations such as people “don’t think”, “forget”, or are unaware. People are “set in their ways” (Alan, GI3), they “get into a routine… they just don’t understand” (Trevor, GI1). In another of the groups, when Antonia was (gently) challenged directly to explain why she had acted in a way which had been stated as not ideal, Sally stepped in and put it down to “habit”, perhaps partly again as this is a “no-blame” way of explaining actions:
Antonia: I do that. [Chuckle]
Sally: What turn it off, or leave it running?
Antonia: Leave it running.
Sally: Oooh!
RR: And why is that?
Antonia: Hmm?
RR: Why is that?
Antonia: Cos I clean my teeth and...
Sally: [cutting in]...it’s just habit to leave the tap on.
RR: Habit right.
Sally: But I do actually, switch mine off.
—GI2
Alan also “admitted” to what he saw to be a non-environmentally friendly behaviour (leaving things on standby), explaining he “can’t help it”, (GI3). However, the very fact that Antonia and Alan had mentioned these behaviours at all showed that they were aware at some level, and at some times, of the behaviours. In this way there is more to the explanation of habit than first appears. Although we all have habits which help us navigate the myriad decisions in day-to-day life, we also have the ability to bring some level of awareness to these habits. The very act of identifying a habit could be seen as a necessary step in deciding whether to change it. A further question this raises is whether and when we encounter the prompts to bring this awareness to the issue of energy conservation, which of course in this case was facilitated by the interview. These stimuli (be they conversations with other people, the media and so forth) may be more likely to be encountered or acted upon by some sections of the public, for example those in particular social groups, or for whom self-reflection is more habitual.
A second perspective on habit, which relates to wider issues of change at the societal level, was the observation that any change does take time. Sometimes a somewhat resigned view of the world or our own behaviour was voiced—it’s just “how things are”: “you’re always going to throw away food no matter what you do” (Sam, GI1). However there was also a recognition that change does happen and has happened before—things today are very different in many ways to life previously. Sally expressed the opinion that it is always worth trying to change undesirable behaviours, even when they are heavily ingrained. When explaining that the elderly might not recycle as much as others because they never used to have to do so, she said:
Sally: So they won’t sort of think to. It’s like the saying you can’t teach an old dog new tricks...but it’s always worth a try. [Laughter]
RR: And do you think that’s true?
Sally: Oh yes. It’s always worth a try.
—GI2
There was also mention of “good” habits participants had, such as never littering or never leaving things on standby. Alan for example provided an anecdote from his previous time in industry about the strict rules there for proper recycling of different items.
These observations highlight the need to think about when awareness of habitual behaviour might be prompted, and if there are useful ways of enabling reflection such as produced by these group interviews. The participants’ discussion of the role of habit also involved the recognition that change doesn’t happen overnight which can be useful to acknowledge when developing materials concerning change of personal behaviour. Finally, the active acknowledgement of the positive habits people already have may be a useful foundation to build upon, to help people further develop habits they see as positive.
3.3.2. “Self-Interest”
Here, we use the label “self-interest” to encompass a range of barriers mentioned. The increased ease or speed associated with taking one particular action (such as driving) over another (such as walking) was mentioned several times. A common accusation made of others for not “making more of an effort” towards energy reduction was that they were “lazy”. However a further insight into this issue was put forward by Trevor who proposed that someone might make a decision for reasons of
immediate self-interest, that are not in their
broader self-interest, for example regarding their state of health. From a different viewpoint, this decision could be seen as potentially leading to less “comfort/ease” over time:
Trevor: A lot of people use their cars when they don’t always have to—you know they could walk, or...could be healthier for them you know.
RR: And why do you think that is?
Trevor: They just...
Bill: Laziness
Sam: Laziness. [Laughter]
Trevor: Quicker to jump in the car and go out. Instead of walking...sort of thing.
RR: OK.
Trevor: Be healthier for them as well but sort of thing.
—GI1
Other participants had stepped in fairly swiftly to label this behaviour as “lazy”, but Trevor saw another angle to it. He was sympathetic to the reasons why someone might choose to take the car, but also recognised there could be an actual cost to the individual of making that decision.
Immediate self-interest, rather than for example long term considerations, was also recognised as a reason behind the impactful actions of other countries, and the patterns of high consumption promoted by industry:
“You got places like China, Indonesia and places like that where they, they do that to keep their costs low so they can put their products out cheaper so they can...economy, it’s all economy. Why should they worry about 200 years’ time? They worry about now.”
—Alan, GI3
Financial considerations were also a direct link made by participants between energy saving and immediate self-interest. When asked by the interviewer “do you think saving energy is important?”, Ben made this connection immediately, responding “yeah saving money” (GI2), following up secondly that it was also to “save the planet and all that”. However the flip side of this link was that if a pro-environmental behaviour was going to cost money, it might be very unattractive. Alan felt he would be much more prepared to give up his time than his money: “if it means that everyone’s gotta go out for an hour picking up litter, then fine! … It would cost you nothing.” (GI3). This approach was seen as more palatable for Alan, perhaps as he felt time was something he was able to give, whereas money was in shorter supply for him. This reflects the fact that the same monetary cost, or time donated, can be small for one person, but very large for another, and thus such demands are rarely equitable. A further consequence of linking energy savings primarily to financial motives was voiced by Alan, namely that if you do have money, why would you ever bother saving energy?:
“Cos I think the people with bloody money...I turn lights off because I’m trying to save me own bill. If people don’t give two monkeys about leaving their lights on, cos they can afford to pay the bill...they’re not worried.”
—Alan, GI3
The question of whether using “intrinsic” motivations (e.g., “doing the right thing”), or extrinsic motivations (e.g., financial gain), impacts the effectiveness of pro-environmental messages is the subject of ongoing academic discussion [
30]. Some limitations of using extrinsic motivations in achieving long term, large scale pro-environmental behaviours have been suggested [
31], including the observation that if one appeals primarily to financial savings, what happens when a pro-environmental behaviour does actually cost more? It is therefore worth noting that some of these limitations were alluded to by participants within the discussions.
Within these conversations over self-interest therefore there seemed to be the beginnings of a debate about personal “short” and “long” term interests. A greater emphasis within public discussion of the benefits one might obtain personally (e.g., related to health) from making pro-environmental changes to behaviour may be merited. Secondly, some of the limitations of financial incentives in promoting pro-environmental behaviours were recognised by participants, and illustrate ways one might be able to talk about these limitations with the public.
3.3.3. “Lack of Agency”
The commonly voiced justification for inaction that “one person’s action on their own will not make an impact” was also repeated in these group interviews. In this way the actions of other people, other countries, or those in positions of power was seen to significantly affect the impact of one’s own actions. As Sally stated: “there’s only a certain amount of things I personally
can do, but you know if no-one else is gonna do it it’s not gonna make any difference” (GI2). Alan spoke similarly regarding whether one person’s energy use would have an impact on the environment:
“Individually no but...if you were to put it all together then yeah. If you put the whole town together and everyone puts their things full on then yeah. It will affect it. One person’s thing won’t affect, but add it together and yeah...”
—Alan, GI3
Sally went on to suggest that in some cases if
one person wasn’t doing their bit that could severely negatively impact on the effectiveness of other people’s actions, even if there were (relatively)
many of them:
Sally: Because you know say five people recycle but you get the one that doesn’t you know…It’s no benefit hardly really. To my way of thinking. You know everybody’s got to be together.
—GI2
A further dimension to the “lack of agency” narrative was also recognised by Sally. This related to the wider impact our actions can have, beyond their direct consequences. She saw a strong factor behind our actions as being the social norms we observe, the fact that we copy those around us, whether we’re aware of it or not:
Sally: Rubbish on the floor, no-one thinks about it… you see kids doin’ it well obviously, they’re just following what the adults do …
RR: Sure.
Sally: If you can sort of set the adults’ minds differently then they can actually teach the children differently but a lot of kids even though they know it’s wrong they still do it anyway.
—GI2
Many of the participants had noticed specific actions of others, and had remembered them, therefore acknowledging that our own actions have implications beyond their immediate consequence. Thus there were two different perspectives given (in this case including by the same person) on the impacts of one’s own actions; both that they only made the difference of one person (or even less if others didn’t act), but also that they could potentially have wider impacts for those who observe our actions.
It is possible that participants’ perception that individual actions are not enough to make a significant difference is linked to perceptions of fairness, in that it is important that everybody contributes equally and be treated the same. For example, one person should not necessarily have the right to consume more energy, or produce more waste, than another. If others are not “doing their bit” then we might feel resentful of having to do our bit. Alan started suggesting ways a more “fair” system might be enacted, through home energy rationing. Other research has found that concepts of fairness and equality (for example, for those in developing countries) can be very strong motivators for pro-environmental behaviours amongst individuals who have made significant life decision relating to energy use. Howell for example interviewed those who had made strong pro-active decisions regarding their carbon emission, with one participant saying “it is deeply unfair that parts of the world are going to suffer because of our needless consumption and unthinkingness” [
20]. Thus discussions of fairness in terms of our own actions contain the seeds of a sentiment which has been found to be significantly motivating for pro-environmental behaviours: the recognition of the unfairness that those who suffer from the effects of climate change are not necessarily those whose actions contribute most to it.
Thus we firstly hypothesise that recognition of the “example” effect, how one’s actions are observed and copied by others, may be a useful tool in overcoming feelings of lack of agency. Secondly, by validating people’s feelings of a sense of fairness, which can be given as a reason not to act when others do not, it may be possible to tap into a strong pro-environmental motivator.
3.4. Visibility of Issues
Finally we touch on one further theme to emerge from the interviews: the importance of visible phenomena in conversations about the environment and energy use. When “the environment” was mentioned pollution was often top-of-mind for participants, with recycling and visible waste or rubbish seen as critical issues, although, as highlighted previously, many other behaviours relating to energy consumption did emerge throughout the conversations. Issues such as pollution and recycling are particularly tangible and real to people, and tended to receive more attention than other issues. For example, Bill frequently referred to visible aspects of energy use:
“Better for the air than burnin’ coal and all that for energy. That way the more energy you would use the more coal you would burn and more pollution you are causing.”
—Bill, GI1
Thus some ways of generating energy were seen as inherently cleaner than others, but this was not necessarily to do with carbon emissions: “Batteries … are cleaner innit.” (Bill, GI1).
Sometimes this tendency to focus on “pollution” for example, is seen in a negative light within the climate change debate, as it diverts attention from behaviours which appear to be “clean” but are in fact associated with high carbon emissions. Carbon emissions can of course be regarded as pollution—however they are invisible and intangible and have no direct health effects. Some have tried to tackle this, for example by producing visualisations of carbon emissions.
People also talked about their personal experiences with pollution or rubbish:
“I’m a great believer in environment. I used to do a lot of fishing … but the fishing went right downhill … that was through pollution … and that really peeved me.”
—Alan, GI3
As has been observed in other studies [
3], recycling was also a top-of-mind issue when one mentioned “the environment”. A complaint sometimes made by those looking for large scale change is a focus by the general public on recycling as being a core aspect of “looking after the environment”. The worry is that people will assume that by doing the recycling they are “doing their bit” and therefore don’t need to worry about the less convenient aspects of sustainability, such as tackling increasing transport emissions.
Rather than seeing a focus on pollution and recycling as an inhibitor, one could ask if it is possible to capitalise on the visible and use it as a mechanism for helping to explain environmental damage. The tendency to be able to relate better to the tangible seems to be a common human trait. But we are also able to make the intangible more tangible, e.g., through discussion, imagination, sharing human experience, narrative, and story.