Keep on Rockin’ in a (Plastic-)Free World: Collective Efficacy and Pro-Environmental Intentions as a Function of Task Difficulty
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Plastic Issue
1.2. Collective Efficacy and Pro-Environmental Action
1.3. Task Difficulty and Challenge Framing
1.4. Present Research
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Design and Pretest of the Plastic Challenge Cards
2.2. Research Participants and Procedure
2.3. Material
- Collective efficacy. Collective efficacy was measured with two general items “I am optimistic that we as plastic challenge participants can protect the environment together”; “We as plastic challenge participants have the capability to protect the environment”; (r = 0.90) and one plastic-specific item (“I think we as plastic challenge participants can collectively protect the environment with reducing plastic usage”). The items were formulated similarly to previous items assessing efficacy beliefs [11,27] to fit the current study design.
- Self-efficacy. We gauged self-efficacy with two general items (“I am optimistic, that I can protect the environment”; “I am capable of protecting the environment”; r = 0.87) and one plastic-specific item (“I think that I am capable of protecting the environment by means of my personal plastic reduction”). These were adapted questions from a questionnaire by [5] (In line with other researchers, we measured efficacy perceptions with regard to individuals’ confidence in their ability (e.g., [7,8,29]) rather than action-/behavior-directed, as Bandura’s conceptualization would suggest. Hanss and colleagues ([43]) provide a comprehensive discussion of different approaches to measure and conceptualize efficacy).
- Behavioral intentions. We tapped behavioral intentions with three general items (“I will try to reduce my eco footprint in the next month”; “I intend to behave more environmentally friendly in the following month”; and “I am planning to waste less of natural resources in the next month”; α = 0.95) and one plastic-specific item (“I intend to abstain from plastic while shopping”). These items were taken from the Green Behavior Intention Scale by [44].
- Descriptive norms. We inquired about descriptive norms with four items, adapted from [40]. Participants were asked to estimate the frequency with which “inhabitants of Germany try to reduce plastic while shopping”; “inhabitants of their city try to reduce plastic while shopping”; “friends and family try to reduce plastic while shopping”; and “the person that challenged them tries to reduce plastic while shopping”. (α = 0.79).
- Injunctive norms. We measured injunctive norms with four items: “If I reduced plastic while shopping, people that matter to me would (disapprove–approve)”; “Most people that matter to me think that consuming less plastic while shopping is (undesirable–desirable)“; “Most people that matter to me think that I should (not at all–very much) advocate for plastic consumption reduction while shopping”; and “The person that challenged me to this card considers plastic reduction while shopping as (irrelevant–very relevant)” (α = 0.66).
- Attitudes. In order to measure attitudes towards plastic use, we provided seven bi-polar items on which participants were asked to rate how they perceive plastic consumption while shopping (“good–bad”, “foolish–wise”, “harmful–useful”, uncomfortable—comfortable”, “dissatisfactory—satisfactory”, “disadvantageous—advantageous”, and “negative—positive”). Those items were adapted from [45] (α = 0.79).
- Demographics. At the end of the questionnaire, we gauged participants’ age, gender, socio-economic status, and education status (student vs. non-student).
3. Results
4. Discussion
- Policy implications. The trade-off between responses to more or less difficult tasks and perceived (collective) efficacy is informative for policy making as it shows that people may more easily engage in behaviors that cost little but feel more efficacious through performing somewhat more difficult behaviors. The latter, however, may be performed by fewer individuals. Thus, it is important to identify sustainable actions that are moderately difficult but at the same time appealing enough on other dimensions (e.g., time efficient) in order to result in stronger response rates. We thus believe that the current experiment suggests applications in the field of policy communication and campaigning. Based on the current findings, it seems helpful to craft policy measures—or at least their way of communication—in a way that enables individuals to collectively address issues through moderate “behavioral challenges”. For example, providing a community with information about moderate behaviors needed to achieve energy autonomy could increase their collective efficacy to actually engage in such an endeavor, increasing people’s self-efficacy to ultimately act. Focus groups or representative surveys could help in identifying broad concepts of difficulty in the public. Of course, such policy measures need to be flanked by additional psychological concepts such as subjects of justice [48], intergenerational and ecological justice appraisals [49], as well as others. Yet, gaining knowledge about possibilities to directly encourage individuals through raising their efficacy beliefs represent an encouraging path to take. Specifically, it could be a fruitful step to investigate the underlying dimensions of difficulty (e.g., economic cost, psychological cost, time, effort, etc.) to understand the difficulty—efficacy trade-off described above.
- Limitations. The limitations of this work also deserve comment. It is in the nature of field experiments that we cannot control every potential confounding variable. First of all, cards were handed out to participants in public spaces with the plea to fill out the online survey (link and QR-Code given at the bottom of the cards) after completing the challenge. Consequently, there was no direct means of ensuring that participants in the different conditions participated to equal extents. This implies that we do not know about the individual reasons of why participants did or did not fill out the survey. Additionally, we do not know how much time passed between participants’ receiving their card and engaging in the challenge neither do we know the time lag between completion of the task and survey participation. These points notwithstanding, it is even more intriguing that the difficulty of the task influenced beliefs and intentions in the subsequent online questionnaire. A second issue that needs clarification is the perceived difficulty of the task. While our pretest was successful in showing that our manipulation resulted in the expected difficulty ratings (yet, with the medium difficulty being statistically somewhat closer to the easy than the difficult task), future studies should implement a methodology that allows participants to select their preferred task difficulty. In our study, participants were confronted with one of the three difficulties at random so that it is likely that “moderate difficulty” could be easy for some, and hard for others. In this line, it is important to mention that the different tasks yielded differing environmental impacts depending on consumption patterns and specific products that participants would purchase while completing the challenge (e.g., glass containers might, under certain circumstances, yield worse environmental impact than the equivalent plastic container). We assume that the great majority of participants did not possess the necessary background knowledge to take this into consideration when consequently rating one’s own efficacy beliefs concerning the behavior change. It is also possible that people from various socio-structural backgrounds (e.g., students vs. non-students) perceive task difficulty differently. Unfortunately, our pretest data did not include such a ratio.
- Future research. Finally, we encourage future researchers to link self- and collective efficacy more strongly to the issue of private vs. public sphere behavior [3]. While it is quite plausible to assume that private sphere behavior (i.e., personal practice) may be less influenced by collective efficacy beliefs than public sphere behavior (i.e., civic action), some empirical evidence suggests otherwise. For example, Morton and colleagues [29] showed that collective efficacy predicted private sphere behavior (specifically, reducing household waste and non-green energy consumption); similarly, Lubell and colleagues [50], using a rational choice framework, came to similar conclusions (see also [30,31]). These findings show that collective efficacy is also important in private sphere settings—a view researchers have just begun to acknowledge (see also [11]). Yet, it would be informative and important to understand whether different aspects of efficacy relate to different private or public behaviors (see [51,52])—and whether such efficacy beliefs could be gauged by identification with groups that support sustainability causes.
5. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Batalha, L.; Reynolds, K. ASPIRing to mitigate climate change: Superordinate identity in global climate negotiations. Polit. Psychol. 2012, 33, 743–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reese, G. Common human identity and the path to global climate justice. Clim. Chang. 2016, 134, 521–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stern, P. Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 407–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bamberg, S.; Möser, G. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2007, 27, 14–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fielding, K.S.; McDonald, R.; Louis, W.R. Theory of planned behavior, identity and intentions to engage in environmental activism. J. Environ. Psychol. 2008, 28, 318–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schultz, P.W. The structure of environmental concern: Concern for self, other people, and the biosphere. J. Environ. Psychol. 2001, 21, 327–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Zomeren, M.; Postmes, T.; Spears, R. Toward an integrative social identity model of collective action: A quantitative research synthesis of three socio-psychological perspectives. Psychol. Bull. 2008, 134, 504–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Barth, M.; Jugert, P.; Fritsche, I. Still underdetected—Social norms and collective efficacy predict the acceptance of electric vehicles in Germany. Transp. Res. F 2016, 37, 64–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doran, R.; Hanss, D.; Larsen, S. Attitudes, efficacy beliefs, and willingness to pay for environmental protection when travelling. Tour. Hosp. Res. 2015, 15, 281–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doran, R.; Hanss, D.; Larsen, S. Intentions to make sustainable tourism choices: Do value orientations, time perspective, and efficacy beliefs explain individual differences? Scand. J. Hosp. Tour. 2016, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cocking, C.; Drury, J. Generalization of Efficacy as a Function of Collective Action and Intergroup Relations: Involvement in an Anti-Roads Struggle. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 34, 417–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jugert, P.; Barth, M.; Greenaway, K.; Buechner, R.; Eisentraut, S.; Fritsche, I. Collective efficacy increases pro-environmental intentions through increasing personal efficacy. J. Environ. Psychol. 2016, 48, 12–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Environment Protection and Heritage Council. Plastic Shopping Bags in Australia: National Plastic Bags Working Group Report to the National Packing Covenant Council; Environment Protection and Heritage Council: Adelaide, Australia, 2002. Available online: http://www.nepc.gov.au/system/files/resources/0c513e54-d968-ac04-758b-3b7613af0d07/files/ps-pbag-rpt-npbwg-report-npcc-200212.pdf (accessed on 27 January 2017).
- Steinmetz, Z.; Wollmann, C.; Schaefer, M.; Buchmann, C.; David, J.; Tröger, J.; Schaumann, G.E. Plastic mulching in agriculture. Trading short-term agronomic benefits for long-term soil degradation? Sci. Total Environ. 2016, 550, 690–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Halden, R.U. Plastics and Health Risks. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2010, 31, 179–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thompson, R.; Moore, C.; vom Saal, F.; Swan, S. Plastics, the environment and human health: Current consensus and future trends. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B 2009, 364, 2153–2166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cherrier, H. Consumer identity and moral obligations in non-plastic bag consumption: A dialectical perspective. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2006, 30, 515–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A. Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. Am. Psychol. 1982, 37, 122–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A. Exercise of Human Agency through Collective Efficacy. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2000, 9, 75–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ajzen, I. Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 32, 665–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A. Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control; Freeman: New York, NY, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Tims, M.; Bakker, A.B.; Derks, D. Daily job crafting and the self-efficacy—Performance relationship. J. Manag. Psychol. 2014, 29, 490–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Riggio, H.R.; Weiser, D.A.; Valenzuela, A.M.; Lui, P.P.; Montes, R.; Heuer, J. Self-efficacy in romantic relationships: Prediction of relationship attitudes and outcomes. J. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 153, 629–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- LaForge-MacKenzie, K.; Sullivan, P.J.; Hansen, S.; Marini, M. The effect of attentional focus on the self-efficacy-performance relationship in a continuous running task: A pilot study. J. Exerc. Mov. Sport 2013, 45, 1. [Google Scholar]
- Sitzmann, T.; Yeo, G. A Meta-Analytic Investigation of the Within-Person Self-Efficacy Domain: Is Self-Efficacy a Product of Past Performance or a Driver of Future Performance? Personal. Psychol. 2013, 66, 531–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lauren, N.; Fielding, K.S.; Smith, L.; Louis, W.R. You did, so you can and you will: Self-efficacy as a mediator of spillover from easy to more difficult pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2016, 48, 191–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fielding, K.S.; Hornsey, M.J. A social identity analysis of climate change and environmental attitudes and behaviors: Insights and opportunities. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morton, T.A.; Rabinovich, A.; Marshall, D.; Bretschneider, P. The future that may (or may not) come: How framing changes responses to uncertainty in climate change communications. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2011, 21, 103–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Homburg, A.; Stolberg, A. Explaining pro-environmental behavior with a cognitive theory of stress. J. Environ. Psychol. 2006, 26, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, M.-F. Self-efficacy or collective efficacy within the cognitive theory of stress model: Which more effectively explains people’s self-reported proenvironmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015, 42, 66–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fritsche, I.; Jonas, E.; Ablasser, C.; Beyer, M.; Kuban, J.; Manger, A.-M.; Schultz, M. The power of we: Evidence for group-based control. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 49, 19–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greenaway, K.H.; Haslam, S.A.; Cruwys, T.; Branscombe, N.R.; Ysseldyk, R.; Heldreth, C. From “we” to “me”: Group identification enhances perceived personal control with consequences for health and well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 109, 53–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Atkinson, J.W. Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychol. Rev. 1957, 64, 359–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baron, R.A.; Mueller, B.A.; Wolfe, M.T. Self-efficacy and entrepreneurs’ adoption of unattainable goals: The restraining effects of self-control. J. Bus. Ventur. 2016, 31, 55–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kanfer, R.; Ackerman, P.L. Aging, adult development, and work motivation. Acad. Manag. Rev. 2004, 29, 440–458. [Google Scholar]
- Vancouver, J.B.; More, K.M.; Yoder, R.J. Self-efficacy and resource allocation: Support for a nonmonotonic, discontinuous model. J. Appl. Psychol. 2008, 93, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Zomeren, M.; Spears, R.; Fischer, A.H.; Leach, C.W. Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 87, 649–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bator, R.J.; Tabanico, J.J.; Walton, M.L.; Schultz, P.W. Promoting energy conservation with implied norms and explicit messages. Soc. Influ. 2014, 9, 69–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldstein, N.J.; Cialdini, R.B.; Griskevicius, V. A room with a viewpoint: Using social norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. J. Consum. Res. 2008, 35, 472–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hamann, K.R.; Reese, G.; Seewald, D.; Loeschinger, D.C. Affixing the theory of normative conduct (to your mailbox): Injunctive and descriptive norms as predictors of anti-ads sticker use. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015, 44, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nolan, J.M.; Schultz, P.W.; Cialdini, R.B.; Goldstein, N.J.; Griskevicius, V. Normative social influence is underdetected. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2008, 34, 913–923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reese, G.; Loew, K.; Steffgen, G. A towel less: Social norms enhance pro-environmental behavior in hotels. J. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 154, 97–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hanss, D.; Böhm, G.; Doran, R.; Homburg, A. Sustainable consumption of groceries: The importance of believing that one can contribute to sustainable development. Sustain. Dev. 2016, 24, 357–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mancha, R.; Muniz, K.; Yoder, C. Studying Executives’ Green Behaviors: An Environmental Theory of Planned Behavior. Unpublished Manuscript. 2014. Available online: http://aisel.aisnet.org/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1574&context=amcis2014 (accessed on 27 January 2017).
- Han, H.; Hsu, L.; Sheu, C. Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior to green hotel choice: Testing the effect of environmental friendly activities. Tour. Manag. 2010, 31, 325–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis; Guildford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Truelove, H.B.; Carrico, A.R.; Weber, E.U.; Raimi, K.T.; Vandenbergh, M.P. Positive and negative spillover of pro-environmental behavior: an integrative review and theoretical framework. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2014, 29, 127–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sikor, T.; Martin, A.; Fisher, J.; He, J. Toward an empirical analysis of justice in ecosystem governance. Conserv. Lett. 2014, 7, 524–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Reese, G.; Jacob, L. Principles of environmental justice and pro-environmental action: A two-step process model of moral anger and responsibility to act. Environ. Sci. Policy 2015, 51, 88–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lubell, M.; Zahran, S.; Vedlitz, A. Collective action and citizen responses to global warming. Polit. Behav. 2007, 29, 391–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alisat, S.; Riemer, M. The environmental action scale: Development and psychometric evaluation. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015, 43, 13–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dono, J.; Webb, J.; Richardson, B. The relationship between environmental activism, pro-environmental behavior and social identity. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 178–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
M | SD | 1. | 2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | 8. | 9. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Collective efficacy—plastic | 4.69 | 1.69 | --- | 0.91 * | 0.51 * | 0.53 * | 0.40 * | 0.38 * | 0.31 * | 0.23 * | 0.27 * |
2. Collective efficacy—general | 4.61 | 1.67 | --- | 0.43 * | 0.58 * | 0.33 * | 0.40 * | 0.31 * | 0.16 * | 0.25 * | |
3. Self-efficacy—plastic | 5.84 | 1.44 | --- | 0.60 * | 0.48 * | 0.24 * | 0.50 * | 0.13 | 0.19 * | ||
4. Self-efficacy—general | 5.36 | 1.51 | --- | 0.33 * | 0.32 * | 0.45 * | 0.08 * | 0.09 * | |||
5. Sustainable intentions—plastic | 6.03 | 1.21 | --- | 0.65 * | 0.51 * | 0.28 * | 0.34 * | ||||
6. Sustainable intentions—general | 5.42 | 1.3 | --- | 0.33 * | 0.23 * | 0.33 * | |||||
7. Attitudes | 6.15 | 0.78 | --- | 0.23 * | 0.29 * | ||||||
8. Descriptive norms | 3.91 | 0.99 | --- | 0.37 * | |||||||
9. Injunctive norms | 5.98 | 0.72 | --- |
Measure | Task Difficulty | ||
---|---|---|---|
Easy | Medium | Difficulty | |
M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | |
Collective efficacy—plastic | 4.40 (1.76) | 5.14 (1.42) | 4.58 (1.84) |
Collective efficacy—general | 4.31 (1.72) | 5.1 (1.48) | 4.46 (1.75) |
Self-efficacy—plastic | 5.77 (1.55) | 6.14 (0.97) | 5.42 (1.86) |
Self-efficacy—general | 5.29 (1.6) | 5.46 (1.26) | 5.33 (1.77) |
N (participants) | 78 | 58 | 29 |
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Reese, G.; Junge, E.A. Keep on Rockin’ in a (Plastic-)Free World: Collective Efficacy and Pro-Environmental Intentions as a Function of Task Difficulty. Sustainability 2017, 9, 200. https://doi.org/10.3390/su9020200
Reese G, Junge EA. Keep on Rockin’ in a (Plastic-)Free World: Collective Efficacy and Pro-Environmental Intentions as a Function of Task Difficulty. Sustainability. 2017; 9(2):200. https://doi.org/10.3390/su9020200
Chicago/Turabian StyleReese, Gerhard, and Eva A. Junge. 2017. "Keep on Rockin’ in a (Plastic-)Free World: Collective Efficacy and Pro-Environmental Intentions as a Function of Task Difficulty" Sustainability 9, no. 2: 200. https://doi.org/10.3390/su9020200
APA StyleReese, G., & Junge, E. A. (2017). Keep on Rockin’ in a (Plastic-)Free World: Collective Efficacy and Pro-Environmental Intentions as a Function of Task Difficulty. Sustainability, 9(2), 200. https://doi.org/10.3390/su9020200