Intentions-Based Reciprocity to Monetary and Non-Monetary Gifts
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Experimental Design and Procedure
2.1. Gift Items
2.2. Experimental Session Overview
2.3. Dictator Game Procedure
2.4. Questionnaire
2.5. Subject Recruitment
3. Results
3.1. Summary Statistics
3.2. Regression Analysis
3.2.1. Wealth Effects
3.2.2. Reciprocity to Gifts
3.2.3. Reciprocity to Intentions
3.2.4. Punishment for Not Giving
3.2.5. Tobit Specification
3.2.6. Gift-Giver Expectations
3.2.7. Effects by Treatment Type
3.3. Dictator Types
3.4. Subject Self-Reported Motives
4. Discussion
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Variable | Mean | SD | Min | Max | Median |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 20.18 | 2.48 | 18 | 33 | 20 |
Male | 0.38 | 0.49 | 0 | 1 | 0 |
Previous Experiments | 4.07 | 4.49 | 0 | 18 | 3 |
Machiavellianism | 58.71 | 6.57 | 45 | 76 | 59 |
DV = Dollars Allocated | (A1) |
---|---|
Specification a | OLS |
Dictators (Rounds) | 59 (1416) |
R2 | 0.119 |
Treatment Indicators | |
Gift | 0.170 * (0.099) |
Overturned Gift | −0.039 (0.059) |
Item Indicators (Wealth Effects) | |
Has pen | 0.566 *** (0.127) |
Has $2 item | 0.939 *** (0.178) |
Has bag | 1.151 *** (0.222) |
Gift-from-Partner * Item Type (Reciprocity Effects) | |
Given pen by partner | 0.260 ** (0.128) |
Given cash by partner | 0.612 *** (0.191) |
Given bag by partner | 0.557 ** (0.262) |
Partner’s gift was overturned | 0.596 *** (0.145) |
Constant | 0.604 *** (0.148) |
Subject Random Effects | YES |
DV = Dollars Allocated | (A2) | (A3) | (A4) |
---|---|---|---|
Specification a | OLS | OLS | OLS |
Subjects | All | All | High Mach |
Dictators (Rounds) | 59 (1416) | 59 (1416) | 24 (576) |
R2 | 0.180 | 0.196 | 0.208 |
Treatment Indicators | |||
Gift | 0.170 * (0.099) | 0.170 * (0.099) | 0.215 (0.155) |
Overturned Gift | −0.038 (0.059) | −0.038 (0.059) | −0.040 (0.107) |
Item Indicators (Wealth Effects) | |||
Has pen | 0.566 *** (0.127) | 0.566 *** (0.127) | 0.554 ** (0.228) |
Has $2 item | 0.939 *** (0.178) | 0.939 *** (0.178) | 1.138 *** (0.321) |
Has bag | 1.151 *** (0.223) | 1.151 *** (0.223) | 1.408 *** (0.371) |
Gift-from-Partner * Item Type (Reciprocity Effects) | |||
Given pen by partner | 0.259 ** (0.128) | 0.259 ** (0.129) | 0.391 * (0.209) |
Given cash by partner | 0.611 *** (0.191) | 0.611 *** (0.191) | 0.272 (0.237) |
Given bag by partner | 0.555 ** (0.263) | 0.555 ** (0.263) | 0.859 ** (0.373) |
Partner’s gift was overturned | 0.594 *** (0.145) | 0.594 *** (0.145) | 0.977 *** (0.261) |
Demographic Controls | |||
Older | 0.252 (0.326) | 0.285 (0.327) | |
Experienced | −0.580 * (0.305) | −0.602 ** (0.298) | |
Male | −0.531 * (0.307) | −0.437 (0.275) | |
Machiavellianism | −0.023 (0.020) | ||
Constant | 0.934 *** (0.267) | 2.203 * (1.188) | 0.238 *** (0.779) |
Subject Random Effects | YES | YES | YES |
Appendix B
Appendix B.1. Introduction
Appendix B.2. General Instructions: Section 1
Appendix B.3. Section 1 Continued
Appendix B.4. Section 1 Continued
Appendix B.5. Section 1 Continued
Appendix B.6. Section 2: General Instructions
Appendix C. Detailed Procedure
References
- Rabin, M. Incorporating fairness into game theory and economics. Am. Econ. Rev. 1993, 83, 1281–1302. [Google Scholar]
- Dufwenberg, M.; Kirchsteiger, G. A theory of sequential reciprocity. Games Econ. Behav. 2004, 47, 268–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McCabe, K.; Rigdon, M.; Smith, V. Positive reciprocity and intentions in trust games. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2003, 52, 267–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Charness, G.; Levine, D. Intention and stochastic outcomes: An experimental study. Econ. J. 2007, 117, 1051–1072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Falk, A.; Fehr, E.; Fischbacher, U. Testing theories of fairness—Intentions matter. Games Econ. Behav. 2008, 62, 287–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Akerlof, G. Labor contracts as partial gift exchange. Q. J. Econ. 1982, 97, 543–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Akerlof, G.; Yellen, J. Fairness and unemployment. Am. Econ. Rev. 1988, 78, 44–49. [Google Scholar]
- Rubin, J.; Sheremeta, R. Principal-agent settings with random shocks. Manag. Sci. 2016, 62, 985–999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Charness, G.; Dufwenberg, M. Promises and partnership. Econometrica 2006, 74, 1579–1601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Battigalli, P.; Dufwenberg, M. Guilt in games. Am. Econ. Rev. 2007, 97, 170–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blanco, M.; Celen, B.; Schotter, A. On Blame-Freeness and Reciprocity: An Experimental Study, 28 June 2010. Universidad del Rosario Faculty of Economics Working Paper No. 85. Available online: https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=1703546 (accessed on 1 September 2018).
- Nelson, W., Jr. Equity and intention: It’s the thought that counts. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2002, 48, 423–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kube, S.; Marechal, M.; Puppe, C. The currency of reciprocity: Gift-exchange in the workplace. Am. Econ. Rev. 2012, 102, 1644–1662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ames, D.; Flynn, F.; Weber, E. It’s the thought that counts: On perceiving how helpers decide to lend a hand. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2004, 30, 461–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stanca, L.; Bruni, L.; Corazzini, L. Testing theories of reciprocity: Do motivations matter? J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2009, 71, 233–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Von Siemens, F.A. Intention-based reciprocity and the hidden costs of control. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2013, 92, 55–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Beltramini, R. Exploring the effectiveness of business gifts: Replication and extension. J. Advert. 2000, 29, 75–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, Y.; Epley, N. Exaggerated, mispredicted, and misplaced: When “it’s the thought that counts” in gift exchanges. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2012, 141, 667–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beltramini, R. Exploring the effectiveness of business gifts: A controlled field experiment. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 1992, 20, 87–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Falk, A. Gift exchange in the field. Econometrica 2007, 75, 1501–1511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pan, X.; Xiao, E. It’s not just the thought that counts: An experimental study on hidden cost giving. J. Public Econ. 2016, 138, 22–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malmendier, U.; Schmidt, K. You owe me. Am. Econ. Rev. 2017, 107, 493–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cox, J.; Deck, C. Assigning intentions when actions are unobservable: The impact of trembling in the trust game. South. Econ. J. 2006, 73, 307–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toussaert, S. Intention-based reciprocity and signaling of intentions. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2017, 137, 132–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, B.; Kerschbamer, R.; Oexl, R. Is reciprocity really outcome-based? A second look at gift-exchange with random shocks. J. Econ. Sci. Assoc. 2017, 3, 149–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fischbacher, U. z-Tree: Zurich Toolbox for Ready-made Economic Experiments. Exp. Econ. 2007, 10, 171–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leider, S.; Mobius, M.; Rosenblat, T.; Do, Q. Directed altruism and enforced reciprocity in social networks. Q. J. Econ. 2009, 124, 1815–1851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Charness, G.; Gneezy, U.; Kuhn, M. Experimental methods: between-subject and within-subject design. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2012, 81, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bolton, G.; Brands, J.; Ockenfels, A. Measuring motivations for the reciprocal responses observed in a simple dilemma game. Exp. Econ. 1998, 1, 207–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Charness, G.; Haruvy, E. Altruism, fairness, and reciprocity in a gift-exchange experiment: An encompassing approach. Games Econ. Behav. 2002, 40, 203–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Charness, G.; Rabin, M. Understanding social preferences with simple tests. Q. J. Econ. 2002, 117, 817–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Charness, G. Attribution and reciprocity in an experimental labor market. J. Labor Econ. 2004, 22, 665–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cox, J. How to identify trust and reciprocity. Games Econ. Behav. 2004, 46, 260–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Xiao, E.; Bicchieri, C. When equality trumps reciprocity. J. Econ. Psychol. 2010, 31, 456–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Christie, R.; Geis, F. Studies in Machiavellianism; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1970. [Google Scholar]
- Flynn, F.; Adams, G. Money can’t buy love: Asymmetric beliefs about the link between gift price and feelings of appreciation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 45, 404–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
1 | Some subjects in this experiment also self-reported inferring kind intentions from a gift, as detailed in the results section. |
2 | Since these gift items are predetermined, the exact items in this study are not chosen by the gift-giver and thus reciprocity due to thoughtfulness or consideration in gift choice is theoretically irrelevant in this context. This is a different context from some experiments in the literature [13], and this experiment should be thought of as testing some but obviously not all of the possible motives for reciprocity to non-monetary gifts in the field. |
3 | This reminder, as well as the possibility of a no-item round, were included to reduce the likelihood that dictators would punish partners when they did not receive gifts. |
4 | Soliciting dictator beliefs on partner-expectations prior to allocation would likely have biased the allocation decision. Soliciting dictator beliefs after an allocation would likely have yielded biased responses on partner-expectations (while possibly also influencing dictator allocations in subsequent rounds). |
5 | Importantly, dictators were not told the item type that was overturned. This design was chosen because displaying the overturned item would make the overturned gift screen appear too similar to the screen for actual gift transfers, potentially causing confusion. The implemented design ensures that the intend-to-give message is visually distinct from actual gift transfers beyond just a simple line of text. In addition, splitting overturned gifts by item type would have made it difficult to obtain sufficient overturned-gift observations for some items. This design naturally restricts some of the analysis, but it still allows estimation of reciprocity to just the intent to give a gift. As the results will demonstrate, there is still a strong response to intentions in this context. |
6 | In some control treatment rounds, the gift-giver is the one endowed with an item. In these rounds, the dictator does not know whether their partner has an item. This is identical in wealth and information to when no gift is given. In rounds with an overturned gift, the dictator knows their partner has an item, so information is not identical to this scenario; however, when comparing an overturned gift round with this control round, this difference in information would only bias against finding a positive effect of intentions on allocations. |
7 | This Caltech session yielded 14 dictators. Adding these subjects yields the same (and in fact more statistically significant) results, but since the subject pool is different, analysis including them is reserved for Appendix A. Note that all SSEL subjects were Caltech undergraduates with verified current Caltech IDs, and all CASSEL subjects were UCLA undergraduates with verified current UCLA IDs, so it is unlikely there were any subjects who belonged to both subject pools. |
8 | Random effects assumptions are met because all covariates are determined exogenously to the dictator (i.e., by their randomly selected partner or by the experimenter). Subject fixed effects yield similar results for all regressions in this paper, and the Hausman test comparing coefficients between the random effects and fixed effects models always suggests there is no systematic difference in their coefficients (p > 0.99 for Model (1)). |
9 | The first CASSEL session did not include these questions, as they were added after the first session; to make up for this, I asked the mTurk workers to also code the responses submitted by subjects in the Caltech session. |
10 | The exact wording of the category was “gratitude/appreciation/to repay their kindness.” |
11 | Subjects in the first session were not asked to estimate values of items, so estimates are based on subjects from the remaining sessions. |
DV = Dollars Allocated | (1) | (2) | (3) | (4) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Specification a | OLS | OLS | Tobit b | OLS |
Dictators (Rounds) | 45 (1080) | 45 (1080) | 45 (1080) | 45 (1080) |
R2 | 0.120 | 0.120 | 0.120 | |
Treatment Indicators | ||||
Gift | 0.190 (0.125) | 0.190 (0.125) | 0.435 (0.348) | 0.187 (0.123) |
Overturned Gift | −0.057 (0.076) | −0.057 (0.076) | −0.265 (0.305) | −0.060 (0.075) |
Item Indicators (Wealth Effects) | ||||
Has pen | 0.560 *** (0.148) | 0.560 *** (0.148) | 1.463 *** (0.351) | 0.563 *** (0.148) |
Has $2 item | 0.915 *** (0.209) | 0.915 *** (0.209) | 2.125 *** (0.409) | 0.918 *** (0.208) |
Has bag | 1.037 *** (0.230) | 1.037 *** (0.230) | 2.256 *** (0.429) | 1.048 *** (0.229) |
Gift-from-Partner * Item Indicators (Reciprocity Effects) | ||||
Given pen | 0.283 * (0.164) | −0.336 * (0.204) | 0.739 * (0.398) | 0.289 * (0.164) |
Given $2 | 0.453 ** (0.198) | −0.167 (0.219) | 1.042 ** (0.414) | 0.469 ** (0.202) |
Given bag | 0.617 ** (0.292) | −0.008 (0.305) | 0.922 * (0.547) | 0.619 ** (0.292) |
Gift overturned | 0.619 *** (0.167) | 1.910 *** (0.446) | 0.630 *** (0.169) | |
Intention-to-give | 0.619 *** (0.167) | |||
Partner Expectation | −0.006 (0.008) | |||
Constant | 0.529 *** (0.139) | 0.529 *** (0.139) | −1.945 *** (0.658) | 0.544 *** (0.133) |
Subject Random Effects | YES | YES | YES | YES |
DV = Dollars Allocated | (5) |
---|---|
Specification a | OLS |
Dictators (Rounds) | 45 (1080) |
R2 | 0.119 |
Treatment Indicators | |
Gift | 0.183 (0.120) |
Overturned Gift | −0.049 (0.083) |
Item Indicators (Wealth Effects) | |
Has pen | 0.455 *** (0.132) |
Has $2 item | 0.922 *** (0.219) |
Has bag | 1.134 *** (0.222) |
Gift-from-Partner * Treatment Indicators | |
Gift * Gift treatment | 0.449 *** (0.165) |
Gift * Overturned Gift treatment | 0.402 ** (0.197) |
Partner’s gift was overturned | 0.611 *** (0.173) |
Constant | 0.529 *** (0.139) |
Subject Random Effects | YES |
Treatment | No Item | Received Item a |
---|---|---|
Control (From Computer) | 26 (58%) | 17 (38%) |
Gift | 22 (49%) | 11 (24%) |
Overturn c | 32 (71%) | 12 (27%) |
Overturned Gift Rounds | 18 (40%) b | N/A |
Always Zero | 10 (22%) |
© 2018 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Chao, M. Intentions-Based Reciprocity to Monetary and Non-Monetary Gifts. Games 2018, 9, 74. https://doi.org/10.3390/g9040074
Chao M. Intentions-Based Reciprocity to Monetary and Non-Monetary Gifts. Games. 2018; 9(4):74. https://doi.org/10.3390/g9040074
Chicago/Turabian StyleChao, Matthew. 2018. "Intentions-Based Reciprocity to Monetary and Non-Monetary Gifts" Games 9, no. 4: 74. https://doi.org/10.3390/g9040074
APA StyleChao, M. (2018). Intentions-Based Reciprocity to Monetary and Non-Monetary Gifts. Games, 9(4), 74. https://doi.org/10.3390/g9040074