Next Article in Journal
FMR1 and Autism, an Intriguing Connection Revisited
Next Article in Special Issue
Analysis of DRD2 Gene Polymorphism in the Context of Personality Traits in a Group of Athletes
Previous Article in Journal
Mitogenome Phylogeny Including Data from Additional Subspecies Provides New Insights into the Historical Biogeography of the Eurasian lynx Lynx lynx
Previous Article in Special Issue
Total Genotype Score Modelling of Polygenic Endurance-Power Profiles in Lithuanian Elite Athletes
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association between Polymorphism rs1799732 of DRD2 Dopamine Receptor Gene and Personality Traits among MMA Athletes

by
Marta Niewczas
1,
Anna Grzywacz
2,*,
Katarzyna Leźnicka
3,
Krzysztof Chmielowiec
4,
Jolanta Chmielowiec
4,
Agnieszka Maciejewska-Skrendo
5,
Pavel Ruzbarsky
6,
Jolanta Masiak
7,
Wojciech Czarny
1 and
Paweł Cięszczyk
3
1
Faculty of Physical Education, University of Rzeszów, 3 Towarnickiego St., 35-959 Rzeszów, Poland
2
Independent Laboratory of Health Promotion, Pomeranian Medical University in Szczecin, 11 Chlapowskiego St., 70-204 Szczecin, Poland
3
Faculty of Physical Education, Gdansk University of Physical Education and Sport, 1 K. Górskiego St., 80-336 Gdansk, Poland
4
Department of Hygiene and Epidemiology, Collegium Medicum, University of Zielona Góra, 28 Zyty St., 65-046 Zielona Góra, Poland
5
Institute of Physical Culture Sciences, University of Szczecin, 40B Piastów St., 71-065 Szczecin, Poland
6
Faculty of Sports, University of Presov, 15, 17. novembra St., 080 01 Prešov, Slovakia
7
Neurophysiological Independent Unit, Department of Psychiatry, Medical University of Lublin, 1 Aleje Racławickie St., 20-059 Lublin, Poland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Genes 2021, 12(8), 1217; https://doi.org/10.3390/genes12081217
Submission received: 9 July 2021 / Revised: 1 August 2021 / Accepted: 4 August 2021 / Published: 6 August 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Genetics and Genomics in Sport)

Abstract

:
Four factors—namely, harm avoidance, novelty seeking, reward addiction and persistence—represent the nature of temperament that is not genetically determined in itself. It was shown in earlier studies that a strong propensity to look for novelty or a tendency to engage in risky behavior is correlated with genetic variants in the area of the genes encoding dopamine receptors. Therefore, the aim of this study is to determine whether there is a relationship between personality traits and genetic variants in the area of the DRD2 dopamine receptor gene in MMA athletes. The participants consisted of 85 mixed martial arts (MMA) athletes and 284 healthy, non-MMA male participants. Their personality traits were measured using the Revised Temperament and Character Inventory. Blood was collected for genetic assays and all samples were genotyped using the real-time PCR method. We observed a statistically significant effect of a complex factor of the DRD2 rs1799732 genotype on MMA participants’ control and reward dependence. Engaging in high-risk sport may be associated with several personality characteristics. The DRD2 rs1799732 polymorphism may be associated with reduced harm avoidance in martial arts athletes, thereby modulating athletes’ predisposition to participate in high-risk sport.

1. Introduction

Being a professional athlete is very demanding. An increased level of competition is always connected with the occurrence of more detailed factors determining success achievement. Elite athletes very often have to act under enormous pressure, which is generally connected with different psychological features influencing the achievement of success at the top level, in comparison with others that are less successful [1].
High resistance of the nervous system and unique personality features such as: persistence, motivation to act, will power and conscientiousness allow athletes to prepare effective strategies for dealing with physical and psychological loads, anxiety or pain [2]. The role of particular personality features reveals itself in the strongest form when a person acts in stressful conditions and carries out tasks requiring special mobilization of the body.
Studies of innate psychological features among combat athletes suggest lower sensitivity and higher resistance of the central nervous system when compared with non-athletes [3]. Competitors who are less sensorily sensitive deal better with tension, lack of comfort, time pressure and physical discomfort. The control of emotional agitation contributes to emotion control and energy management to obtain the maximal results, especially in situations of great stress during sport competition. Athletes who practice individual sports are more introverted in comparison with athletes in team sports [4]. The anxiety of the competitor is connected with a continuous increase in the level of physical fitness and maintenance of effective muscle tension, but also coordination difficulties and disturbances in concentration, especially when an athlete observes they lack control in a specific situation.
Meta-analysis describing the problem of personality variety in different groups of athletes and non-athletes indicates a higher level of extraversion and lower level of neuroticism among athletes in comparison with the general population [5]. Most research shows that the individual effectiveness of competitors is positively correlated with extraversion and negatively with neuroticism [6,7,8].
Previous reports regarding the temperamental characteristics of athletes, using the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) by Cloninger, have observed significant connections between impulsiveness, impetuousness and novelty seeking (NS) [9,10]. Han et al. in the conducted study observed the predictive role of persistence in terms of the possibility of winning [11]. Cloninger, in his report (1986), observed that individuals with higher persistence are more predisposed to continuous task performance without immediate successive reward or boosting [12].
Studies conducted by Hollis et al. [13] emphasized how individual personality traits could modulate sport injury susceptibility. Authors have noted that rugby players with increased concussion risk achieve higher impulsiveness scores. Both impulsiveness and aggression were related to concussion history in that same group of athletes [14].
It is probable that relations between the genetically conditioned temperament and environmentally conditioned character highly modulate the will to participate in and continue with sport training, as well as modifying the tolerance to strenuous effort, which as a result, may affect the achievement of success in this area [15].
Four factors—namely, harm avoidance (HA), novelty seeking (NS), reward dependence (RD) and persistence—represent the character of one’s temperament, which is not genetically conditioned by itself, but is moderately heritable and time stable [16]. Personality traits connected with a strong will for novelty seeking (NS) or a tendency to undertake risky behaviors were previously correlated with genetic variants in the area of the genes coding dopamine receptors. Dopamine is one of the main neurotransmitters within the behavioral approach system [17] involved in regulating the brain systems that control the cognitive and emotional decision processes that underlie both extraversion and neuroticism [18]. Subtypes of dopamine receptors D2 and D4 participate in dopamine neurotransmission and may modulate memory, behavior and executive functions.
Research confirms that variation in the genes regulating the dopaminergic brain system modulate differences in processing in the striatum, prefrontal cortex areas and limbic system, which are centrally involved in decision-making, reinforcement learning and risk assessment [19,20].
Thus, the aim of this paper is to determine whether there are connections among temperament, personality traits and genetic variants in the area of the dopamine receptor gene DRD2 in MMA athletes.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Research Groups

Both the sport and control groups in this study were of Caucasian origin and lived in a single Polish region. The experiment was based on a group of 85 healthy (no prior history of substance dependency or psychosis) Polish mixed martial arts (MMA) athletes aged 25.95 ± 6.69. The study population was classified as ‘sub-elite’ (participants in international competitions with no less than eight years of training experience). Various methods were used to obtain the samples, including targeting national teams and providing information to national coaching personnel and athletes attending training camps. Controls included 284 healthy (non-dependent and non-psychosis), non-MMA-athletic Polish male volunteers aged 22.89 ± 4.77. All athletes and controls were Caucasian to reduce the possibility of racial gene skewing and to overcome any potential problems due to population stratification (Table 1).

2.2. DNA Isolation and Genotyping

A standard procedure of collecting venous blood was applied to obtain genomic DNA that was used for genotyping in accordance with the real-time PCR method. Genotyping of rs1799732 in the DRD2 gene was performed by fluorescence resonance energy transfer in a LightCycler 480 II System (Roche Diagnostic, Basel, Switzerland) according to the standard manufacturer’s protocols.

2.3. Psychometric Tests

The following psychometric tests were also performed. The Revised Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI-R) is a self-report questionnaire assessing the four aspects of temperament (harm avoidance, novelty seeking, reward dependence and persistence) and three higher-order character dimensions (self-directedness, cooperativeness and self-transcendence). Temperament refers to individual differences in perception-based habits and skills that are regulated by the limbic system and measured by four independently inherited dimensions that are moderately stable throughout life: novelty seeking (NS) refers to a tendency toward exploratory activities in response to novelty and is hypothesized to be mediated by a dopaminergic behavioral activation system; harm avoidance (HA) refers to pessimistic worrying in anticipation of problems and is hypothesized to be mediated by a serotonergic behavioral inhibition system; reward dependence (RD) is defined as a tendency to maintain behaviors in response to reward by others and is mediated by a noradrenergic behavioral maintenance subsystem; persistence(PS) is an independent dimension and refers to a tendency to perseverance despite frustration and fatigue [12,16].

2.4. Statistical Analysis

The DRD2 rs1799732 genotype’s distribution was tested according to the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) with the following HWE software: https://wpcalc.com/en/equilibrium-hardy-weinberg/ (accessed on 3 June 2021).
The analyzed variables did not have a normal distribution, so a Mann-Whitney U test was applied to determine the differences in the analyzed traits of novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, self-management, cooperation ability and self-transcendence skills.
Not all assumptions required for analysis of variance (ANOVA) analysis were met. The assumption of a normal distribution was not fulfilled for all dependent variables, but the variance was the same (Levene’s test p > 0.05). Because the numbers of subjects in the groups were large, a multivariate analysis 2 × 3 factorial ANOVA was decided upon. The test was used to reveal associations of novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, self-management, cooperation ability and self-transcendence skills in the MMA and control groups with the DRD2 rs1799732 polymorphism (control × personality traits; MMA subjects × genetic feature).
The frequencies of the genotypes and alleles of the DRD2 rs1799732 polymorphism in the analyzed groups were compared by the chi-squared test. All analyses were performed using STATISTICA 13 (Tibco Software Inc., Palo Alto, CA, USA) for Windows (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, WA, USA)

3. Results

The frequency distributions were in accordance with the HWE. There was no statistical differences between the MMA and control subjects (Table 2).
The DRD2 rs1799732 genotypes’ and alleles’ frequencies in the studied sample did not differ between the subjects of the two analyzed groups (Table 3).
The means and standard deviations for novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, self-management, cooperation ability and self-transcendence skills in the MMA and control subjects are presented in Table 3. In comparison with the controls, the case group subjects had significantly higher scores for self-management (M 26.59 vs. M 23.88, p < 0.0001) and lower scores for harm avoidance (M 9.92 vs. M 11.35, p = 0.019) and reward dependence (M 9.50 vs. M 10.47, p = 0.008; Table 4).

3.1. Reward Dependence and DRD2 rs1799732

The results of a 2 × 3 factorial ANOVA of the MMA and control subjects were found for reward dependence (F1363 = 10.53, p = 0.0013, η2 = 0.028; Table 5). The analysis had a 90% power to detect in the MMA and control subjects the effects of the studied reward dependence and the interaction (about 3% of the phenotype variance). We found a statistically significant effect of a complex factor of the DRD2 rs1799732 genotype on reward dependence in both groups (F2363 = 3.11, p = 0.0458, η2 = 0.017; Table 5 and Figure 1). The analysis had a more than 59% power to detect the complex factor of MMA/control x DRD2 rs1799732 and their interaction effect they exert (about 2% of the phenotype variance).

3.2. Cooperative Abilities and DRD2 rs1799732

The results of a 2 × 3 factorial ANOVA of cooperative abilities were found for the MMA group in relation to the DRD2 rs1799732 genotype (F2363 = 3.24, p = 0.0402, η2 = 0.018; Table 5). The analysis had a more than 61% power to detect the DRD2 rs1799732 genotype’s effects in the studied group on their cooperative abilities, along with the interaction effect (about 2% of the phenotype variance).

4. Discussion

The goal of every athlete is to achieve the maximum level of body adaptation to exercise and significant sporting success. High-performance athletes are subject to enormous training loads, which can lead to an imbalance between training and regeneration, as well as having a negative impact on the athlete’s psyche.
Researchers generally agree that sporting development is based on mutual influences of athlete’s predispositions, environment, practice and training [21]. At the same time, when we consider sport talents, we can observe an increased tendency to limit the concentration on physiology and anthropometry and promote the psychological aspect as the key element of talent development [21,22,23]. However, there are only a few publications concerning the genetic conditioning of the psychological traits of athletes, despite great progress in the direction of understanding the genetics of physical fitness [24] and previous encouragement for psychological testing [25,26].
Motivational behavior is usually augmented in the event of increased reward-related activity and is connected with increased synaptic DA availability. Mutations in DRD4 gene are connected with different behavioral phenotypes, for instance, risk-taking [27,28]. There are a few dozen polymorphic loci in a promoter region of DRD4. Research has, for instance, indicated that allele C of the rs1800955 polymorphism is connected with novelty seeking and extraversion. The other polymorphism is a variation of tandem repeats (VNTR) of 48 base pairs in exon 3, ranging from 2-repeat (2R) to 11-repeat (11R) alleles [29]. People with a 7R allele engage in increased levels of physical activity [30,31,32] and are more sensitive to external factors [33,34,35]. The 7R allele is also connected with risky financial decisions [36,37], concentration deficit/hyperactivity [30,35], alcoholism [38] and over-sensitiveness [39]. Research on a group of MMA athletes showed that they have an increased frequency of 7R alleles in comparison with a control group [40].
In our research, we observed that the controls, in comparison with the case group subjects, scored significantly higher on the scale of self-management (M 26.59 vs. M 23.88, p < 0.0001), while at the same time, scoring lower on the scales of harm avoidance (M 9.92 vs. M 11.35, p = 0.019) and reward dependence (M 9.50 vs. M 10.47, p = 0.008; Table 4). What is more, we noted a statistically significant effect of a complex factor of the DRD2 rs1799732 genotype on reward dependence in both groups (F2363 = 3.11, p = 0.0458, η2 = 0.020; Table 5 and Figure 1). It was found that the DRD2 rs1799732 genotype was related to cooperation ability (F2363 = 3.24, p = 0.0402, η2 = 0.018; Table 5).
In this study, we observed a distinct connection between personality traits and genetic variants in the area of the dopamine receptor gene DRD2 among MMA athletes. However, a similar study of 109 normal healthy people did not confirm the association of DRD2 genotypes with TCI scales (NS, harm avoidance, reward dependence and PS) [41]. It is clear when we consider the aforementioned relation of the dopaminergic system with sport activity, but additionally, we have discovered new aspects of athletes’ characteristics and an importance of psychological features that not only predispose people to practice their chosen sport disciplines, but are also characteristic of successful athletes. In a study by Abrahams et al. (2019) among professional rugby players, the DRD4 rs1800955 CC genotype and the inferred DRD2 (rs12364283–rs1076560)–DRD4 (rs1800955) A-C-C allele combination were over-represented in the control group. The results indicated that the rs1800955 C allele was related to reduced concussion susceptibility. The authors hypothesized that the C allele may stimulate DRD4 expression, increasing the D4 receptor’s availability to dopamine. Thus, the C allele acts as a neuro-protective response against concussion injury by inhibiting ‘risk-taking’ behavior [42].
There are several forms of evidence that the development of elite athletes is genetically conditioned. However, our present progress in understanding the molecular bases of cognitive skills and personality features among athletes is still at its very beginning. Further research could open up the area to more possibilities of sports psychogenetics applications with the usage of new DNA technologies (for example, whole genome sequencing, genome-wide association study (GWAS), epigenetic, transcriptomic and proteomic profiling) and bioinformatics, to analyze the influence of genes on athletes’ behaviors.

5. Conclusions

Engaging in a high-risk sport (martial arts) may be associated with several personality characteristics. In the present study, we found an interaction between the dopamine DRD2 rs1799732 polymorphism and the athletic status in terms of harm avoidance and cooperation ability. The association with reduced harm avoidance in martial arts athletes, in particular, could modulate athletes’ predisposition to participate in high-risk sport. Behavioral studies of personality traits and their interactions with biological factors, such as personality genetics, constitute a new, promising area of research. Linking personality traits to the basics of inheritance could contribute to improved understanding of sports motivation and behavior.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.G. and M.N.; methodology, J.C.; software, K.C.; validation, K.L., and A.M.-S.; formal analysis, K.C.; investigation, J.M.; resources, P.C.; data curation, P.R.; writing—original draft preparation, M.N.; writing—review and editing, A.G.; visualization, W.C.; supervision, A.G.; project administration, P.C.; funding acquisition, P.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The study was supported by National Science Centre of Poland (no. UMO-2017/27/B/NZ7/00204).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Morris, T. Psychological characteristics and talent identification in soccer. J. Sports Sci. 2000, 18, 715–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Pawlak, M.; Jażdżewska, A.; Leźnicka, K. Can physical activity modulate the pain perception during the ontogenesis? Balt. J. Health Phys. Activity 2019, 11, 90–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Leźnicka, K.; Starkowska, A.; Tomczak, M.; Cięszczyk, P.; Białecka, M.; Ligocka, M.; Żmijewski, P.; Pawlak, M. Temperament as a modulating factor of pain sensitivity in combat sport athletes. Physiol. Behav. 2017, 180, 131–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Bäckmand, H.; Kaprio, J.; Kujala, U.; Sarna, S. Personality and mood of former elite male athletes-a descriptive study. Int. J. Sports Med. 2001, 22, 215–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Fisher, A.C.; Horsfall, J.S.; Morris, H.H. Sport personality assessment: A methodological re-examination. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 2003, 8, 92–102. [Google Scholar]
  6. Baanac, L.J. The psychological profile of Yugoslav boxers. Facta. Univ. Ser. Phys. Educ. Sport 2001, 1, 13–24. [Google Scholar]
  7. Lemieux, P.; McKelvie, S.J.; Stout, D. Self-reported hostile aggression in contact athletes, no contact athletes and non-athletes. Athl. Insight 2002, 4, 42–56. [Google Scholar]
  8. McKelvie, S.J.; Lemieux, P.; Stout, D. Extraversion and Neuroticism in Contact Athletes No Contact Athletes and Non-athletes: A Research Note. Athl. Inside Online J. Sport Psychol. 2003, 5, 19–27. [Google Scholar]
  9. Cloninger, C.R. The Temperament and Character Inventory—Revised, 240-Itemversion (TCI-240); Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University: St. Louis, MO, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  10. O’sullivan, D.; Zuckerman, M.; Kraft, M. Personality characteristics of male and female participants in team sports. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1998, 25, 119–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Han, D.H.; Kim, J.H.; Lee, Y.S.; Bae, S.J.; Kim, H.J.; Sim, M.Y.; Sung, Y.H.; Lyoo, I.K. Influence of temperament and anxiety on athletic performance. J. Sports Sci. Med. 2006, 5, 381–389. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cloninger, C.R. A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1986, 44, 573–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Hollis, S.J.; Stevenson, M.R.; McIntosh, A.S.; Arthur Shores, E.; Collins, W.M.; Taylor, C.B. Incidence, risk, and protective factors of mild traumatic brain injury in a cohort of Australian nonprofessional male rugby players. Am. J. Sports Med. 2009, 37, 2328–2333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Goswami, R.; Dufort, P.; Tartaglia, M.C.; Green, R.E.; Crawley, A.; Tator, C.H.; Wennberg, R.; Mikulis, D.J.; Keightley, M.; Davis, K.D. Frontotemporal correlates of impulsivity and machine learning in retired professional athletes with a history of multiple concussions. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016, 221, 1911–1925. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Michałowska-Sawczyn, M.; Niewczas, M.; Król, P.; Czarny, W.; Rzeszutko, A.; Chmielowiec, K.; Chmielowiec, J.; Grzywacz, A.; Humińska-Lisowska, K.; Lachowicz, M.; et al. Associations between the dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphisms and personality traits in elite athletes. Biol. Sport. 2019, 36, 365–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Cloninger, C.R.; Przybeck, T.R.; Svrakic, D.M. The tridimensional personality questionnaire: U.S. normative data. Psychol. Rep. 1991, 69 Pt 1, 1047–1057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. DeYoung, C. Cybernetic big five teory. J. Res. Personal. 2014, 56, 33–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Wacker, J.; Smillie, L.T. Extraversion and dopamine function. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2015, 9, 225–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Schultz, W. Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends Neurosci. 2007, 30, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Smillie, L.; Wacker, J. Dopaminergic foundations of personality and individual diferences. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Rees, T.; Hardy, L.; Güllich, A.; Abernethy, B.; Côté, J.; Woodman, T.; Montgomery, H.; Laing, S.; Warr, C. The Great British medalists project: A review of current knowledge into the development of the world’s best sporting talent. Sports Med. 2016, 46, 1041–1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. MacNamara, A.; Button, A.; Collins, D. The role of psychological characteristics in facilitating the pathway to elite performance. Part 1: Identifying mental skills and behaviours. Sport Psychol. 2010, 24, 52–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Blijlevens, S.J.E.; Elferink-Gemser, M.T.; Wylleman, P.; Bool, K.; Visscher, C. Psychological characteristics and skills of top-level Dutch gymnasts in the initiation, development and mastery stages of the athletic career. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2018, 38, 202–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Ahmetov, I.I.; Egorova, E.S.; Gabdrakhmanova, L.J.; Fedotovskaya, O.N. Genes and athletic performance: An update. Med. Sport Sci. 2016, 61, 41–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Singer, R.N.; Janelle, C.M. Determining sport expertise: From genes to supremes. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 1999, 30, 117–150. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lippi, G.; Favaloro, E.J.; Guidi, G.C. The genetic basis of human athletic performance. Why are psychol. compon. so often overlooked? J. Physiol. 2008, 586, 3817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Abidin, S.Z.; Tan, E.L.; Chan, S.C.; Jaafar, A.; Lee, A.X.; Hamid, M.H.N.A.; Murad, N.A.A.; Razy, N.F.P.; Azmin, S.; Annuar, A.A.; et al. DRD and GRIN2B polymorphisms and their association with the development of impulse control behavior among Malaysian Parkinson’s disease patients. BMC Neurol. 2015, 15, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Mallard, T.T.; Doorley, J.; Esposito-Smythers, C.L.; McGeary, J.E. Dopamine D4 receptor VNTR polymorphism associated with greater risk for substance abuse among adolescents with disruptive behavior disorders: Preliminary results. Am. J. Addict. 2016, 25, 56–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Chang, F.M.; Kidd, J.R.; Livak, K.J.; Pakstis, A.J.; Kidd, K.K. The world-wide distribution of allele frequencies at the human dopamine D4 receptor locus. Hum. Genet. 1996, 98, 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Faraone, S.V.; Doyle, A.E.; Mick, E.; Biederman, J. Meta-analysis of the association between the 7-repeat allele of the dopamine D4 receptor gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2001, 158, 1052–1057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kluger, A.N.; Siegfried, Z.; Ebstein, R.P. A meta-analysis of the association between DRD4 polymorphism and novelty seeking. Mol. Psychiatry 2002, 7, 712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Li, D.; Sham, P.C.; Owen, M.J.; He, L. Meta-analysis shows significant association between dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Hum. Mol. Genet. 2006, 15, 2276–2284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Belsky, J.; Jonassaint, C.; Pluess, M.; Stanton, M.; Brummett, B.; Williams, R. Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Mol. Psychiatry 2009, 14, 746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Olsson, C.A.; Moyzis, R.K.; Williamson, E.; Ellis, J.A.; Parkinson-Bates, M.; Patton, G.C.; Dwyer, T.; Romaniuk, H.; Moore, E.E. Gene environment interaction in problematic substance use: Interaction between DRD4 and insecure attachments. Addict. Biol. 2013, 18, 717–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Grady, D.L.; Thanos, P.K.; Corrada, M.M.; Barnett, J.C., Jr.; Ciobanu, V.; Shustarovich, D.; Napoli, A.; Moyzis, A.G.; Grandy, D.; Rubinstein, M.; et al. DRD4 genotype predicts longevity in mouse and human. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 286–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Dreber, A.; Apicella, C.L.; Eisenberg, D.T.; Garcia, J.R.; Zamore, R.S.; Lum, J.K.; Campbell, B. The 7R polymorphism in the dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4) is associated with financial risk taking in men. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2009, 30, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Muda, R.; Kicia, M.; Michalak-Wojnowska, M.; Ginszt, M.; Filip, A.; Gawda, P.; Majcher, P. The dopamine receptor D4 Gene (DRD4) and financial risk-taking: Stimulating and instrumental risk-taking propensity and motivation to engage in investment activity. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. MacKillop, J.; Menges, D.P.; McGeary, J.E.; Lisman, L.A. Effects of craving and DRD4 VNTR genotype on the relative value of alcohol: An initial human laboratory study. Behav. Brain Funct. 2007, 3, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Congdon, E.; Lesch, K.P.; Canli, T. Analysis of DRD4 and DAT polymorphisms and behavioral inhibition in healthy adults: Implications for impulsivity. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2008, 147, 27–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Cherepkova, E.V.; Maksimov, V.N.; Kushnarev, A.P.; Shakhmatov, I.I.; Aftanas, L.I. The polymorphism of dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) and dopamine transporter (DAT) genes in the men with antisocial behaviour and mixed martial arts fighters. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 2017, 20, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gebhardt, C.; Leisch, F.; Schüssler, P.; Fuchs, K.; Stompe, T.; Sieghart, W.; Hornik, K.; Kasper, S.; Aschauer, H.N. Non-association of dopamine D4 and D2 receptor genes with personality in healthy individuals. Psychiatr. Genet. 2000, 10, 131–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Abrahams, S.; McFie, S.; Lacerda, M.; Patricios, J.; Suter, J.; September, A.V.; Posthumus, M. Unravelling the interaction between the DRD2 and DRD4 genes, personality traits and concussion risk. BMJ Open Sport Exerc. Med. 2019, 5, e000465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Interactions between DRD2 rs1799732 and reward dependence (RD) for mixed martial arts (MMA) subjects and control groups.
Figure 1. Interactions between DRD2 rs1799732 and reward dependence (RD) for mixed martial arts (MMA) subjects and control groups.
Genes 12 01217 g001
Table 1. Fundamental biological features of tested mixed martial arts (MMA) group and control group.
Table 1. Fundamental biological features of tested mixed martial arts (MMA) group and control group.
FeatureMixed Martial Arts (MMA)
N = 85
Control
N = 284
Age25.95 ± 6.6922.89 ± 4.77
Body mass83.07 ± 13.0181.31 ± 10.76
Body height180.09181.76 ± 6.07
body mass index (BMI)25.1824.29 ± 3.92
Table 2. Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium of the DRD2 rs1799732 in group of MMA subjects and controls.
Table 2. Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium of the DRD2 rs1799732 in group of MMA subjects and controls.
GroupDRD2 rs1799732
Observed (Expected)Alleles Frequencyχ2p Value
MMA
N = 85
GG71 (69.8)p allele freq (C) = 0.91
q allele freq (A) = 0.09
2.5170.113
GO12 (14.5)
OO2 (0.8)
Controls
N = 284
GG229 (225.4)p allele freq (C) = 0.89
q allele freq (A) = 0.11
4.8700.027
GO48 (55.2)
OO7 (3.4)
p, statistical significance; χ2, chi-squared test result; N, number of subjects.
Table 3. Frequencies of genotypes and alleles of the DRD2 rs1799732 polymorphism in MMA subjects and controls.
Table 3. Frequencies of genotypes and alleles of the DRD2 rs1799732 polymorphism in MMA subjects and controls.
GroupDRD2 rs1799732
GenotypesAlleles
GG
N (%)
GO
N (%)
OO
N (%)
G
N (%)
O
N (%)
MMA
N = 85
71
(0.84)
12
(0.14)
2
(0.02)
154
(0.91)
16
(0.09)
Controls
N = 284
229
(0.81)
48
(0.17)
7
(0.02)
506
(0.89)
62
(0.11)
χ2
p value
0.383
0.826
0.313
0.576
p, statistical significance; χ2, chi-squared test result; N, number of subjects.
Table 4. Analysis of novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, self-management, ability to cooperate and self-transcendence in MMA subjects and controls.
Table 4. Analysis of novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, self-management, ability to cooperate and self-transcendence in MMA subjects and controls.
MMAControlUZp Value
(N = 85)
M ± SD
(N = 284)
M ± SD
Novelty Seeking 20.36 ± 5.4420.27 ± 4.63−0.1520.879
Harm Avoidance9.92 ± 5.0011.35 ± 4.602.3410.019 *
Reward dependence9.50 ± 3.0210.47 ± 2.912.6410.008 *
Self-management 26.59 ± 3.9223.88 ± 5.10−4.3470.00001 *
Cooperative abilities19.87 ± 4.4819,97 ± 4.630.2240.8230
Self-transcendence skills6.65 ± 3.617.06 ± 3.541.0210.3069
M, mean; SD, standard deviation; UZ, U Mann-Whitney Z-test. * Statistically significant between-group differences.
Table 5. Results of 2 × 3 factorial ANOVA for MMA subjects and controls, incorporating novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, self-management, ability to cooperate, self-transcendence skills and DRD2 rs1799732.
Table 5. Results of 2 × 3 factorial ANOVA for MMA subjects and controls, incorporating novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, self-management, ability to cooperate, self-transcendence skills and DRD2 rs1799732.
DRD2 rs17997322 × 3-Factor ANOVA
MMA
(N = 85)
M ± SD
Control
(N = 284)
M ± SD
GG
(N = 300)
M ± SD
GO
(N = 60)
M ± SD
OO
(N = 9)
M ± SD
Full Model
F (p Value)
FactorF (p Value)ɳ2Power
(Alpha = 0.05)
Novelty Seeking 20.36 ± 5.4420.27 ± 4.6420.28 ± 4.8020.87 ± 4.8716.88 ± 4.62F5363 = 1.749
p = 0.1227
R2 = 0.023
interceptF1363 = 714.78
(p < 0.0001 *)
0.6631.000
MMA/controlF1363 = 2.47
(p = 0.1162)
0.0070.348
DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 3.726
(p = 0.0250 *)
0.0200.681
MMA/control × DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 1.67
(p = 0.1900)
0.0030.149
Harm Avoidance9.92 ± 5.0011.36 ± 4.6011.07 ± 4.8010.78 ± 4.5311.33 ± 3.43F5363 = 2.177
p = 0.0561
R2 = 0.029
interceptF1363 = 274.07
(p < 0.0001 *)
0.4301.000
MMA/controlF1363 = 0.40
(p = 0.5265)
0.0010.097
DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 0.57
(p = 0.5606)
0.0030.146
MMA/control × DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 2.20
(p = 0.1121)
0.0120.449
Reward Dependence9.51 ± 3.0210.47 ± 2.9110.35 ± 2.869.62 ± 3.2711.22 ± 3.63F5363 = 3.581
p = 0.0036 *
R2 = 0.047
interceptF1363 = 515.08
(p < 0.0001 *)
0.5861.000
MMA/controlF1363 = 10.53
(p = 0.0013 *)
0.0280.899
DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 1.09
(p = 0.336)
0.0060.242
MMA/control × DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 3.11
(p = 0.0458 *)
0.0170.597
Self-Management26.59 ± 3.9223.88 ± 5.1024.39 ± 5.0924.85 ± 4.5726.00 ± 3.84F5363 = 4.754
p = 0.0003 *
R2 = 0.061
interceptF1363 = 1280.54
(p < 0.0001 *)
0.7791.000
MMA/control ×F1363 = 0.50
(p = 0.4810)
0.0010.108
DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 0.18
(p = 0.837)
0.0010.077
MMA/control × DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 0.95
(p = 0.387)
0.0050.215
Cooperative Abilities19.87 ± 4.4819.98 ± 4.6319.94 ± 4.6219.45 ± 4.3623.67 ± 3.94F5363 = 2.461
p = 0.0328 *
R2 = 0.0327
interceptF1363 = 957.38
(p < 0.0001 *)
0.7251.000
MMA/controlF1363 = 1.82
(p = 0.1777)
0.0050.270
DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 3.24
(p = 0.0402 *)
0.0180.616
MMA/control × DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 2.75
(p = 0.0650)
0.0150.542
Self-Transcendence Skills6.66 ± 3.617.06 ± 3.547.01 ± 3.536.98 ± 3.785.67 ± 2.92F5363 = 1.052
p = 0.3866
R2 = 0.014
interceptF1363 = 147.62
(p < 0.0001 *)
0.2891.000
MMA/controlF1363 = 0.84
(p = 0.3583)
0.0020.151
DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 1.47
(p = 0.2304)
0.0080.314
MMA/control × DRD2 rs1799732F2363 = 1.58
(p = 0.2071)
0.0090.335
M, mean; SD, standard deviation. * Statistically significant between-group differences.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Niewczas, M.; Grzywacz, A.; Leźnicka, K.; Chmielowiec, K.; Chmielowiec, J.; Maciejewska-Skrendo, A.; Ruzbarsky, P.; Masiak, J.; Czarny, W.; Cięszczyk, P. Association between Polymorphism rs1799732 of DRD2 Dopamine Receptor Gene and Personality Traits among MMA Athletes. Genes 2021, 12, 1217. https://doi.org/10.3390/genes12081217

AMA Style

Niewczas M, Grzywacz A, Leźnicka K, Chmielowiec K, Chmielowiec J, Maciejewska-Skrendo A, Ruzbarsky P, Masiak J, Czarny W, Cięszczyk P. Association between Polymorphism rs1799732 of DRD2 Dopamine Receptor Gene and Personality Traits among MMA Athletes. Genes. 2021; 12(8):1217. https://doi.org/10.3390/genes12081217

Chicago/Turabian Style

Niewczas, Marta, Anna Grzywacz, Katarzyna Leźnicka, Krzysztof Chmielowiec, Jolanta Chmielowiec, Agnieszka Maciejewska-Skrendo, Pavel Ruzbarsky, Jolanta Masiak, Wojciech Czarny, and Paweł Cięszczyk. 2021. "Association between Polymorphism rs1799732 of DRD2 Dopamine Receptor Gene and Personality Traits among MMA Athletes" Genes 12, no. 8: 1217. https://doi.org/10.3390/genes12081217

APA Style

Niewczas, M., Grzywacz, A., Leźnicka, K., Chmielowiec, K., Chmielowiec, J., Maciejewska-Skrendo, A., Ruzbarsky, P., Masiak, J., Czarny, W., & Cięszczyk, P. (2021). Association between Polymorphism rs1799732 of DRD2 Dopamine Receptor Gene and Personality Traits among MMA Athletes. Genes, 12(8), 1217. https://doi.org/10.3390/genes12081217

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop