Vertical Symmetry Is Special to Infants; Vertical Symmetry in Upright Human Faces More So
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Infant Perception of Symmetry
1.2. Symmetry in Faces
1.3. The Current Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. General Method for the Three Experiments
2.1.1. Infants
2.1.2. Stimuli
2.1.3. Procedures
3. Experiment 1
3.1. Experiment 1 Method: Materials and Apparatus
3.2. Experiment 1 Method: Procedure
3.3. Experiment 1 Method: Data Analysis
3.4. Experiment 1 Results
4. Experiment 2
4.1. Experiment 2 Method: Materials and Apparatus
4.2. Experiment 2 Method: Procedure
4.3. Experiment 2 Results
5. Experiment 3
5.1. Experiment 3 Method: Materials, Apparatus, and Procedure
5.2. Experiment 3 Results and Discussion
6. General Discussion
7. Limitations and Future Directions
8. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Aristotle. Metaphysics, Volume II: Books 10–14. Oeconomica. Magna Moralia; Number 287 in Loeb Classical Library; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1935. [Google Scholar]
- Darwin, C. The Descent of Man: And Selection in Relation to Sex; John Murray, Albemarle Street.: London, UK, 1888. [Google Scholar]
- Wagemans, J. Detection of visual symmetries. In Human Symmetry Perception and Its Computational Analysis; Tyler, C.W., Ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2002; pp. 25–48. [Google Scholar]
- Attneave, F. Some informational aspects of visual perception. Psychol. Rev. 1954, 61, 183–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garner, W.R. Good patterns have few alternatives. Am. Sci. 1970, 58, 34–42. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Szilagyi, P.G.; Baird, J.C. A quantitative approach to the study of visual symmetry. Percept. Psychophys. 1977, 22, 287–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruce, V.G.; Morgan, M.J. Violations of symmetry and repetition in visual patterns. Perception 1975, 4, 239–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carmody, D.P.; Nodine, C.F.; Locher, P.J. Global detection of symmetry. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1977, 45, 1267–1273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Locher, P.; Krupinski, E.A.; Mello-Thoms, C.; Nodine, C.F. Visual interest in pictorial art during an aesthetic experience. Spat. Vis. 2008, 21, 55–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Locher, P.J.; Nodine, C.F. Influence of stimulus symmetry on visual scanning patterns. Percept. Psychophys. 1973, 13, 408–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoffman, D.D.; Richards, W.A. Parts of recognition. Cognition 1984, 18, 65–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barlow, H.B.; Reeves, B.C. The versatility and absolute efficiency of detecting mirror symmetry in random dot displays. Vis. Res. 1979, 19, 783–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corballis, M.C.; Roldan, C.E. Detection of symmetry as a function of angular orientation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1975, 1, 221–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitts, P.M.; Simon, C.W. Some relations between stimulus patterns and performance in a continuous dual-pursuit task. J. Exp. Psychol. 1952, 43, 428–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitts, P.M.; Weinstein, M.; Rappaport, M.; Anderson, N.; Leonard, J.A. Stimulus correlates of visual pattern recognition: A probability approach. J. Exp. Psychol. 1956, 57, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Julesz, B. Foundations of Cyclopean Perception; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Ortmann, M.R.; Schutte, A.R. The relationship between the perception of axes of symmetry and spatial memory during early childhood. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2010, 107, 368–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Palmer, S.E.; Hemenway, K. Orientation and symmetry: Effects of multiple, rotational, and near symmetries. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1978, 4, 691–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rock, I.; Leaman, R. An experimental analysis of visual symmetry. Acta Psychol. 1963, 21, 171–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deregowski, J.B. Symmetry, Gestalt and information theory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1971, 23, 381–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Appelle, S. Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: The “oblique effect” in man and animals. Psychol. Bull. 1972, 78, 266–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Essock, E.A. The oblique effect of stimulus identification considered with respect to two classes of oblique effects. Perception 1980, 9, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Essock, E.A.; Krebs, W.K.; Prather, J.R. Superior sensitivity for tactile stimuli oriented proximally-distally on the finger: Implications for mixed class 1 and class 2 anisotropies. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1997, 23, 515–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bornstein, M.H.; Ferdinandsen, K.; Gross, C.G. Perception of symmetry in infancy. Dev. Psychol. 1981, 17, 82–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fisher, C.B.; Ferdinandsen, K.; Bornstein, M.H. The role of symmetry in infant form discrimination. Child Dev. 1981, 52, 457–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bornstein, M.H.; Krinsky, S.J. Perception of symmetry in infancy: The salience of vertical symmetry and the perception of pattern wholes. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1985, 39, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wilcox, T.; Stubbs, J.A.; Wheeler, L.; Alexander, G.M. Infants’ scanning of dynamic faces during the first year. Infant Behav. Dev. 2013, 36, 513–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, M.H. Developmental neuroscience psychophysiology, and genetics. In Developmental Science: An Advanced Textbook (7e); Bornstein, M.H., Lamb, M.E., Eds.; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 217–260. [Google Scholar]
- Fantz, R.L. Pattern vision in newborn infants. Science 1963, 140, 296–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Farroni, T.; Johnson, M.H.; Menon, E.; Zulian, L.; Faraguna, D.; Csibra, G. Newborns’ preference for face-relevant stimuli: Effects of contrast polarity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2005, 102, 17245–17250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goren, C.C.; Sarty, M.; Wu, P.Y.K. Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics 1975, 56, 544–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mondloch, C.J.; Lewis, T.L.; Budreau, D.R.; Maurer, D.; Dannemiller, J.L.; Stephens, B.R.; Kleiner-Gathercoal, K.A. Face perception during early infancy. Psychol. Sci. 1999, 10, 419–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turati, C.; Valenza, E.; Leo, I.; Simion, F. Three-month-olds’ visual preference for faces and its underlying visual processing mechanisms. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2005, 90, 255–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samuels, C.A.; Butterworth, G.; Roberts, T.; Graupner, L.; Hole, G. Facial aesthetics: Babies prefer attractiveness to symmetry. Perception 1994, 23, 823–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rhodes, G.; Geddes, K.; Jeffery, L.; Dziurawiec, S.; Clark, A. Are average and symmetric faces attractive to infants? Discrimination and looking preferences. Perception 2002, 31, 315–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haith, M.M.; Bergman, T.; Moore, M.J. Eye contact and face scanning in early infancy. Science 1977, 198, 853–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hunnius, S.; de Wit, T.C.J.; Vrins, S.; von Hofsten, C. Facing threat: Infants’ and adults’ visual scanning of faces with neutral, happy, sad, angry, and fearful emotional expressions. Cogn. Emot. 2011, 25, 193–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Auden, W.H.; Kronenberger, L. (Eds.) The Viking Book of Aphorisms; Viking: New York, NY, USA, 1962. [Google Scholar]
- Haith, M.M. Progress and standardization in eye movement work with human infants. Infancy 2004, 6, 257–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.G.; Buchner, A. G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bornstein, M.H.; Mash, C.; Arterberry, M.E. Young infants’ eye movements over “natural” scenes and “experimental” scenes. Infant Behav. Dev. 2011, 34, 206–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bornstein, M.H.; Mash, C.; Arterberry, M.E. Perception of object-context relations: Eye-movement analysis in infants and adults. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 47, 364–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gredebäck, G.; Johnson, S.; von Hofsten, C. Eye tracking in infancy research. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2010, 35, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zusne, L.; Michels, K.M. Nonrepresentational shapes and eye movements. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1964, 18, 11–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hollingworth, A.; Henderson, J.M. Accurate visual memory for previously attended objects in natural scenes. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2002, 28, 113–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vogel, E.K.; Woodman, G.F.; Luck, S.J. The time course of consolidation in visual working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2006, 32, 1436–1451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oakes, L.M.; Ross-Sheehy, S.; Luck, S.J. Rapid development of feature binding in visual short-term memory. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 781–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cohen, L.B.; Atkinson, D.J.; Chaput, H.H. Habit 2002: A New Program for Obtaining and Organizing Data in Infant Perception and Cognition Studies (v 1.0); Computer Software; University of Texas: Austin, TX, USA, 2002; Available online: https://habit.faculty.ucdavis.edu/ (accessed on 6 September 2023).
- Hood, B.M.; Atkinson, J. Disengaging visual attention in the infant and adult. Infant Behav. Dev. 1993, 16, 405–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corballis, M.C.; Miller, A.; Morgan, M.J. The role of left-right orientation in interhemispheric matching of visual information. Percept. Psychophys. 1971, 10, 385–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corballis, M.C.; Roldan, C.E. On the perception of symmetrical and repeated patterns. Percept. Psychophys. 1974, 16, 136–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Broadbent, D.E. The hidden preattentive processes. Am. Psychol. 1977, 32, 109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cassia, V.M.; Kuefner, D.; Westerlund, A.; Nelson, C.A. Modulation of face-sensitive event-related potentials by canonical and distorted human faces: The role of vertical symmetry and up-down featural arrangement. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006, 18, 1343–1358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Civile, C.; McLaren, R.P.; McLaren, I.P.L. The face inversion effect—Parts and wholes: Individual features and their configuration. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2014, 67, 728–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boswell, S.L. Young children’s processing of asymmetrical and symmetrical patterns. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1976, 22, 309–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hogben, J.H.; Julesz, B.; Ross, J. Short-term memory for symmetry. Vis. Res. 1976, 16, 861–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bornstein, M.H.; Gross, C.G.; Wolf, J.Z. Perceptual similarity of mirror images in infancy. Cognition 1978, 6, 89–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mach, E. The Analysis of Sensations, and the Relation of the Physical to the Psychical; Dover Publications: New York, NY, USA, 1959. [Google Scholar]
- Niimi, R.; Yokosawa, K. It seems to turn away from me: Foreshortened front-back axes bias determination of depth orientation of familiar objects. J. Vis. 2007, 7, 1042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leehey, S.C.; Moskowitz-Cook, A.; Brill, S.; Held, R. Orientational anisotropy in infant vision. Science 1975, 190, 900–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Salapatek, P. Pattern perception in early infancy. In Infant Perception: From Sensation to Cognition; Cohen, L., Salapatek, P., Eds.; Academic: New York, NY, USA, 1975; Volume 1, pp. 133–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cornell, E.H. Infants’ visual attention to pattern arrangement and orientation. Child Dev. 1975, 46, 229–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fagan, J.F. An attention model of infant recognition. Child Dev. 1977, 48, 345–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, M.; Day, R.H. Visual shape perception in early infancy. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1979, 44, 1–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colombo, J.; Laurie, C.; Martelli, T.; Hartig, B. Stimulus context and infant orientation discrimination. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1984, 37, 576–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ghim, H.R.; Eimas, P.D. Global and local processing by 3- and 4-month-old infants. Percept. Psychophys. 1988, 43, 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Giffen, K.; Haith, M.M. Infant visual response to Gestalt geometric forms. Infant Behav. Dev. 1984, 7, 335–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bornstein, M.H.; Mash, C.; Arterberry, M.E.; Esposito, G. Vertical Symmetry Is Special to Infants; Vertical Symmetry in Upright Human Faces More So. Symmetry 2023, 15, 1767. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym15091767
Bornstein MH, Mash C, Arterberry ME, Esposito G. Vertical Symmetry Is Special to Infants; Vertical Symmetry in Upright Human Faces More So. Symmetry. 2023; 15(9):1767. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym15091767
Chicago/Turabian StyleBornstein, Marc H., Clay Mash, Martha E. Arterberry, and Gianluca Esposito. 2023. "Vertical Symmetry Is Special to Infants; Vertical Symmetry in Upright Human Faces More So" Symmetry 15, no. 9: 1767. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym15091767
APA StyleBornstein, M. H., Mash, C., Arterberry, M. E., & Esposito, G. (2023). Vertical Symmetry Is Special to Infants; Vertical Symmetry in Upright Human Faces More So. Symmetry, 15(9), 1767. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym15091767