Next Article in Journal
Understanding Procrastination in First-Year Undergraduates: An Application of Attribution Theory
Next Article in Special Issue
Effectiveness of Regulation of Educational Requirements for Non-Bank Credit Providers in Czech Republic
Previous Article in Journal
Pre-Service Teachers’ Critical Digital Literacy Skills and Attitudes to Address Social Problems
Previous Article in Special Issue
Territorial Intelligence, a Collective Challenge for Sustainable Development: A Scoping Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Graduates’ Opium? Cultural Values, Religiosity and Gender Segregation by Field of Study

Institute for Socio-Economics, University of Duisburg-Essen, 47057 Duisburg, Germany
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9(8), 135; https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci9080135
Submission received: 22 June 2020 / Revised: 15 July 2020 / Accepted: 16 July 2020 / Published: 29 July 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Social, Legal and Educational Challenges of Contemporary Economics)

Abstract

:
This paper studies the relationship between cultural values and gender distribution across fields of study in higher education. I compute national, field and subfield-level gender segregation indices for a panel dataset of 26 OECD countries for 1998–2012. This panel dataset expands the focus of previous macro-level research by exploiting data on gender segregation in specific subfields of study. Fixed-effects estimates associate higher country-level religiosity with lower gender segregation in higher education. These models crucially control for potential segregation factors, such as labor market and educational institutions, and gender gaps in both self-beliefs and academic performance in math among young people.

1. Introduction

Women currently outnumber men in virtually all higher education systems in Western countries. Nevertheless, women and men are strikingly concentrated in specific fields of study. This horizontal gender segregation in higher education results in the over-representation of women in some specific fields (generally in care and humanistic-related fields) and the over-representation of men in others (generally, in technical and science-related fields) (Barone 2011).
Horizontal gender segregation in education is considered an issue of first-order importance insofar as it shapes the skill composition of the future workforce (Altonji et al. 2015) and thus may represent a hurdle for labor market productivity gains and economic development (Dollar and Gatti 1999; Knowles et al. 2002). Furthermore, gender segregation in education accounts for a notable share of the gender wage gap (Brown and Corcoran 1997; Blau and Kahn 2000; Bobbitt-Zeher 2007). Indeed, the female shortfall in the fields of science, technology, engineering and math (STEM) has recently attracted the attention of scholars (Sassler et al. 2017; Card and Payne 2017; Kahn and Ginther 2017) and is a major concern in educational and labor market policy-making (Burchell et al. 2014; Figures 2012).
Social scientists from different disciplines point to socio-economic factors, such as gender differentials in career and family aspirations, gender-based discrimination and cultural values as major causes of horizontal gender segregation in education (Ceci et al. 2014). Yet, the theories of horizontal gender segregation in education have not been systematically examined using actual trends (Mann and DiPrete 2013). Bertrand (2017) argues that the scarcity of women on particular educational tracks might be partly driven by constraints expected by women in the jobs associated with those tracks, and highlights the need for further research to help understand the full set of determinants of current gender disparities in educational outcomes.
This paper seeks to close this gap by focusing on the role that cultural values play in horizontal gender segregation in higher education from a cross-country time-series econometric approach. Anti-egalitarian gender attitudes have previously been found to slowdown gender convergence in labor market outcomes (Fortin 2005). The literature also associates religiosity with more traditional gender roles and less favorable attitudes towards working women (Guiso et al. 2003; Algan and Cahuc 2006). These accounts motivate the current paper to assess the impact of two focal cultural values, namely gender-egalitarian social norms and levels of religiosity, on the gender distribution of higher education graduates across fields of study.
To map segregation trends, I combine national-level measures of gender segregation with disaggregated indices of gender segregation in 9 fields and 23 subfields of study for a panel of 26 Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) countries for 1998–2012. This combination of data allows us to uncover patterns of gender segregation that remain concealed when aggregate data on higher education are used. Hence, I am able to identify the precise fields and subfields that drive national-level gender segregation. Cases in point are of agriculture, a generally male-dominated field made up of a highly male-dominated subfield (agriculture, forestry and fishery) and a highly female-dominated sub-field (veterinary), among other fields of study.
I link the data on horizontal gender segregation in higher education with information on two focal cultural traits: gender equality and religious beliefs. I measure country-level gender equality by means of either the Gender Equality index of the International Institute for Democracy and Electoral Assistance (IDEA). I use the level of religiosity obtained from five waves (1990–1994; 1995–1998; 2000–2004; 2004–2009; 2010–2014) of the World Value Survey (WVS) as a measure of the extent to which social norms are attached to traditional gender roles (Inglehart et al. 2014).
To isolate the impact of cultural values, I control for economic structural changes, labor market and education system features, along with marriage market indicators, such as fertility and divorce rates, as potential determinants of gender disparities in education choices. Finally, I attempt to control for gender gaps in academic performance and self-reported math beliefs among young people that might relate to choices at later stages of their education (Ceci et al. 2014; Eccles and Wang 2016). I use two waves of survey data (2003 and 2012) collected from the Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) to construct aggregate indices of gender differences in anxiety, self-concept and self-efficacy towards mathematics.
The main finding suggests that there is a significant relationship between religiosity and lower levels of gender segregation. The indices of gender equality or inequality are not found to be significantly related to horizontal gender segregation. Gender gaps in math beliefs among young people are found to be correlated with higher gender segregation, which hints at an important link between attitudes acquired in early stages of a lifetime and later education choices. Field and subfield-specific analyses provide a bigger picture of these correlations. The disaggregated results suggest that religiosity might be conducive to lower gender segregation in the fields of agriculture and health and welfare, and more specifically in the subfields of mathematics and statistics, agriculture, forestry and fishery and social services.
The remainder of the paper is structured as follows. Section 2 provides reasons for considering a link between culture and gender segregation. Section 3 describes the data. Section 4 specifies the empirical strategy. Section 5 shows national, field and subfield-level results. Section 6 concludes.

2. Gendered Choices of Field of Study

Standard economic literature considers the choice of major as a dynamic process of decision-taking under uncertainty in which individuals make assumptions so as to infer the outcomes of their specific, field-of-study choices (Altonji 1993; Arcidiacono 2004; Zafar 2013). Those assumptions may include neoclassical economic explanations such as foreseen family burdens and discrimination to explain gender disparities in education choices1. Experimental economics, for its part, seems to debunk the often-repeated arguments of innate gender differentials in cognitive skills by showing that gender gaps in risk-taking, competitive-leaning and social beliefs drive gendered choices of fields of study (Croson and Gneezy 2009; Buser et al. 2014).
Parallel to these explanations, economic research on cultural values emphasizes the role of gender identity and social norms in shaping the economic behavior of people (Guiso et al. 2006; Blau et al. 2013; Giuliano 2017) (see footnote 1). The shift from traditional to egalitarian social norms regarding gender roles has paved the way towards gender convergence in educational investment and labor market outcomes (Fortin 2005; Mandel and Semyonov 2006). On this bedrock of cultural values, Guiso et al. (2003) affirms that religion is likely to affect every aspect of life in society. Using World Value Survey data, they associate religiosity with less favorable attitudes towards working women. Algan and Cahuc (2006) assess the attachment of religion to traditional family values that favor a male breadwinner division of labor. They document differences between religion denominations, in which Catholics and Muslims are more likely to agree with traditional gender role prescriptions than Protestants or non-religious people. Based on these different prescriptions on the role of working women across societies, one might consider that culture can either encourage or hinder gender divergence in the choices of major in higher education.
The epidemiological methodology developed in Fernández (2011) reinforces the explanatory power of the intergenerational transmission of gender norms on gender disparities in both individual and constrained preferences in the labor market and educational choices (Farré and Vella 2013; van de Werfhorst 2017; Charles et al. 2018). However, the role of culture has not been addressed in international comparisons of horizontal gender segregation in education in depth due to scarcity of data available. Drawing on the empirical evidence supporting the idea that economic outcomes and social beliefs are correlated,1 the canonical arguments of gender segregation are framed in rational choice theory and are divided into demand-side factors (Mincer and Polachek 1974) and supply-side factors (Becker 1957). For recent research, see Goldin (2006, 2014a, 2014b).
The current paper considers whether cultural values (e.g., gender equality and religion) play a role in horizontal gender segregation in higher education. Gender segregation explanations drawn from prior mathematical achievement have been steadily replaced by findings suggesting that gender disparities in perceived ability have stronger effects (Friedman-Sokuler and Justman 2016; Justman and Méndez 2018). Eccles and Wang (2016) use survey data on 1200 college-bound students in Michigan (U.S.) to study whether their self-concept of math ability in 12th-grade (ages 17–18) encouraged them to choose STEM occupations at age 29. Their results indicate that gender differences in the likelihood of entering STEM careers were strongly predicted by math self-concept, together with lifestyle expectations, demographics and high school course-taking, rather than by actual math performance. In a similar vein, Shi (2018) uses data in the transition from high school to college for North Carolina (U.S.) to study female under-representation in engineering. She finds that the scarcity of women in engineering is partly explained by their relative lack of confidence in math abilities, but she finds gender disparities in preferences and professional goals to have stronger explanatory power. Ultimately, these analyses disentangle the segregative effects of sex differences in preferences and aspirations from those arising from disparities in math performance and math-ability perceptions.
This paper adopts a macro-level approach grounded on two earlier works on gender segregation across fields of study: First, the paper by Charles and Bradley (2009), which uses a cross-country analysis of gender segregation in four fields of study for 44 countries in 1999; and second, the panel data analysis of US graduates in 225 fields between 1975–2002 by England and Li (2006). I depart from these previous papers by conducting a panel data analysis of gender segregation at national, field and subfield levels and focusing on cultural values while using more nuanced measures of gender gaps in math beliefs. Hence, my approach is intended to tackle both within-country time dynamics of segregation and by-subfield heterogeneity within gender-dominated fields (e.g., veterinary versus forestry within agriculture). Due to data limitations, I can only test macro-level relationships between cultural and horizontal values. Cohort-data research finds gender differentials in education outcomes on the basis of demographics, such as immigration (Alonso-Villar et al. 2012), socioeconomic status (Bailey and Dynarski 2011; van de Werfhorst 2017), parents’ educational attainment and labor market participation rates (Fernández 2011; Farré and Vella 2013), role models of teachers and parental expectations (Bettinger and Long 2005; Xie and Shauman 2003) and peer-related processes (Schoon et al. 2014). The potential intersection between gender and demographics is left for future research.

3. Data on Gender Segregation

The OECD Education Database classifies the number of female and male graduates based on the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED1997) in 9 broad fields of study (1 digit-level) and 23 narrow fields of study (2 digit-level), which I refer to as subfields (see Table A1 in Appendix A). I collect data for 26 OECD countries for 1998–20122. The selection of these countries corresponds to the availability of data and the quality thereof, which is the highest coverage possible using OECD countries. Using data on graduate completion instead of enrolment rates mitigates issues of attrition in gender-atypical choices, specifically in female students (Mastekaasa and Smeby 2008). To the best of my knowledge, this data allows for the greatest country coverage, time span and data disaggregation to compute gender segregation indices in the context of Western countries, although some limitations in the data collection and harmonization across countries might be expected. I use two nominal measures of gender segregation: The Dissimilarity Index (Duncan and Duncan 1955) and the Association Index (Charles and Grusky 1995). The former provides information at the national-level and the latter at field or subfield-levels of segregation3.

3.1. Country-Level Segregation: Dissimilarity Index

The index of dissimilarity (ID hereafter) was first developed in racial segregation studies by Duncan and Duncan (1955). The ID is one of the primary measures of segregation applied to the context of gender segregation in labor markets and education (Gelbgiser and Albert 2018). It is given by the following formula4:
I D = 1 2   | F i F M i M | * 100
where Fi and Mi are females and males in field or subfield i, F and M are the total numbers of female and total male graduates, respectively. As defined in Duncan and Duncan (1955), the ID provides the percentage of women who would have to change fields without replacement in order to make their distribution identical to that of men. The index takes values from 0%, indicating total gender integration across fields, to 100%, indicating complete gender segregation.
Figure 1a shows the trend of the sample average ID computed based on broad (ID at 1 digit-level, blue line) and narrow (ID at 2 digit-level, red line) classifications of higher education. The ID is sensitive to the techniques and categorizations used in defining fields (Reskin 1993; Nelson 2017). Consequently, the ID can be manipulated into being smaller (by using very broad categories) or larger (by using narrow categories). This sensitivity is evident in the different average levels taken by the ID in broad or narrow categorizations (disaggregation at 1 digit-level vs. 2 digit-level), where the latter give higher figures for segregation. Regardless of the category used to compute the ID, the indices show a decreasing trend in 1998–2012, with a drop of around 3 percentage points (pp) by the end of the period. However, ID values remain quite stable throughout this period in comparison with de-segregative fashion taken from 1970 to 1990 (England and Li 2006; Mann and DiPrete 2013; Bronson 2015). This might feed into the afore-mentioned slowdown in gender integration in higher education and other areas of society since the mid-1990s (see inter alia Blau et al. (2006); Olivetti and Petrongolo (2016)).
Figure 1b shows average levels of the ID computed at 1 digit-level (blue) and 2 digit-level (red) for each country in the sample. Turkey is the least segregated country in the sample (an ID of 17.1% at 1 digit-level), whereas Finland is the most segregated (42.1%). Cross-country comparisons show that more affluent, more gender-egalitarian countries have greater segregation (e.g., Scandinavian countries5).
Current cross-national comparisons challenge rational choice theories that predict less segregation as societies become economically richer and gender egalitarian (see Estevez-Abe (2005)). Economists note that gender disparities that do not clearly define hierarchical structures relative to vertical disparities are less easily undermined (Goldin 2006; Shavit 2007). Thus, horizontal segregation in higher education can reconcile gender-egalitarian and gender-essentialist values to a greater extent6. Indeed, this conundrum is already formulated as the education-gender-equality paradox in other social science disciplines (Stoet and Geary 2018), as gender-essentialism has been already explored in the context of choice of majors (Ochsenfeld 2016).

3.2. Field and Subfield-Level Segregation: Association Index

I combine the data on country-level gender segregation with data on field-level segregation and subfield-level segregation. To that end, I use the log-linear modeling approach from Charles and Grusky (1995), namely the Association Index (Ai henceforth), which provides the factor at which each field or subfield of study is associated with a gender (female or male)7. The Ai index is computed as follows8:
A = l n F i M i { 1 j *   l n ( F i M i ) }
where ln is the natural logarithm, j is the number of fields (this number is 9 when the ISCED1997 1 digit-level is used and 23 for the ISCED1997 2 digit-level), Fi is the number of women in field i and Mi is the number of men in field or subfield i. Positive values of the Ai indicate that the field is associated with women, near zero values indicate gender-neutrality, and negative values that the field is associated with men. A well-suited feature of the association index is that it compares the extent of segregation of male-dominated and female-dominated fields or subfields.
Figure 2 shows the average factor of gender-labeling of fields ordered from most male-dominated to the most female-dominated for 1998–2006 and 2007–2012. Engineering is the most segregated field that happens to be male-dominated, showing an Ai of −1.5. Science and agriculture are also male-dominated, although to a lesser extent than engineering. Fields placed in the middle of the table, with values around zero, are gender-neutral fields (services and social sciences). Humanities and arts comprises a female-dominated field, with values close to 0.5. Finally, education and health and welfare are the most female-dominated fields with values around 1, and are the most segregated fields after engineering.
Figure 2 also shows that the gender labeling of fields remains similar before and after the Great Recession, although agriculture and humanities are slightly less segregated and science is more segregated on average in 2007–2012. This descriptive data is consistent with the care-technical and humanistic-scientific divides highlighted in Barone (2011).
Figure 3 reveals high heterogeneity in gender-labeling within fields of study. The field of engineering is divided into three subfields with varying factors of gender-labeling: Manufacturing is slightly male-dominated, with an index close to zero (−0.16), whereas engineering and architecture are more male- dominated with values of −1.7 and −0.83. The overall male-dominated fields of science and agriculture have also female-dominated subfields, such as life science and veterinary studies. Similarly, the field of services is made up of highly male-labeled subfields (transport services and security services) plus a female-labeled subfield (personal services). The most segregated subfields are engineering (male-labeled) and social services (female-labeled). Averages from before-and-after the Great Recession show that computing and veterinary are more segregated in 2007–2012, whereas security services and personal services are less segregated in this latter period.

4. Empirical Strategy

The hypothesis that I test is whether cultural values play a role in the gender distribution across fields of study in higher education. I first specify panel data regression models using country-level segregation (ID) as the left-hand-side (LHS) variable, computed using either a broad (field) or narrow (subfield) classification of higher education.
I D c t = β 0 + β 1 C u l t u r a l V a l u e s c , t 4 + X c , t 4 J β 2 + γ t + α c + u c t c = c o u n t r y ; t = y e a r
where IDct is the dissimilarity index in country c in year t, γt and αc are time and country fixed-effects, respectively. CulturalValuesc,t−4 is the focal explanatory variable referring to either country-level gender equality or religiosity. Xc,t−4 is a set of control variables. Following England and Li (2006), I lag the full set of independent variables four years behind the dependent variable to alleviate causality issues. Considering that the data covers all types of higher education graduates (2-year college, bachelor’s degree, master’s degree and Ph.D.) a time span of four years to completion might be reasonable. Using lags of proxies for cultural values, like religiosity, can alleviate reverse causation between education and religiosity9. I am aware of the difficulty of interpreting the results below as causal effects, so I follow the literature to ease the exposition of the results by talking about “impacts” or “effects”. Provided the difficulty of solving for multiple sources of endogeneity common to fixed-effects panel data models (inter alia, variable biased, reverse causation), the reader should interpret the results below as mere correlations. Baseline models are computed based on information for 26 countries, although the sample of countries is reduced to 18 when WVS data are used and to 17 for PISA data (see summary statistics and sample countries in Table A2, Appendix A).

4.1. Measures of Cultural Values: Gender Equality and Religiosity

I measure gender-egalitarian values by employing the IDEA Gender Equality index. This index is operationalized using five indicators: Power distribution by gender, female participation in civil society organizations, the ratio between mean years of schooling for women and men, the proportion of lower chamber female legislators, and the proportion of women in ministerial- level positions (Skaaning 2017)10.
I measure country-level religiosity using five waves (1990–1994; 1995–1998; 1999–2004; 2005–2008; 2010–2012) of the WVS. As in the reference literature (Guiso et al. 2003), religiosity is measured by the proportion of WVS respondents who, on a 0–10 scale, give a score of 10 for the statement “God is very important in my life”. This statement is present in all WVS waves, whereas other religion-related WVS questions were asked in fewer waves. Average values for Gender Equality and religiosity by country can be found in Figure A1 and Figure A2 shows the evolution of the sample mean of these covariates over time (Appendix C).
Gender-unequal cultural values are thought to reinforce gender-essentialist ideals, i.e., widely shared beliefs that women are better at caring, nurturing and human interaction, whereas men excel at abstract thinking, problem solving and analysis (Sikora and Pokropek 2012; Charles et al. 2015). Anti-egalitarian values might be expected to shape gendered identities of individual men and women to encourage the choice of gender-confirming fields of study, and thus increase segregation. At the same time, the evidence states that more religious ideologies go in lockstep with traditional division of labor and gender roles, which might lead to the expectation of higher levels of segregation in more religious societies.
Figure 4 challenges this view by showing scatter plots of the three alternative proxies of cultural values and gender segregation at national level (dissimilarity index) in 2012. Religiosity is negatively correlated with segregation, meaning that in less gender-egalitarian and more religious societies, gender segregation is lower. By contrast, greater gender equality is positively correlated with horizontal gender segregation in higher education.

4.2. Control Variables

The term Xc,t−4 is a vector of variables measuring economic, labor market and educational institutions that previous literature has related to horizontal gender segregation. These variables are the population density (Pop. density), the share of employees in the service sector to total employment (% Services) and the female labor force participation rate (Female Labor Force). At the same time, I include the percentage of professionals who are female (% Prof. Fem). Regarding educational features, the models include the number of graduates as a proportion of the total population (Size Grads), the percentage of women in the total graduate student body (% Grad. Fem) and the breadth of vocational education via the number of graduates in ISCDE1997 level 5 Type B as a proportion of total higher education (Diversification), as well as the gender gap in academic performance (boys’ scores minus those of girls) in secondary education (Performance Gap).
The marriage market-related covariates, specifically fertility and divorce rates and gender gaps in math beliefs are also considered. The set of controls also includes fertility and divorce rates. Goldin (2006) argues that these indicators were among the underpinnings of the transformation of women’s role in the labor market from a job-focus to a career-design in the aftermath of World War II. They might in turn foster a convergence between men’s and women’s choices of education paths. Along these lines, past papers find that gender discrepancies in marriage aspirations and family formation plans to impact on the share of women in math-related and female higher educational attainment (Badgett Lee and Folbre 2003; Ceci et al. 2014; Bronson 2015; Attanasio and Kaufmann 2017). The current paper controls for these marriage market features, supplementing existing international analyses of segregation such as that of Charles and Bradley (2009). Further information on data sources and pairwise correlations of the explanatory variables are relegated to Table A3 and Table A4 in Appendix A.

4.3. Education System and Performance

Attitudes of Young People towards Math

An important contribution of this paper is the consideration of gender disparities in self-believes related to math skills during early stages of the educational career. I use the 2003 and 2012 waves of PISA surveys, which focused on mathematics (OECD 2013). This in-depth focus provides data on self-reported beliefs regarding math anxiety (measured by means of students’ responses about feelings of stress and helplessness when dealing with mathematics), math self-concept (based on students’ responses about their perceived competence in mathematics) and math self-efficacy (based on students’ perceived ability to solve a range of pure and applied mathematical problems).
PISA assesses these self-reported math beliefs on the basis of strong agreement or agreement on a number of items in each dimension, which are relegated here to Appendix B. I compute gender gaps in national-level indices of math anxiety, self-concept and self-efficacy based on average agreement with the items for each dimension11. In virtually all the countries in the sample, girls are more likely to report math anxiety and less likely to report a self-concept of math than boys. As for math self-efficacy, boys generally report higher levels than girls, although there is some heterogeneity depending on the item in question. Based on the sample of countries, girls show higher levels of self-efficacy in items related to equations (first and second order linear equations). However, boys score higher than girls in the rest of the self-efficacy items. I compute gender gaps for these indices based on the gender that shows higher levels of these self-reported math beliefs: Math anxiety gender gaps are computed as girls’ indices of math anxiety minus that of boys, whereas gender gaps in self-concept and self-efficacy are computed as boys’ indices minus those of girls.
Figure 5 shows average gender gaps between 2003 and 2012 in math anxiety (blue points), math self-concept (red diamonds) and math self-efficacy (green triangles) in the sample of countries, which are listed from lower to higher gender gaps in math anxiety. Switzerland, Norway, France and Canada show the biggest levels of gender gaps in math anxiety, while Poland, Turkey, Portugal and the Republic of Korea show the lowest. Gender gaps in math self-concept are larger than for math anxiety, gender gaps in math self-efficacy are closer to those of math anxiety.
The data displays a pattern in which affluent and more gender-egalitarian countries generally have wider national gender gaps in self-reported math beliefs than less affluent countries, as found by Stoet et al. (2016).
Figure A3 in Appendix D provides scatter plots of ID and gender gaps in self-reported math beliefs. In all three cases, the plots tend to positively correlate wider gender gaps for young people with gender segregation in higher education.
To control for gender gaps in math beliefs, I apply a linear adjustment for 2003–2012 under the assumption of an equal year by year change in math beliefs over that time. Notice that segregation data spans 1998–2012 and using lags of math beliefs would substantially reduce the number of years (2008–2012) and thus of observations. Hence, following previous literature, I study the contemporaneous effect of math attitudes of young people and gender segregation in higher education graduates. This data does not measure the effects of gendered attitudes towards math at individual level, but it enables me to assess to a certain extent whether patterns of gender segregation correspond to aggregate-level gender differences in math anxiety, self-concept or self-efficacy. The approach here seeks to supplement the cross-country analysis in Charles and Bradley (2009), in which they include TIMSS data on disparities in affinity for math between boys and girls.
I D c t = β 0 + β 1 C u l t u r a l V a l u e s c , t 4 + β 2 M a t h B e l i e f s c t + X c , t 4 J β 3 + γ t + α c + u c t c = c o u n t r y ; t = y e a r
As in Equation (3), IDct is the dissimilarity index in country c in year t, γt and αc are time and country fixed-effects, respectively. Note that the model includes contemporaneous MathBeliefs (e.g., gender gaps in math anxiety, self-concept and self-efficacy), whereas the rest of independent variables are four years lagged.

5. Results

5.1. Country-Level Analysis

I estimate the model in (3) using the within-group estimator. The Breusch and Pagan post-estimation test confirms the presence of conditional heteroskedasticity in the data, so I use cluster standard errors at the country-level and allow residuals to be correlated within but uncorrelated between countries (Cameron and Miller 2015). The Hausman test’ initial hypothesis that individual-level effects are adequately modeled by a random-effects model- is resoundingly rejected. Among other post-estimation tests, I take the issue of outliers by identifying observations with very large leverage or squared residuals. I use the lvr2plot Stata command (Cox 2004) to analyze high leverage observations such as those for Turkey and Sweden separately. Excluding these two countries from the sample, the results are unchanged.
A potential caveat on the validity of the estimation is concerned with endogeneity issues arising from the relationship between the ID (dependent variable) and the regressor %ttrad.Fem. In separate models, I use the Two Step Least Squared (2SLS) and the number of women in parliaments to instrument the share of female graduates (see Stockemer and Byrne (2011) for a justification of this instrument), and corroborate the main results of the paper. Indeed, post-estimation tests of the 2SLS approach fail to reject the hypothesis that the proportion of females in the graduate body is an exogenous covariate.
Table 1 shows that greater religiosity is associated with lower gender segregation four periods later. Yet the estimates of this effect become less significant when gender gaps in self-reported math beliefs are accounted for. Column 1 estimates a baseline model that includes the main set of control variables. The female labor force variable is associated with a negative impact on segregation, which is consistent with previous research (Ramirez and Wotipka 2001). Increasing female participation in higher education seems to be related to greater segregation by field of study, which is consistent with Charles and Bradley (2009). Nevertheless, that association is not robust to the inclusion of religiosity. The revolutionary indicators in Goldin’s parlance, fertility and divorce, are associated with a significant negative and positive effect, respectively, on segregation. The effect of fertility is highly robust and challenges the idea that reducing fertility might foster a convergence between the educational choices of men and women.
Column 33 (Table 1) introduces the Gender Equality index, and it is not associated with the significant coefficient. Column 3 uses instead the level of religiosity, which enters with a negative and significant coefficient. This finding is in line with recent evidence on the link between more traditional societies and greater participation of women in math-related fields (Friedman-Sokuler and Justman 2016), and the findings related to closer gender gaps in math performance in Muslim countries (Fryer and Levitt 2010). Ultimately, this negative correlation suggests that gender is less salient in higher education systems in more religious societies. This finding is consistent with that brought by Falk and Hermle (2018), who use survey data to provide evidence on that higher gender equality favor the manifestation of gender differences in preferences across countries.
I provide two mechanisms to tentatively explain the negative association between religiosity and gender segregation12: (i) In more religious societies, women play a traditional role in the labor market (e.g., low female labor force participation rates and high fertility rates). As argued in Bertrand (2017), the constraints and challenges that women expect in the jobs associated with certain education tracks make women reluctant to choose them. Thus, if women expect to play a minor role in the labor market, their choices of majors may be less influenced by these future constraints and they will be more likely to opt for male-dominated education paths (e.g., STEM). (ii) In more religious societies, female participation in higher education is relatively lower. Therefore, those women who do access higher education possess an elite identity that encourages them to transgress gender-confirming norms and opt for male-dominated fields (Charles and Bradley 2009).
I test these potential mechanisms by interacting religiosity with either fertility, the female labor force participation rate or the proportion of women in the total number of graduates in separate models. These interactions are not associated with a significant effect, but estimates on the constitutive terms remain similar to the additive model in Equation (3). Due to the limitations of macro-level data used here, it goes beyond the scope of this paper to go further in these explanations.
Column 4 studies whether different religion denominations explain gender segregation by using four waves of WVS data on the proportion of Catholics, Muslims, Protestants and Jews as the main religion denominations in the sample of countries, using the percentage of the total WVS respondents over the five waves used here who claim to belong to a specific religion. However, none of them are associated with a significant coefficient. Columns 6–8 show within-group estimates of Equation (4). The results positively associate gender gaps in math beliefs of the youth with gender segregation. Recall that the anxiety index is composed by the girls’ index minus that of boys, whereas self-concept and self-efficacy are based on the boys’ index minus that of girls. As girls report higher levels of anxiety, the gender segregation of higher education graduates across fields is also higher. Similarly, as boys surpass girls in their sense of self-concept and efficacy towards math, higher education graduates tend to be more segregated. Note that religiosity is not significant when accounting for math anxiety and self-concept gender disparities (Columns 5 and 6) but it remains statistically significant at the 0.10 level when including self-efficacy (Column 7). Table 2 checks the robustness of these results by estimating Equation (3) using the ID at 2 digit-level as the LHS variable. The results are similar to those found using the ID at the broader level.

5.2. Field and Subfield-Level Analyses

Thus far the estimates provide evidence that religiosity may partly matter to country-level horizontal gender segregation, and that gender gaps in math beliefs among young could be a more decisive factor of segregation in later education choices. This subsection seeks to identify whether religiosity and math beliefs matter to the level of gender segregation in specific fields or subfields. The models specified in Equation (4) employ the association index of either field or subfield i, in country c in year t as the LHS variable.
A i c t = β 0 + β 1 R e l i g i o s i t y c , t 4 + β 2 F i e l d W e i g h t c , t 4 + X c , t 4 J β 3 + γ t + α c + u c t i = f i e l d ( s u b f i e l d ) ; c = c o u n t r y ; t = y e a r
where Aict is the gender association of field or subfield i in country c and year t, with αc and γt being country and time fixed-effects. Xc,t−4 is the same set of controls as described above. To alleviate potential omitted variable bias issues, I include the proportion of graduates in each field or subfield of study in the whole of higher education in the set of control variables (F ieldWeightct). By doing so, I also attempt to account for preferences towards specific fields of study of the whole graduate body, which might differ across countries (Alesina et al. 2013).
I first compute 9 models corresponding the 9 fields (broad classification). This step helps to narrow down the focus to estimate the impact of religiosity in specific subfields13.
Before I review the results, it is worth noting that the Aict is a continuous variable: positive values mean over-representation of women in the field, negative values mean over-representation of men and values close to zero mean gender neutrality. Thus, to accurately interpret a significant coefficient of the regressors, one needs to know ex-ante whether the field or subfield at stake is male-labeled or female-labeled. Positive coefficients associated with the regressors in female-dominated fields would imply a positive relation with gender segregation in that it means a perpetuation of females in female-dominated fields. Negative values for the same coefficients would imply a negative effect on gender segregation. In considering male-dominated fields, positive (negative) values associated with the regressors would imply a negative (positive) correlation with segregation. To ease the interpretation, tables of results (Table 3 and Table 4) provide the average gender-label of each field or subfield, with “F” female-domination and “M” male-domination.
Table 3 shows that religiosity seems to be associated with lower gender segregation in specifically four fields of study, namely education, science, agriculture and health and welfare. These findings might shed some light on the correlation between religiosity and lower horizontal gender segregation at national levels. All the models in Table 3 introduce the full set of controls of Equation (5), but I report the coefficients of religiosity, fertility and gender gaps in math beliefs, as they are the main contribution of the paper.
Models in Panel A (Table 3) exclude math beliefs. Fertility is not associated with a significant role in gender-labeling in any field. Religiosity is significantly associated with gender segregation in four out of the eight fields: Education and health and welfare (Columns 1 and 7), Religiosity enters with a negative coefficient, thus Religiosity is associated with reducing segregation; for science and agriculture (Columns 4 and 6), the sign is positive and the fields are male-dominated, thus Religiosity is associated with lower segregation in these fields.
Panels B–D (Table 3) introduce gender gaps in math anxiety, self-concept and self-efficacy, respectively. Field-level estimates tend to corroborate the finding that higher gender gaps in math beliefs are associated with higher horizontal gender segregation. Increasing gender gaps in math beliefs are persistently associated with higher male-labeling in the field of science (Column 4), but their explanatory power varies across math beliefs. However, Column (5) in Panel B associates higher math anxiety gender gaps with lower male-labeled engineering. Once gender gaps are controlled for, religiosity is still significantly associated with lower male-labeling in agriculture (Column 6) and female-labeling in health and welfare (Column 7). That is, the negative association between religiosity and gender segregation is also found in the field-level estimate. The final step in this paper is to regress Equation (5) against the Aict at subfield level. The results in Table 3 suggest that religiosity and gender gaps might be important for the gender-labeling of agriculture, health and welfare, and to a lesser extent education and science. Thus, Table 4 focus on the subfields that make up these specific fields: Science is divided into life science, physical science, mathematics and statistics and computing. Agriculture is divided into agriculture, forestry and fishery and veterinary studies. Health and welfare is divided into health and social services. Recall that education stands alone on the basis of ISCED97; it is dropped from the subfield-level analysis to avoid repetition.
Panel A in Table 4 identifies a significant link between religiosity and lower levels of male-labeling in mathematics and statistics (Column 3) and agriculture, forestry and fishery (Column 5), whereas religiosity is associated with lower female-labeling in social services (Column 8). These estimates suggest the same direction of the link between religiosity and segregation as previously found. When accounting for math beliefs gender gaps (Panels B–D), only the link between religiosity and social services remains significant at the 0.01 level. The estimates in Table 4 (Panel B) provide little evidence of a link between anxiety gaps and segregation by subfields. However, Panel C significantly associates gender disparities in math self-concept with greater segregation in computing and veterinary studies. Panel D associates math self-efficacy gaps with lower segregation in agriculture, forestry and fishery and veterinary studies.

6. Conclusions

Persisting levels of gender segregation across fields of study in Western countries seem at odds with the increase in female participation in higher education. This observation is particularly puzzling against the backdrop of affirmative action, anti-discrimination policies and gender-egalitarian ideals in developed countries. The literature highlights individual factors (gender gaps in preferences and foreseeing family obligations) and external factors (economic structure, institutions, discrimination) as causes of gender segregation. This paper studies whether cultural values, in particular gender equality and religion, play a role in horizontal gender segregation in higher education.
I construct a panel dataset with information on gender segregation indices at national level, at 9-field level and at 23-subfield level for 26 OECD countries for 1998–2012. I link this data with two focal cultural traits: Gender equality, measured on the basis of the Gender Equality measure (IDEA), and religiosity, taken from the World Value Survey. I propose fixed-effects models that control for potential segregative factors such as economic structural change, labor market and educational systems features. The estimates fail to associate gender (in)equality measures with a significant role in horizontal gender segregation. By contrast, religiosity is significantly associated with lower levels of horizontal gender segregation.
I expand the models seeking to control for gender gaps in math beliefs developed during the youthhood. Using two waves of data taken from PISA surveys, I find a contemporaneous association between gender gaps in anxiety, self-concept and self-efficacy with higher gender segregation of graduates across fields of study. These gaps seem to be stronger predictors of national-level gender segregation than religiosity. Field and subfield-levels analyses pinpoint to a robust association between religiosity and lower segregation levels in the fields of agriculture and health and welfare, and more specifically in the subfield of social services.
From a policy viewpoint, the role of religiosity may be controversial. However, the findings regarding gender gaps in math beliefs tend to indicate that efforts to close gaps between boys and girls might enhance a more gender-equal distribution across fields of study in higher education. Nevertheless, it should be stressed that the findings above are based on macro-level data on segregation, and should be taken with caution. Two natural ways to extend this paper would be first to scrutinize whether there is any link between cultural traits and vertical segregation, i.e., gender segregation at different attainment levels within higher education; and second to expand the gender gaps in ability perceptions among young people into other dimensions, such as reading and science.

Funding

This research was funded by Institute for New Economic Thinking - Young Scholar Initiative, MINECO Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness and Gender Studies Ph.D. Theses Grant, University of the Basque Country.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A. Tables in Appendices

Table A1. Fields and Subfields Classification (ISCED 1997) 1 digit-level. 2 digit-level.
Table A1. Fields and Subfields Classification (ISCED 1997) 1 digit-level. 2 digit-level.
EducationTeacher Training and Education Science
Humanities and artsArts
Humanities
Social Sciences, business and lawSocial and behavioral science
ScienceJournalism and Information Business and Administration Law
Life science
Physical science Mathematics and statistics Computing
Engineering, manufacturing and constructionEngineering and engineering trades
Manufacturing and processing Architecture and building
AgricultureAgriculture, forestry and fishery Veterinary
Health and welfareHealth
Social services
ServicesPersonal services
Transport services Environmental protection Security services
Not known or unspecifiedNot known or unspecified
Table A2. Summary Statistics of Explanatory Variables.
Table A2. Summary Statistics of Explanatory Variables.
MeanStd. Dev.Min.Max.N
Gender Equality (IDEA)0.7890.1230.311196
Religiosity22.13815.7997.53275.78168
% Jew0.7461.5340.0527.378168
% Catholic36.03229.5240.15794.400168
% Protestant22.3923.4510.15784.117168
% Muslim7.5823.5840.06698.886168
Pop. Density142.32132.5182.734505.562218
Fem. Labor Force44.8792.6529.18648.452218
% Services67.3217.3649.17182.964218
% Prof. Female49.4247.41530.5164.707218
Size Grads11.5691.4715.82315.012218
Diversification19.116.0420.0460.004218
% Graduates Fem.57.2545.67325.39167.5218
Performance gap4.9847.413−21.0521.36218
Divorce rate2.1670.6870.43.8218
Fertility1.5940.291.0762.23218
Marri. Age (females)28.3392.04823.332.8218
Field weight0.1180.0970.0000.463970
Subfield weight0.0450.0530.0000.322556
Anxiety gap5.324.726−5.04214.17450
Self-concept gap21.519.6584.49341.8450
Self-efficacy gap9.142.8993.15915.78350
Sample of Countries (Columns 1, Table 1): Austria, Belgium, Canada, Czech Republic, Denmark, Finland, France, Germany, Greece, Hungary, Iceland, Ireland, Italy, Republic of Korea, Luxembourg, Netherlands, Norway, Poland, Portugal, Slovak Republic, Spain, Sweden, Switzerland, Turkey, United Kingdom, United States.
Sample of Countries (Data on WVS and math beliefs): Canada, Czech Republic, Finland, France, Germany, Hungary, Italy, Korea, Rep., Netherlands, Norway, Poland, Slovak Republic, Spain, Sweden, Switzerland, Turkey, United Kingdom (not in PISA), United States.
Table A3. Data Sources.
Table A3. Data Sources.
VariableDescriptionData Source
Population DensityNumber of people per square kilometerWorld Bank data
Female Labor ForceFemale labor force participation rateILOSTAT database
% Service Economy Share of employment in service sector to total employment using the International Standard Classification of Occupations (ISCO-88)
% Prof. FemaleShare of females in the occupational status of “professionals” (ISCO-88: group 2)
Size GradsShare of total graduates in higher education to total population in percentagesOECD Educa- tion Database, World Bank
% Graduates Fem.Share of females in total graduates in higher educationOECD Education Database
Performance gapFemale to male ratio of mean scores in PISA, TIMSS and PIRLS international tests from Quality of Education DatabaseAltinok et al. (2014)
ReligiosityShare of WVS respondents who say that “God is important in my life” equal to 10 on a 0 to 10 scale thatWorld Value Survey
Gender Equality (GE)Measure of gender equality in participation in civil society organizations and politics and education (Skaaning 2017)International IDEA
Divorce rateNumber of divorces during the year per 1000 people OECD FamilyDatabase
FertilityTotal number of births per woman World Bank
Table A4. Cross-correlation table.
Table A4. Cross-correlation table.
PDSerProfFLGradDiverGFemPGFertDivCathProtMusJewRelGE
Ser0.017
Prof−0.281−0.367
FL−0.1610.4430.317
Grad0.066−0.0480.2820.232
Diver0.2420.082−0.420−0.3330.044
GFem−0.2790.0640.4910.4710.295−0.519
PG0.3540.073−0.216−0.128−0.3140.300−0.280
Fert−0.2810.409−0.219−0.1450.1000.069−0.028−0.242
Div0.1310.217−0.0820.428−0.0140.1240.016−0.066−0.025
Cath0.001−0.4230.4820.0810.173−0.2300.1560.178−0.619−0.417
Prot−0.1840.415−0.2040.3980.025−0.0960.137−0.1300.1860.276−0.458
Mus−0.095−0.651−0.389−0.790−0.2870.263−0.539−0.1330.576−0.446−0.302−0.303
Jew−0.3300.2770.3330.228−0.001−0.1540.1710.1070.2970.505−0.1180.379−0.114
Rel−0.243−0.3210.169−0.5600.1070.040−0.252−0.1640.389−0.3230.099−0.3110.6840.031
GE−0.1310.6650.0760.7480.023−0.2210.4430.0570.0030.3270.0120.452−0.5980.167−0.633
Anx−0.1080.323−0.3880.209−0.2660.098−0.0900.0090.1220.110−0.1770.340−0.3760.140−0.3750.399
Con−0.0020.285−0.5110.120−0.3780.142−0.2570.1840.0800.169−0.2310.305−0.1520.268−0.0990.244
Effi0.2120.451−0.5090.247−0.3500.010−0.1630.3150.1470.138−0.4390.527−0.3220.005−0.5610.489
PD (Pop. density); Ser (% Services); Prof (% Prof. Fem.); FL (Fem. Labor Force); Grad (Grads Size); Diver. (Diversif.); GFem (% Grads Female); PG (Performance gap); Fert (Fertility); Div (Divorce); Cath (% Catholic); Prot (% Protest.); Mus (% Muslims); Jew (% Jew); Rel (Religiosity); GE.

Appendix B. PISA Assessment of Math Affinities

Table A5. Math Anxiety PISA Questions.
Table A5. Math Anxiety PISA Questions.
QuestionBoysGirlsGirls − Boys
I often worry that it will be difficult for me in mathematics classes56.3762.947.45
I get very tense when I have to do mathematics homework28.0531.993.94
I get very nervous doing mathematics problems28.4732.243.77
I feel helpless when doing a mathematics problem29.2534.995.74
I worry that I will get poor (grades) in mathematics57.7964.416.61
Table A6. Math Self-Concept PISA Questions.
Table A6. Math Self-Concept PISA Questions.
QuestionBoysGirlsBoys − Girls
I am just not good at mathematics (strongly disagree or disagree)63.2652.2711.11
I get good grades in mathematics60.2054.605.64
I learn mathematics quickly58.6922.9240.10
I have always believed that mathematics is one of my best subjects43.5615.8629.76
In my mathematics class, I understand even the most difficult work42.7615.2229.03
Table A7. Math Self-Efficacy PISA Questions.
Table A7. Math Self-Efficacy PISA Questions.
QuestionBoysGirlsBoys − Girls
Using a train timetable to work out how long it would take
to get from one place to another82.9977.675.31
Calculating how much cheaper a TV would be after a 30% discount84.3275.988.35
Calculating how many square metres of tiles you need to cover a floor75.7761.4314.34
Understanding graphs presented in newspapers81.1576.274.88
Solving an equation like 3x + 5 = 1783.885.2−1.40
Finding the actual distance between two places on a map with a 1:10 000 scale67.4448.3619.08
Solving an equation like 2(x + 3) = (x + 3)(x − 3)70.7971.65−0.86
Calculating the petrol-consumption rate of a car68.2544.8223.43
Table A8. Cross-correlation table.
Table A8. Cross-correlation table.
VariablesAnxiety GapSelf-Concept Gap
Self-concept gap0.816
Self-efficacy gap0.3880.383

Appendix C

Figure A1. Gender-related Social Norms.
Figure A1. Gender-related Social Norms.
Socsci 09 00135 g0a1
Figure A2. Evolution of Gender Equality and Religiosity.
Figure A2. Evolution of Gender Equality and Religiosity.
Socsci 09 00135 g0a2

Appendix D

Figure A3. Gender Segregation and Math Self-reported Belief.
Figure A3. Gender Segregation and Math Self-reported Belief.
Socsci 09 00135 g0a3
Figure A4. Women in the LM and HE and Religiosity.
Figure A4. Women in the LM and HE and Religiosity.
Socsci 09 00135 g0a4

References

  1. Akerlof, George A., and Rachel E. Kranton. 2000. Economics and identity. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 115: 715–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Albrecht, James, Anders Björklund, and Susan Vroman. 2003. Is there a glass ceiling in Sweden? Journal of Labor Economics 21: 145–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Alesina, Alberto, Paola Giuliano, and Nathan Nunn. 2013. On the origins of gender roles: Women and the plough. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 128: 469–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Algan, Yann, and Pierre Cahuc. 2006. Job protection: The macho hypothesis. Oxford Review of Economic Policy 22: 390–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Alonso-Villar, Olga, Coral Del Rio, and Carlos Gradín. 2012. The extent of occupational segregation in the United States: Differences by race, ethnicity, and gender. Industrial Relations: A Journal of Economy and Society 51: 179–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Altinok, Nadir, Claude Diebolt, and Jean-Luc Demeulemeester. 2014. A new international database on education quality: 1965–2010. Applied Economics 46: 1212–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Altonji, Joseph G. 1993. The demand for and return to education when education outcomes are uncertain. Journal of Labor Economics 11, Pt 1: 48–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Altonji, Joseph G., Ching-I. Huang, and Christopher R. 2015. Estimating the cream skimming effect of school choice. Journal of Political Economy 123: 266–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Andersson, Ronnie, and Anna-Karin Olsson. 1999. Fields of education and training manual. Manual for ISCED, 97. Available online: http://intranet.vef.hr/other/erasmus/20100614_fields_of_education_and_training_eurostat1999.pdf (accessed on 24 July 2020).
  10. Arcidiacono, Peter. 2004. Ability sorting and the returns to college major. Journal of Econometrics 121: 343–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Attanasio, Orazio P., and Katja M. Kaufmann. 2017. Education choices and returns on the labor and marriage markets: Evidence from data on subjective expectations. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organiza- Tion 140: 35–55. [Google Scholar]
  12. Badgett Lee, M. V., and Nancy Folbre. 2003. Job gendering: Occupational choice and the marriage market. Industrial Relations: A Journal of Economy and Society 42: 270–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Bailey, Martha J., and Susan M. Dynarski. 2011. Gains and Gaps: Changing Inequality in Us College Entry and Completion. Technical Report. National Bureau of Economic Research: Available online: https://academic.oup.com/qje/article/129/4/1553/1853754#173449000 (accessed on 24 July 2020).
  14. Barone, Carlo. 2011. Some things never change: Gender segregation in higher education across eight nations and three decades. Sociology of Education 84: 157–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Becker, Gary Stanley. 1957. The Economics of Discrimination: An Economic View of Racial Discrimination. Chicago: Chicago University Press. [Google Scholar]
  16. Beffy, Magali, Denis Fougere, and Arnaud Maurel. 2012. Choosing the field of study in postsecondary education: Do expected earnings matter? Review of Economics and Statistics 94: 334–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Bertrand, Marianne. 2017. The glass ceiling. In Becker Friedman Institute for Research in Economics Working Paper No. 2018-38. Chicago: University of Chicago. [Google Scholar]
  18. Bettinger, Eric P., and Bridget Terry Long. 2005. Do faculty serve as role models? The impact of instructor gender on female students. American Economic Review 95: 152–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Blackburn, Robert M., Jennifer Jarman, and Janet Siltanen. 1993. The analysis of occupational gender segregation over time and place: Considerations of measurement and some new evidence. Work, Employment and Society 7: 335–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Blau, Francine D., and Lawrence M. Kahn. 2000. Gender differences in pay. Journal of Economic Perspectives 14: 75–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Blau, Francine D., Mary C. Brinton, and David B. Grusky, eds. 2006. The Declining Significance of Gender? New York: Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  22. Blau, Francine D., Peter Brummund, and Albert Yung-Hsu Liu. 2013. Trends in occupational segregation by gender 1970–2009: Adjusting for the impact of changes in the occupational coding system. Demography 50: 471–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Bobbitt-Zeher, Donna. 2007. The gender income gap and the role of education. Sociology of Education 80: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Bronson, Mary Ann. 2015. Degrees Are Forever: Marriage, Educational Investment, and Lifecycle Labor Deci- Sions of Men and Women. Unpublished Manuscript 2. Available online: https://pubs.aeaweb.org/doi/pdfplus/10.1257/jel.20160995 (accessed on 24 July 2020).
  25. Brown, Charles, and Mary Corcoran. 1997. Sex-based differences in school content and the male-female wage gap. Journal of Labor Economics 15: 431–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Burchell, Brendan, Vincent Hardy, Jill Rubery, and Mark Smith. 2014. A New Method to Understand Occupational Gender Segregation in European Labour Markets. Available online: https://op.europa.eu/s/oaNv (accessed on 27 July 2020).
  27. Buser, Thomas, Muriel Niederle, and Hessel Oosterbeek. 2014. Gender, competitiveness, and career choices. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 129: 1409–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Cameron, A. Colin, and Douglas L. Miller. 2015. A practitioners guide to cluster-robust inference. Journal of Human Resources 50: 317–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Card, David, and A. Abigail Payne. 2017. High School Choices and the Gender Gap in Stem. Technical Report. National Bureau of Economic Research: Available online: https://www.nber.org/papers/w24904 (accessed on 24 July 2020).
  30. Carlsson, Magnus. 2011. Does hiring discrimination cause gender segregation in the swedish labor market? Feminist Economics 17: 71–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ceci, Stephen J., Donna K. Ginther, Shulamit Kahn, and Wendy M. Williams. 2014. Women in academic science: A changing landscape. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 15: 75–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Charles, Maria, and Karen Bradley. 2009. Indulging our gendered selves? Sex segregation by field of study in 44 countries. American Journal of Sociology 114: 924–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Charles, Maria, and David B. Grusky. 1995. Models for describing the underlying structure of sex segregation. American Journal of Sociology 100: 931–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Charles, Maria, Corrie Ellis, and Paula England. 2015. Is there a caring class? Intergenerational transmission of care work. Sociological Science 2: 527–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Charles, Kerwin Kofi, Jonathan Guryan, and Jessica Pan. 2018. The Effects of Sexism on American Women: The Role of Norms vs. Discrimination. Working Paper: NO. 2018-56. Available online: ttps://spssi.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/josi.12352?casa_token=-laRExXsnL8AAAAA%3AvfbWXNftT3LnvKiv8MxdGWs4yqklwAY5Mq1zfIFqBts7iSTT1oiYiiw1zDDpD7jYJD7XJ2Uja7O0vQ (accessed on 24 July 2020).
  36. Cox, Nicholas J. 2004. Speaking stata: Graphing distributions. Stata Journal 4: 66–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Croson, Rachel, and Uri Gneezy. 2009. Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature 47: 448–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. de González de San Román, Ainara, and Sara de la Rica. 2016. Gender gaps in pisa test scores: The impact of social norms and the mothers transmission of role attitudes. Estudios de Econom´ıa Aplicada 34: 79–108. [Google Scholar]
  39. Dilmaghani, Maryam. 2019. Education and religious decline: Evidence from the Canadian compulsory schooling laws. Education Economics 27: 287–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Dollar, David, and Roberta Gatti. 1999. Gender inequality, Income, and Growth: Are Good Times Good for Women. Washington: Development Research Group, The World Bank, vol. 1. [Google Scholar]
  41. Duncan, Otis Dudley, and Beverly Duncan. 1955. A methodological analysis of segregation indexes. American Sociological Review 20: 210–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Eccles, Jacquelynne S., and Ming-Te Wang. 2016. What motivates females and males to pursue careers in mathematics and science? International Journal of Behavioral Development 40: 100–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. England, Paula. 2010. The gender revolution: Uneven and stalled. Gender & Society 24: 149–66. [Google Scholar]
  44. England, Paula, and Su Li. 2006. Desegregation stalled: The changing gender composition of college majors, 1971–2002. Gender & Society 20: 657–77. [Google Scholar]
  45. Estevez-Abe, Margarita. 2005. Gender bias in skills and social policies: The varieties of capitalism perspective on sex segregation. Social Politics: International Studies in Gender, State & Society 12: 180–215. [Google Scholar]
  46. Evertsson, Marie, Paula England, Irma Mooi-Reci, Joan Hermsen, Jeanne De Bruijn, and David Cotter. 2009. Is gender inequality greater at lower or higher educational levels? Common patterns in the Netherlands, Sweden, and The United States. Social Politics 16: 210–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Falk, Armin, and Johannes Hermle. 2018. Relationship of gender differences in preferences to economic development and gender equality. Science 362: eaas9899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Farré, Lídia, and Francis Vella. 2013. The intergenerational transmission of gender role attitudes and its impli- cations for female labour force participation. Economica 80: 219–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Fernández, Raquel. 2011. Does culture matter? In Handbook of Social Economics. Amsterdam: Elsevier, vol. 1, pp. 481–510. [Google Scholar]
  50. Figures, She. 2012. Gender in Research and Innovation. STATISTICS and Indicators. European Commission. Brussels: Publications Office of the European Union. [Google Scholar]
  51. Fortin, Nicole M. 2005. Gender role attitudes and the Labour-market outcomes of women across Oecd countries. Oxford Review of Economic Policy 21: 416–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Friedman-Sokuler, Naomi, and Moshe Justman. 2016. Gender streaming and prior achievement in high school science and mathematics. Economics of Education Review 53: 230–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Fryer, Roland G., Jr., and Steven D. Levitt. 2010. An empirical analysis of the gender gap in mathematics. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 2: 210–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Gelbgiser, Dafna, and Kyle Albert. 2018. Green for all? gender segregation and green fields of study in american higher education. Social Problems 65: 564–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Giuliano, Paola. 2017. Gender: An Historical Perspective. Technical Report. Cambridge: National Bureau of Economic Research. [Google Scholar]
  56. Goldin, Claudia. 2006. The quiet revolution that transformed women’s employment, education, and family. American Economic Review 96: 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Goldin, Claudia. 2014a. A pollution theory of discrimination: Male and female differences in occupations and earnings. In Human Capital in History: The American Record. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 313–48. [Google Scholar]
  58. Goldin, Claudia. 2014b. A grand gender convergence: Its last chapter. American Economic Review 104: 1091–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Guiso, Luigi, Paola Sapienza, and Luigi Zingales. 2003. People’s opium? Religion and economic attitudes. Journal of Monetary Economics 50: 225–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Guiso, Luigi, Paola Sapienza, and Luigi Zingales. 2006. Does culture affect economic outcomes? Journal of Economic Perspectives 20: 23–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Humlum, Maria K., Kristin J. Kleinjans, and Helena S. Nielsen. 2012. An economic analysis of identity and career choice. Economic Inquiry 50: 39–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Inglehart, Roland, Christian Haerpfer, Alejandro Moreno, Christian Welzel, Kseniya Kizilova, Jaime Diez-Medrano, Marta Lagos, Pippa Norris, Eduard Ponarin, and Bi Puranen. 2014. World Values Survey: Round Six—Country-Pooled Datafile. Madrid: JD Systems Institute. [Google Scholar]
  63. Justman, Moshe, and Susan J. Méndez. 2018. Gendered choices of stem subjects for matriculation are not driven by prior differences in mathematical achievement. Economics of Education Review 64: 282–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Kahn, Shulamit, and Ginther Donna. 2017. Women and Stem. Technical Report. Cambridge: National Bureau of Economic Research. [Google Scholar]
  65. Knowles, Stephen, Paula K. Lorgelly, and P. Dorian Owen. 2002. Are educational gender gaps a brake on economic development? Some cross-country empirical evidence. Oxford Economic Papers 54: 118–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Mandel, Hadas, and Moshe Semyonov. 2006. A welfare state paradox: State interventions and womens employ- ment opportunities in 22 countries. American Journal of Sociology 111: 1910–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Mann, Allison, and Thomas A. DiPrete. 2013. Trends in gender segregation in the choice of science and engineering majors. Social Science Research 42: 1519–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Mastekaasa, Arne, and Jens-Christian Smeby. 2008. Educational choice and persistence in male-and female- dominated fields. Higher Education 55: 189–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Mincer, Jacob, and Solomon Polachek. 1974. Family investments in human capital: Earnings of women. Journal of political Economy 82, Pt 2: S76–S108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Nelson, Julie A. 2017. Gender and Risk-Taking: Economics, Evidence, and Why the Answer Matters. Oxfordshire: Taylor & Francis, vol. 17. [Google Scholar]
  71. Ochsenfeld, Fabian. 2016. Preferences, constraints, and the process of sex segregation in college majors: A choice analysis. Social Science Research 56: 117–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. OECD. 2013. PISA 2012 Results: Ready to Learn: Students’ Engagement, Drive and Self-Beliefs Volume III: Preliminary Version. Paris: OECD. [Google Scholar]
  73. Olivetti, Claudia, and Barbara Petrongolo. 2016. The evolution of gender gaps in industrialized countries. Annual Review of Economics 8: 405–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Ramirez, Francisco O., and Christine Min Wotipka. 2001. Slowly but surely? The global expansion of women’s participation in science and engineering fields of study, 1972–92. Sociology of Education 74: 231–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Reskin, Barbara. 1993. Sex segregation in the workplace. Annual Review of Sociology 19: 241–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Rodríguez-Planas, Núria, and Natalia Nollenberger. 2018. Let the girls learn! It is not only about math it’s about gender social norms. Economics of Education Review 62: 230–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Sassler, Sharon, Katherine Michelmore, and Kristin Smith. 2017. A tale of two majors: Explaining the gender gap in stem employment among computer science and engineering degree holders. Social Sciences 6: 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Schoon, Ingrid, and Jacquelynne S. Eccles, eds. 2014. Gender Differences in Aspirations and Attainment: A Life Course Perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  79. Semuonov, Moshe, and Frank L. Jones. 1999. Dimensions of gender occupational differentiation in segregation and inequality: A cross-national analysis. Social Indicators Research 46: 225–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Shavit, Yossi, ed. 2007. Stratification in Higher Education: A Comparative Study. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  81. Shi, Ying. 2018. The puzzle of missing female engineers: Academic preparation, ability beliefs, and preferences. Economics of Education Review 64: 129–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Sikora, Joanna, and Artur Pokropek. 2012. Gender segregation of adolescent science career plans in 50 countries. Science Education 96: 234–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Skaaning, Svend-Erik. 2017. The Global State of Democracy Indices Methodology: Conceptualization and Measurement Framework. Stockholm: International IDEA, Available online: https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/canadian-journal-of-political-science-revue-canadienne-de-science-politique/article/is-there-a-distinct-quebec-media-subsystem-in-canada-evidence-of-ideological-and-political-orientations-among-canadian-news-media-organizations/835FC4D4BDAAF96976B53F28D0A05619 (accessed on 24 July 2020).
  84. Stockemer, Daniel, and Maeve Byrne. 2011. Women’s Representation around the World: The Importance of Women’s Participation in the Workforce. Parliamentary Affairs 65: 802–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Stoet, Gijsbert, and David C. Geary. 2018. The Gender-equality paradox in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics education. Psychological Science 29: 581–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Stoet, Gijsbert, Drew H. Bailey, Alex M. Moore, and David C. Geary. 2016. Countries with higher levels of gender equality show larger national sex differences in mathematics anxiety and relatively lower parental mathematics valuation for girls. PLoS ONE 11: e0153857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. van de Werfhorst, Herman G. 2017. Gender segregation across fields of study in post-secondary education: Trends and social differentials. European Sociological Review 33: 449–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Watts, Martin. 1998. Occupational gender segregation: Index measureiient and econometric modeling. Demography 35: 489–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Xie, Yue, and Kimberlee A. Shauman. 2003. Women in Science: Career Processes and Outcomes. No. 73.4. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, vol. 26. [Google Scholar]
  90. Zafar, Basit. 2013. College major choice and the gender gap. Journal of Human Resources 48: 545–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
1
Akerlof and Kranton (2000) provide a game theoretical model that defines an identity-based utility of individual choices. Obeying social prescriptions of one’s identity as a “man” or as a “woman” is rewarded while violating them evokes anxiety and discomfort. Hence, this model defines non-pecuniary benefits derived from the choice of educational paths, as formulated for instance by Humlum et al. (2012) and Beffy et al. (2012).
2
3
In sharp contrast to ordinal measures, nominal measures of segregation do not take into account a hierarchical ordering of the education system (Semyonov and Jones 1999). A large body of American literature on the pay-offs to human capital suggests that generally female-dominated fields (humanities and social science) result in lower incomes than male-dominated fields (scientific and technical fields) (Charles and Bradley 2009). Nevertheless, given the lack of specific data on wages associated with each field or subfield for the sample of countries, the current paper does not distinguish between female and male-dominated fields in any income or social status ordering.
4
Cross-national and inter-temporal comparisons using the ID might entail computational issues due to its sensitivity to the share of fields in total higher education (Charles and Grusky 1995; Watts 1998). If education systems are dominated by one highly segregated field, the ID would yield higher values than if the dominant field was evenly composed by women and men, and numerous small fields were highly segregated.
5
Studies on Scandinavian labor markets (Albrecht et al. 2003; Evertsson et al. 2009; Carlsson 2011) suggest that the disparities in expansion of the welfare state across developed countries (e.g., care work transfers from families to the public sectors), might be a potential driver of cross-country differences in women’s concentration by fields of study.
6
This logic corresponds to “separate-but-equal” gender beliefs as a cause of persisting horizontal gender segregation as suggested by Charles and Bradley (2009) and England (2010).
7
See Charles and Bradley (2009); Barone (2011) and Mann and DiPrete (2013) for applications of the index in the context of segregation in education. Following the sociological literature in which this index was developed, I use the term of “gender-labeling” of fields, although the term “gender-typing” is also used in the literature.
8
The Ai index outperforms ID in cross-country and inter-temporal comparisons as by using log-linear techniques the index is unaffected by the weight of each field in different countries or weight of women. See Watts (1998); Blackburn et al. (1993) for these computational issues of segregation indices.
9
Dilmaghani (2019) founds a causal link between education and religious unaffiliation. Applying this evidence to the case of gender segregation, one might consider the extent to which the advancement of women in higher education might lead to lower religiosity levels in coming generations.
10
In spite of other measures used in related literature, such as the World Economic Forum’s gender gap index in de González de San Román and de la Rica (2016); Rodríguez-Planas and Nollenberger (2018), the IDEA gender equality index is available yearly for the period 1975–2015.
11
PISA provides scale indices of self-reported math beliefs measuring the distance from national levels to average of the total sample of countries participating in PISA surveys. It would be misleading to link these scale indices with my database of gender segregation because my panel is unbalanced and only covers a cluster of OECD countries. Thus, I construct aggregate-level gender gaps in self-reported math beliefs instead of using scale indices in OECD (2013).
12
See Figure A4 in the Appendix A for scatter plots of female participation in the labor force, share of graduates and fertility with religiosity.
13
For the sake of space, all the models of the 23 subfields are not included here, but they are available upon request.
Figure 1. Country-level Horizontal Gender Segregation. (a) Trend; (b) By country.
Figure 1. Country-level Horizontal Gender Segregation. (a) Trend; (b) By country.
Socsci 09 00135 g001
Figure 2. Field Segregation—Association Index.
Figure 2. Field Segregation—Association Index.
Socsci 09 00135 g002
Figure 3. Subfield Segregation—Association Index.
Figure 3. Subfield Segregation—Association Index.
Socsci 09 00135 g003
Figure 4. Gender Segregation and Cultural Values. (a) Gender Equality, (b) Religiosity.
Figure 4. Gender Segregation and Cultural Values. (a) Gender Equality, (b) Religiosity.
Socsci 09 00135 g004aSocsci 09 00135 g004b
Figure 5. Gender Gaps in Self-reported Math Beliefs.
Figure 5. Gender Gaps in Self-reported Math Beliefs.
Socsci 09 00135 g005
Table 1. Country-level Gender Segregation. Dependent variable: Dissimilarity index (1 digit-level).
Table 1. Country-level Gender Segregation. Dependent variable: Dissimilarity index (1 digit-level).
BaselineCultural ValuesMath Beliefs
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)
L4.Pop. density−0.0020.018−0.012−0.220−0.114−0.152−0.092
(0.126)(0.132)(0.102)(0.232)(0.089)(0.107)(0.097)
L4.% Services−0.074−0.035−0.031−0.239−0.2070.012−0.111
(0.198)(0.188)(0.142)(0.173)(0.170)(0.162)(0.152)
L4.% Prof. Fem.−0.038−0.037−0.0610.171 *0.0030.082−0.077
(0.072)(0.080)(0.098)(0.095)(0.077)(0.105)(0.100)
L4.Fem. Labor Force−0.881 **−0.887**−0.801 ***−0.9290.371−1.228 ***−0.592 *
(0.396)(0.386)(0.259)(0.791)(0.532)(0.266)(0.321)
L4.Grads Size−1.796−2.8181.470−7.311 ***2.6921.9741.317
(2.351)(2.496)(2.389)(2.106)(2.371)(2.518)(2.173)
L4.Diversification0.0340.0420.0260.067 ***−0.009−0.0090.019
(0.032)(0.032)(0.036)(0.020)(0.032)(0.033)(0.034)
L4.% Grad. Fem.0.117 ***0.124 **0.0470.037−0.0010.054 *0.043
(0.038)(0.045)(0.031)(0.178)(0.029)(0.028)(0.031)
L4.Performance gap−0.072−0.037−0.071 **−0.040−0.029−0.003−0.058 *
(0.042)(0.040)(0.030)(0.064)(0.036)(0.046)(0.031)
L4.Fertility−7.147 ***−8.241 ***−7.017 ***−11.102 *−9.806 ***−6.013 ***−9.575 ***
(2.501)(2.608)(2.251)(5.593)(3.129)(1.592)(2.570)
L4.Divorce rate1.020 **0.934 *0.2680.0360.3320.2640.667 **
(0.460)(0.451)(0.325)(1.136)(0.270)(0.235)(0.255)
L4.Gender Equality −18.475
L4.Religiosity (0.260)−0.231 *** −0.033−0.081−0.181 **
(0.062) (0.065)(0.050)(0.068)
L4.% Catholic 1.353
(18.047)
L4.% Protest. 18.116
(14.690)
L4% Muslim −25.867
(237.958)
L4.% Jew 34.634
Anxiety gap (316.463)0.637 ***
Self−concept gap (0.193)0.367 ***
Self−efficacy gap (0.109)0.550 *
(0.280)
No. of Obs.21819613675128128128
No. of Groups26231812171717
log−likelihood−391.491−347.718−214.043−104.929−194.005−195.702−200.472
Within R-squared0.3370.3630.4080.5790.4700.4560.414
p value0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
Country-level clustered standard errors in parentheses, * p < 0.1, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01. Within-group estimates including time fixed-effects, constant terms are not reported. The fourth period lagged explanatory variables except for variables of math beliefs.
Table 2. Country-level Gender Segregation (2-digit Level Classification). Dependent variable: Dissimilarity index (2 digit-level).
Table 2. Country-level Gender Segregation (2-digit Level Classification). Dependent variable: Dissimilarity index (2 digit-level).
Cultural ValuesMath Beliefs
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)
L4.Performance gap−0.042−0.031−0.0040.016−0.032
(0.070)(0.033)(0.036)(0.044)(0.031)
L4.Fertility−10.627 **−8.639 ***−10.886 ***−8.046 ***−9.110 ***
(4.764)(2.650)(3.298)(2.324)(2.858)
L4.Gender Equality−25.743
L4.Religiosity(22.314)−0.195 **−0.024−0.060−0.155 *
Anxiety gap (0.080)(0.084)
0.477 *
(0.083)(0.081)
Self−concept gap (0.233)0.274 **
(0.108)
Self−efficacy gap −0.045
(0.331)
No. of Obs.195136128128128
No. of Groups2318171717
P-value0.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
log-likelihood−479.668−204.972−188.434−189.500−193.537
Within R-squared0.1320.4800.5130.5050.472
Country-level clustered standard errors in parentheses, * p < 0.1, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01. Within-group estimates including time fixed-effects, constant terms not reported. Fourth period lagged explanatory variables except for variables of math beliefs. The models include the full set of controls but are not reported.
Table 3. Field-level Gender Segregation. Dependent variable: Association Index (fields).
Table 3. Field-level Gender Segregation. Dependent variable: Association Index (fields).
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)
EducHum & ArtsSoc. SciScienceEng.Agri.HealthServ
Gender-LabelFFFMMMFM
PANEL A:
L4.Fertility−0.125−0.1660.024−0.1830.2260.453−0.085−0.410
(0.222)(0.186)(0.138)(0.187)(0.247)(0.350)(0.119)(0.359)
L4.Religiosity−0.016 ***−0.0010.0020.012 *0.0050.016 **−0.015 **0.005
(0.005)(0.005)(0.005)(0.006)(0.007)(0.007)(0.006)(0.004)
P-value0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
No. of Obs.136136136136136136136136
No. of Groups1818181818181818
log-likelihood131.314185.155201.917123.755168.06191.927159.39992.940
Within R-squared0.3050.3040.2280.2760.4730.2720.2400.267
PANEL B: Math Anxiety Gender Gaps
Anxiety gap0.031 *−0.013−0.002−0.044 **0.040 ***0.0080.039 ***0.057 **
(0.015)(0.016)(0.011)(0.018)(0.014)(0.034)(0.012)(0.023)
L4.Fertility−0.230−0.1180.071−0.1210.0890.325−0.236−0.537
(0.218)(0.145)(0.149)(0.211)(0.202)(0.385)(0.154)(0.347)
L4.Religiosity−0.007−0.005−0.0010.0010.021 ***0.026 **−0.013 ***0.015
(0.009)(0.008)(0.008)(0.008)(0.006)(0.011)(0.004)(0.006)
p-value0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
log-likelihood124.510175.381188.541136.085174.28185.535155.71399.945
Within R-squared0.3170.3380.1980.4250.4980.2710.3270.342
PANEL C: Math Self-concept Gender Gaps
Self-concept gap0.015 *−0.0040.006−0.015 *−0.009−0.0050.0050.051 ***
(0.008)(0.007)(0.005)(0.008)(0.010)(0.021)(0.011)(0.017)
L4.Fertility−0.067−0.1790.088−0.326 *0.2110.344−0.069−0.119
(0.202)(0.185)(0.164)(0.185)(0.195)(0.365)(0.132)(0.291)
L4.Religiosity−0.009−0.0030.0020.0070.0070.023 ***−0.019 ***0.016 ***
(0.007)(0.007)(0.007)(0.006)(0.009)(0.006)(0.006)(0.005)
p-value0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
log-likelihood123.322174.441189.128131.181165.99885.529149.416106.066
Within R-squared0.3040.3280.2050.3790.4290.2700.2580.402
PANEL D: Math Self-efficacy Gender Gaps
Self-efficacy gap0.0440.0000.026−0.069 ***0.008−0.0200.009−0.005
(0.028)(0.028)(0.025)(0.021)(0.028)(0.042)(0.024)(0.050)
L4.Fertility−0.266−0.160−0.025−0.0700.2130.434−0.122−0.324
(0.214)(0.136)(0.154)(0.191)(0.193)(0.414)(0.127)(0.414)
L4.Religiosity−0.013 *−0.0010.0010.009 *0.0100.023 ***−0.020 ***−0.000
(0.007)(0.007)(0.006)(0.005)(0.007)(0.007)(0.006)(0.007)
p-value0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
log-likelihood123.267174.188190.117133.821165.05485.637149.19893.625
Within R-squared0.3030.3250.2170.4040.4200.2720.2550.274
N128128128128128128128128
No. of Groups1717171717171717
Country-level clustered standard errors in parentheses, * p < 0.1, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01. Within-group estimates including time fixed-effects and constant terms are not reported. Fourth period lagged explanatory variables except for variables of math beliefs. The models include the full set of controls but are not reported. Panels B-D include math beliefs and the number of clusters and observations are the same across fields. Educ (Education); Hum & Arts (Humanities and Arts); Soc. Sci (Social Sciences, Business and Law); Science (Science, Mathematics and Computing); Eng. (Engineering, Manufacturing and Construction); Agri. (Agriculture and Veterinary); Health (Health and Welfare); Serv. (Services). To ease the interpretation of the coefficients, behind the name of each field is the sample average gender label of M (male) and F (female), meaning whether the field is male-dominated or female-dominated, respectively. Recall that the dependent variable is a continuous variable ranging negative values for male-dominated fields and positive values for female-dominated fields.
Table 4. Subfield-level Segregation (selected subfields). Dependent variable: Association Index (subfields).
Table 4. Subfield-level Segregation (selected subfields). Dependent variable: Association Index (subfields).
ScienceAgricultureHealth & Welfare
Gender Label(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)
Life S.Phy S.Math.Comp.Agri.Vet.HealthSoc. Serv.
FMMMMFFF
PANEL A
L4.Fertility−0.0050.0090.144−0.3380.3890.3220.234 **−0.425
(0.200)(0.171)(0.224)(0.346)(0.493)(0.452)(0.107)(0.330)
L4.Religiosity0.0090.0010.024 **0.0080.018 **0.015−0.008−0.034 ***
(0.007)(0.006)(0.011)(0.009)(0.007)(0.020)(0.006)(0.010)
No. of Obs.136136136136136136136136
No. of Groups1818181818181818
p-value0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
log-likelihood109.827143.73787.49388.301104.2872.154163.98778.003
Within R-squared0.3090.2100.2390.7310.2620.3700.3320.476
PANEL B: Math Anxiety Gender Gaps
Anxiety gap−0.0420.001−0.012−0.0230.009−0.0300.027 *0.029
(0.025)(0.010)(0.032)(0.024)(0.019)(0.051)(0.013)(0.022)
L4.Fertility0.168−0.0720.151−0.1230.1800.4770.123−0.727 *
(0.286)(0.190)(0.210)(0.316)(0.454)(0.503)(0.124)(0.416)
L4.Religiosity−0.0060.0040.022−0.0010.018 *0.027−0.008−0.043 ***
P-value0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
(0.007)(0.006)(0.015)(0.014)(0.010)(0.016)(0.004)(0.011)
log-likelihood104.780137.05381.97589.082102.64212.700159.60477.704
Within R-squared0.3400.2400.1850.7510.2490.4530.3790.528
PANEL C: Math Self-concept Gender Gaps
Self-concept gap0.0170.002−0.019−0.031 ***−0.0200.044 ***−0.0060.016
(0.014)(0.010)(0.014)(0.010)(0.012)(0.012)(0.012)(0.010)
L4.Fertility0.109−0.0630.030−0.3240.1760.5680.170−0.556
(0.257)(0.180)(0.147)(0.309)(0.440)(0.334)(0.101)(0.356)
L4.Religiosity0.0110.0040.018−0.0050.0110.049 **−0.016 **−0.045 ***
(0.008)(0.004)(0.015)(0.010)(0.006)(0.017)(0.007)(0.009)
p-value0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
log-likelihood102.345137.08083.24592.443104.16514.792156.63077.316
Within R-squared0.3140.2410.2010.7640.2660.4710.3500.525
PANEL D: Math Self-efficacy Gender Gaps
Self-efficacy gap0.0210.0130.070−0.0400.061 **−0.124 **0.024−0.063
(0.036)(0.023)(0.043)(0.030)(0.025)(0.047)(0.017)(0.044)
L4.Fertility−0.040−0.111−0.139−0.076−0.0160.8120.103−0.438
(0.226)(0.200)(0.160)(0.348)(0.436)(0.532)(0.104)(0.330)
L4.Religiosity0.0060.0040.028 **0.0030.018 **0.030 *−0.012 *−0.054 ***
(0.007)(0.005)(0.012)(0.011)(0.006)(0.016)(0.006)(0.008)
p-value0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
log-likelihood101.141137.23284.01988.948104.89214.668157.02078.043
Within R-squared0.3010.2430.2110.7510.2750.4700.3540.531
No. of Obs.128128128128128128128128
No. of Groups1717171717171717
Country-level clustered standard errors in parentheses, * p < 0.1, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01. Within-group estimates including time fixed-effects and constant terms are not reported. Fourth period lagged explanatory variables except for variables of math beliefs. The models include the full set of controls but are not reported. Panels B-D include math beliefs and the number of clusters and observations are the same across subfields. Life S. (Life Science); Phys. S. (Physical Science); Maths. (Mathematics and statistics); Comp. (Computing); Agri. (Agriculture, forestry and fishery); Vet. (Veterinary); Soc. Serv. (Social Services). To ease the interpretation of the coefficients, behind the name of each subfield is the sample average gender label of M (male) and F (female), meaning whether the subfield is male-dominated or female-dominated, respectively. Recall that the dependent variable is a continuous variable ranging negative values for male-dominated fields and positive values for female-dominated fields.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zuazu, I. Graduates’ Opium? Cultural Values, Religiosity and Gender Segregation by Field of Study. Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 135. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci9080135

AMA Style

Zuazu I. Graduates’ Opium? Cultural Values, Religiosity and Gender Segregation by Field of Study. Social Sciences. 2020; 9(8):135. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci9080135

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zuazu, Izaskun. 2020. "Graduates’ Opium? Cultural Values, Religiosity and Gender Segregation by Field of Study" Social Sciences 9, no. 8: 135. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci9080135

APA Style

Zuazu, I. (2020). Graduates’ Opium? Cultural Values, Religiosity and Gender Segregation by Field of Study. Social Sciences, 9(8), 135. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci9080135

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop