Next Article in Journal
Stimulus-Stimulus-Pairing to Reduce Stereotypies in Three Children with Autism during Movie Watching
Next Article in Special Issue
Personality Traits, Cognitive Styles, Coping Strategies, and Psychological Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic Lockdown on Healthy Youngsters
Previous Article in Journal
Reconstructing Attitudes towards Work from Home during COVID-19: A Survey of South Korean Managers
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Self-Esteem on Self-Viewing: An Eye-Tracking Investigation on Mirror Gazing

Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, 8010 Graz, Austria
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11(12), 164; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11120164
Submission received: 14 October 2021 / Revised: 24 November 2021 / Accepted: 26 November 2021 / Published: 29 November 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Personality, Intervention and Psychological Treatment)

Abstract

:
While some people enjoy looking at their faces in the mirror, others experience emotional distress. Despite these individual differences concerning self-viewing in the mirror, systematic investigations on this topic have not been conducted so far. The present eye-tracking study examined whether personality traits (self-esteem, narcissism propensity, self-disgust) are associated with gaze behavior (gaze duration, fixation count) during free mirror viewing of one’s face. Sixty-eight adults (mean age = 23.5 years; 39 females, 29 males) viewed their faces in the mirror and watched a video of an unknown person matched for gender and age (control condition) for 90 s each. The computed regression analysis showed that higher self-esteem was associated with a shorter gaze duration for both self-face and other-face. This effect may reflect a less critical evaluation of the faces.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Looking at one’s face in the mirror can evoke strong feelings. A prominent example from Greek mythology is Narcissus, who fell in love with his own image reflected in a pool of water. However, in everyday life, many people find it distressing to look at themselves [1,2]. For example, in a study by Barnier and Collison [3], five minutes of mirror gazing decreased satisfaction with one’s appearance, perceived attractiveness, and self-esteem. Such adverse effects were especially prominent in those observers who had been dissatisfied with their appearance already before the mirror viewing [4].
An interesting question is: how do people view themselves in the mirror? Elaborate measures such as eye-tracking can help to answer this question. To the best of our knowledge, no eye-tracking studies have been conducted before on the viewing of one’s face in a mirror, which, therefore, was the focus of the present study. Previous investigations have examined gaze behavior toward photos or videos depicting self-face vs. other-face [5,6,7,8,9,10]. These studies showed that the “own face” (compared to the faces of others) captures attention. The own face is identified faster among other faces and better recalled [9,11]. Additionally, self-faces are explored differently than unfamiliar faces during free viewing (e.g., more fixations are directed on the eyes of other faces than the self-face [8]). Finally, self-faces can distract attention from a task. In a study by Devue et al. [9], participants searched for a target face (e.g., with an open mouth) among distractor faces (e.g., with a closed mouth). Self-face distractors were fixated longer than other-face distractors.
The inspection of the own face (in pictures) is influenced by different personality traits, such as self-disgust and self-esteem. Self-disgust involves a lasting appraisal of aspects of the self as disgusting [12]. Ypsilanti, Gettings et al. [5] presented participants with picture pairs consisting of their face and an unknown face. Those participants with high self-disgust showed attentional avoidance of their faces (reduced gaze duration). In the second study of this research group, Ypsilanti, Robson et al. [6] focused on maintaining visual attention vs. gaze avoidance of the own face. Again, high self-disgust was associated with avoidance of the own face. The participants shifted their gaze over the trial duration away from their face toward the unknown face. These findings suggest that dissatisfaction with one’s visual appearance is associated with a reduced viewing time of one’s face.
Additionally, the personality trait self-esteem (confidence and satisfaction in oneself) is related to self-viewing behavior. In a study by Hu et al. [13], participants high vs. low in self-esteem showed a greater pupil size while looking at their face compared to an unfamiliar face. Participants with high self-esteem also identified their faces faster than unknown faces [13].
Inflated self-esteem, narcissism, might also play a role in the way people view their faces. Narcissism is characterized by an overestimation of one’s capabilities, combined with a high need for admiration by others, perception of uniqueness, lack of empathy, and a tendency to take advantage of others [14]. People with high narcissism propensity (NP) enjoy watching themselves more than people with low NP. In a study by Robins and John [15], participants were asked whether they would prefer watching themselves or others in a videotaped group exercise. Participants with high scores on the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; [16]) chose to view themselves more often instead of choosing to view someone else. Furthermore, participants high in NP reported more frequent mirror viewing than participants with low NP. In a brain imaging study, self-face viewing was associated with increased activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in participants with high NP [17]. The authors interpreted the ACC activation as an indicator of negative affect. The elicitation of negative emotions reflects that looking at the own face can challenge ego inflation [18]. The results of this investigation suggest that vulnerable aspects of narcissism may not be observable in self-report studies. Therefore, it is necessary to reevaluate the assumption that narcissists look at themselves in the mirror more extensively than individuals with low-level narcissism [15].
It has to be noted that viewing oneself in a picture vs. a mirror differs in many ways. First, mirror viewing is dynamic and can be adjusted based on visual feedback. Second, dynamic faces are perceived as more attractive than static faces [19]. Third, when we look at a mirror, the left side of our face is reflected on the right side. Since we are used to this mirrored self-depiction, we tend to perceive our faces more favorably in a mirror than in a photo [20,21].
The present study aimed at investigating associations between personality traits (self-disgust, self-esteem, narcissism propensity), and gaze parameters (gaze duration, fixation counts) during self-inspection of one’s face in the mirror. As the control stimulus, we used a video of the face of an unknown person (matched for age and gender) to compare the gaze parameters between self-face and other-face free viewing (for 90 s each).
We predicted that participants high in self-disgust would show conscious gaze avoidance of their faces (a shorter gaze duration) and a lower number of fixations as an indicator of less detail-exploring gaze behavior (i.e., fewer fixations = fewer details or locations are viewed) [5,6,22]. In contrast, high self-esteem and narcissism propensity should be associated with the opposite pattern, prolonged gaze [13,15], and an increased number of fixations (a more detailed viewing style).
An exploratory research question addressed possible gender differences in self-face viewing. To the best of our knowledge, there is no published eye-tracking study on this topic. However, there is some evidence that females tend to be more critical of their physical appearance than males [23,24]. This gender difference might contribute to differences in self-face processing. Because of the lack of eye-tracking investigations in this field, the present study conducted an exploratory comparison between female and male participants’ self-face and other-face viewing behavior.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

This study was conducted with 75 participants (aged between 18 and 35 years), who reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision. We excluded seven participants because the eye-tracker recorded eye movements less than 50% of the time or calibration was impossible due to reflections on participants’ glasses. The final sample (n = 68) had a mean age of 23.5 years (SD = 3.8). Males (n = 29) and females (n = 39) did not differ in mean age (p = 0.14). Sixty-one participants (90%) were university students. The other participants were white-collar workers. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University (ethical approval code: 39/104/63 ex 2019/20) and conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent.

2.2. Procedure

The participants were invited to an online survey that assessed demographic data (age, gender, occupation, educational status), exclusion criteria (reported diagnoses of somatic illness, mental disorders, psychotropic medication), and personality traits (self-disgust, self-esteem, narcissism propensity).
In the eye-tracking experiment, the participants freely viewed themselves in a mirror (attached to the screen of the computer; Figure 1) and watched a video of an unknown person displayed at the computer screen (same gender, similar age, neutral facial expression, no head movements, no speaking) for 90 s each (mirror/video: 39 × 29 cm; viewing distance: 60 cm) in a lit room (illuminance ~250 lux). The lighting enabled the participants to see their faces clearly in the mirror. The vertical and horizontal extent of the self-face and other-face were marked as perceived by two experimenters (see Figure 1). While one experimenter looked at his face in the mirror, another experimenter moved a ruler upward from the edge of the mirror until the ruler reached the reflection of the chin (above the chin rest). This position was defined as the lower end of the face and marked on the edge of the monitor with removable markers. The same procedure was repeated for the upper, right, and left borders of the face. The size and location of the face in the video were matched (resized and cropped) to the mirror condition. Areas of interest (AOIs) were determined in SMI BeGaze based on these outlines for self-face and other-face (same size of self-face and other-face AOI; Figure 1). The chinrest was positioned at the same height for all participants to position the self-face within the defined AOI. The trial order was randomized. Before each trial (when attaching/removing the mirror), the participants closed their eyes. At the beginning of each trial, the participants were asked to open their eyes. Keeping the eyes closed (instead of looking at a fixation cross before the trial started) ensured that both stimuli were visible for the same duration. The whole eye-tracking experiment took ap-proximately 10 min (informed consent <5 min; eye-tracker calibration ~2 min; 2 trials: 90 s per trial, intertrial interval ~30 s).
The paradigm was preregistered; data are available online: https://osf.io/fmv5s/ (accessed on 10 August 2021).

2.3. Eye Movement Recording and Analysis

We recorded eye movements with an SMI RED250mobile eye-tracker. Eye movements were defined by the standard SMI thresholds (saccade velocity-threshold: 40°/s; minimum fixation duration 50 ms). We analyzed gaze duration (i.e., total fixation duration) and fixation count within both face AOIs (self-face, other-face).

2.4. Questionnaires

The Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) has been developed by Raskin and Hall [25] and is one of the most widely used personality measures for non-clinical levels of the trait narcissism. A German (NPI-D-17) version with 17 items has been validated by von Collani [26]. Typical items are “I think I am a special person”, “I will never be satisfied until I get all that I deserve.” or “I will usually show off if I get the chance.” which are rated on 5-point scales (1 = “does not apply”, 5 = “completely applies”). Cronbach’s alpha in the present sample was 0.90.
The Questionnaire for the Assessment of Self-Disgust (QASD; [12]) measures personal (devaluation of one’s physical appearance; 9 items; e.g., “I find myself repulsive”; 5-point scales: 0 = “not true at all”, 4 = “absolutely true”) and behavioral self-disgust (devaluation of one’s behavior; 5 items; e.g., “Some of my behaviors are repulsive to others”). The two self-disgust scores displayed high intercorrelation (r = 0.78) in the present sample, thus we used a global measure of self-disgust (encompassing all QASD items; α = 0.91). The same approach was used in previous research [27].
The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale was developed by Rosenberg in 1965 [28] and is a measure of global self-esteem (e.g., “On the whole, I am satisfied with myself”). In the present study, self-esteem was assessed via the German version of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, which was validated by Ferring and Filipp (RSES; [29]; α = 0.84; 10 items: e.g., “I am able to do things as well as most other people”; 4-point scales: 0 = “does not apply at all”; 3 = “completely applies”).

2.5. Statistical Analysis

First, Pearson correlations were calculated to investigate associations between the personality traits (self-esteem, narcissism propensity, self-disgust,) and gaze behavior (gaze duration, fixation count). Then, multiple regression analyses (simultaneous en-try/enter method) estimated the relationship between the traits and gaze behavior (view-ing of self/others). A mixed ANOVA tested the effects of Gender (male, female) and Condition (self-face, other-face) on total gaze duration and fixation count. If Mauchly’s test suggested a violation of sphericity, the Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons were Bonferroni–Holm corrected. We computed t-tests to compare questionnaire scores between males and females.

3. Results

3.1. Correlation Analyses

Pearson correlations between personality traits and gaze duration (self-face and other-face) are depicted in Table 1. Reported self-esteem correlated negatively with gaze duration (self-face and other-face) and positively with fixation count (other-face).

3.2. Regression Analyses

The regression for gaze duration on self-face was significant (F(3,64) = 4.45; p = 0.007; R2 = 0.173). Self-esteem was a significant predictor of gaze duration (Table 2). Higher self-esteem was associated with a shorter gaze duration. Also, for the gaze duration on the other-face the regression was significant (F(3,63) = 3.42; p = 0.022; R2 = 0.140) with higher self-esteem predicting a shorter gaze duration.
For the fixation count, the regression model was not significant for self-face (F(3,64) = 1.41; p = 0.247; R2 = 0.062) and other-face (F (3,63) = 1.91; p = 0.137, R2 = 0.083).

3.3. Exploratory Analyses (Gender Effects)

Males and females did not differ in narcissism propensity, self-disgust, and self-esteem (Table 3).
For gaze duration, the ANOVA revealed significant main effects for Condition (F(1,65) = 8.52, p = 0.005, partη2 = 0.116) and Gender (F(1,65) = 13.49, p < 0.001, partη2 = 0.172). The interaction Condition × Gender was not significant (F(1,65) = 0.15, p = 0.70, partη2 = 0.002). The participants viewed their own face shorter than the unknown face (d = 0.372). On average, males (M = 81.33 s, SD = 4.39) looked longer at the faces (self and other) compared to females (M = 76.50 s, SD = 5.88, t(65) = 3.67, p < 0.001, d = 0.91).
For fixation count, the interaction Condition × Gender was significant (F(1, 65) = 5.50, p = 0.022, partη2 = 0.078). However, the post-hoc comparisons were not significant (p > 0.060).

4. Discussion

This study investigated whether gaze parameters while viewing one’s face in the mirror are associated with the personality traits of self-esteem, self-disgust, and narcissism propensity. High self-esteem indicates that, on the whole, individuals are satisfied with themselves and are not overly critical [30,31]. In previous eye-tracking research, elevated self-esteem was associated with attentional biases towards self-faces [13]. Surprisingly, in the present investigation, higher self-esteem was associated with shorter—possibly less critical—viewing of the own face. It is possible that people with high self-esteem need less time to evaluate themselves (critically), while low self-esteem seems to be associated with a more thorough and more prolonged evaluation of one’s facial appearance.
Following this interpretation, the shorter viewing time of the other face associated with higher self-esteem would imply that individuals with high self-esteem are also less critical of others [32]. It is possible that in previous research [13] this viewing pattern could not be identified due to brief exposure times. The participants looked at the faces until they identified them. This took them less than one second on average. Additionally, in previous research, faces were not viewed freely, and task demands (face identification) may have overruled a critical and thorough evaluation of the faces [13]. Future research needs to investigate the effect of exposure time and viewing tasks on gaze behavior concerning self-face and other-face.
Self-esteem did neither predict the number of fixations on self-face nor other-face. Many short fixations (i.e., hyperscanning) are commonly conducted when affective stimuli with negative valence and high intensity are explored [22,33]. In the present study, dynamic faces with neutral expressions were viewed, which do not induce high arousal, at least in non-clinical samples [19,20,21]. While self-esteem predicted a more thorough evaluation of faces (longer viewing time), it was not associated with visual hyperscanning, which would have required elevated arousal. Taken together, the results suggest that low self-esteem is related to a thorough but calm evaluation of faces.
Earlier investigations indicated that the number of fixations on specific face areas differs between self-face and other-face viewing. Hoffmann et al. [8] observed that more fixations were conducted on the eyes when viewing unfamiliar faces than the own face. We refrained from using a similar analysis approach (an analysis of fixation counts for specific facial areas) because of insufficient spatial precision of the measuring procedure. There are slight interindividual differences in the position of facial features (e.g., location of eyes, mouth), which cannot be detected with sufficient spatial accuracy during mirror viewing. It would have been possible to control for the location of the eyes during mirror viewing by adjusting the chinrest for each participant until they see their eyes at a predefined mirror location. This procedure would, however, lead to self-face exposure before the eye-tracking experiment begins. Therefore, we focused on the gaze behavior for the face as a whole.
There was no relationship between NPI scores and gaze behavior. Narcissism propensity was neither associated with a more thorough (prolonged) viewing nor hyperscanning. These null-findings contrast our expectations and previous research, especially for self-face viewing [15]. In the present paradigm, participants viewed self-faces and other faces one after another and not simultaneously (e.g., as image pairs). It is possible that associations between narcissism propensity and viewing behavior did not show up because participants could not directly compare their faces with the faces of others. Furthermore, participants did not perform any task in which they could have compared their performance with the performance of others. Future studies should investigate whether narcissism propensity is associated with specific self-face viewing behaviors in competitive contexts (i.e., when comparing one’s own and others’ physical appearance).
The NPI assesses feelings of self-importance. However, narcissism has different facets. It has been suggested to distinguish between grandiose and vulnerable narcissism [34,35]. Vulnerable narcissistic individuals are anxious, defensive, and avoidant, while grandiose narcists are extraverted and self-satisfied [36]. Additionally, there are intrapersonal and interpersonal aspects of narcissism [37]. To maintain a grandiose self, people with a high narcissism propensity might, on the one hand, strive for feelings of uniqueness (intrapersonal) [38]. On the other hand, they can also devaluate others or strive for supremacy (interpersonal) [39]. Therefore, more differentiated NP measures should be used in the future, which can differentiate between these separable yet related expressions of narcissism that should prompt different styles of self-viewing [40].
Self-disgust also showed no association with viewing the own face. Self-disgust has been conceptualized as a dysfunctional personality trait [12,41] which typically has a very low level in mentally healthy individuals [41]. This was also the case in the present investigation. In contrast, Ypsilanti, Robson et al. [6] analyzed eye-tracking data from participants with mental health problems. This group with elevated self-disgust displayed avoidance of self-viewing. Therefore, the present null results do not contrast previous findings [5,6]. Future research should further investigate self-face viewing in conditions with elevated self-disgust, such as depression [2,42], disordered eating [43,44,45], and body dissatisfaction [45,46,47]. For example, body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) is associated with high self-disgust [47] but not with avoidance of self-viewing. Many patients with BDD even report spending a significant amount of time checking themselves in the mirror [45]. Mirror viewing paradigms can therefore contribute to a deeper understanding of self-viewing avoidance as well as excessive mirror viewing in eating disorders and BDD. For these disorders, mirror exposure is a commonly used therapy component [48].
We also investigated possible gender effects on face-viewing behavior. In general, males, as well as females, spent the majority of the 90 s-viewing time on the faces. Thus, there was no indication of face avoidance. Male participants were characterized by a longer total viewing time. They showed a longer gaze duration for both the own face and the other face. Previous studies have already demonstrated that face exploration dynamics (for other faces) differentiate men and women [49,50]. In an investigation by Coutrot et al. [49], the participants (203 males and 202 females) watched videos of actors. Male participants showed longer fixation durations for the faces than female participants. However, this difference was small. Overall, face viewing has many functions and serves multiple purposes (e.g., aiding speech perception [51], emotion recognition [52], person identification [53], or affective evaluation [15]). Moreover, context factors (e.g., cultural, social) have to be considered as well [54]. It needs further investigation whether differences in face-viewing intentions might have contributed to the observed gender difference in gaze duration.
It is plausible that several limitations might have influenced the results obtained. We studied a convenience sample, and the participants were healthy, predominantly young students. Therefore, the present results cannot be generalized to other samples. Furthermore, the study did not differentiate between different aspects of narcissism propensity.

5. Conclusions

This study is the first eye-tracking investigation on self-face viewing in a mirror. Gaze duration was associated with self-esteem. People with lower self-esteem viewed faces longer and possibly more critical. Furthermore, males looked longer at the faces than females. These insights contribute to a deeper understanding of mirror viewing behavior which is needed to improve and evaluate mirror exposure therapy in the future.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.P. and A.S.; methodology, J.P.; formal analysis, J.P.; investigation, J.P.; resources, A.S.; data curation, J.P. and A.S.; writing—original draft preparation, A.S.; writing—review and editing, J.P. and A.S.; visualization, J.P.; supervision, A.S.; project administration, J.P. and A.S.; funding acquisition, A.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee of the University of Graz (39/104/63 ex 2019/20; 1 October 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are openly available in the Open Science Framework (OSF): https://osf.io/fmv5s/ (accessed on 10 August 2021).

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the financial support by the University of Graz.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Windheim, K.; Veale, D.; Anson, M. Mirror gazing in body dysmorphic disorder and healthy controls: Effects of duration of gazing. Behav. Res. Ther. 2011, 49, 555–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Powell, P.A.; Overton, P.G.; Simpson, J. The revolting self: An interpretative phenomenological analysis of the experience of self-disgust in females with depressive symptoms. J. Clin. Psychol. 2014, 70, 562–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Barnier, E.M.; Collison, J. Experimental induction of self-focused attention via mirror gazing: Effects on body image, appraisals, body-focused shame, and self-esteem. Body Image 2019, 30, 150–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Mulkens, S.; Jansen, A. Mirror gazing increases attractiveness in satisfied, but not in dissatisfied women: A model for body dysmorphic disorder? J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2009, 40, 211–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Ypsilanti, A.; Gettings, R.; Lazuras, L.; Robson, A.; Powell, P.A.; Overton, P.G. Self-Disgust Is Associated With Loneliness, Mental Health Difficulties, and Eye-Gaze Avoidance in War Veterans With PTSD. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 2948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Ypsilanti, A.; Robson, A.; Lazuras, L.; Powell, P.A.; Overton, P.G. Self-disgust, loneliness and mental health outcomes in older adults: An eye-tracking study. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 266, 646–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Chakraborty, A.; Chakrabarti, B. Looking at My Own Face: Visual Processing Strategies in Self-Other Face Recognition. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Hoffmann, A.; Maran, T.; Sachse, P. How We Perceive Others Resembling Us. Iperception 2020, 11, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Devue, C.; van der Stigchel, S.; Brédart, S.; Theeuwes, J. You do not find your own face faster; you just look at it longer. Cognition 2009, 111, 114–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Li, Y.H.; Tottenham, N. Seeing yourself helps you see others. Emotion 2011, 11, 1235–1241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Tong, F.; Nakayama, K. Robust representations for faces: Evidence from visual search. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1999, 25, 1016–1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Schienle, A.; Übel, S.; Schöngaßner, F.; Ille, R.; Scharmüller, W. Disgust regulation via placebo: An fMRI study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014, 9, 985–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Hu, Y.; Liao, S.; Luo, W.; He, W. Effects of Self-Esteem on Self-Face Recognition: An Eye Movement Study. Open J. Soc. Sci. 2013, 1, 40–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Brown, R.P.; Budzek, K.; Tamborski, M. On the meaning and measure of narcissism. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 35, 951–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Robins, R.W.; John, O.P. Effects of Visual Perspective and Narcissism on Self-Perception: Is Seeing Believing? Psychol. Sci. 1997, 8, 37–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Raskin, R.; Terry, H. A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 890–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Jauk, E.; Benedek, M.; Koschutnig, K.; Kedia, G.; Neubauer, A.C. Self-viewing is associated with negative affect rather than reward in highly narcissistic men: An fMRI study. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 5804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Pincus, A.L.; Lukowitsky, M.R. Pathological narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2010, 6, 421–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Hughes, S.M.; Aung, T. Symmetry in Motion: Perception of Attractiveness Changes with Facial Movement. J. Nonverbal Behav. 2018, 42, 267–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Frautschi, R.S.; Orfahli, L.M.; Zins, J.E. Reflecting on Your Reflection: Examining the Effect of a Non-Reversing Mirror on Self Perception. Aesthetic Surg. J. 2021, 41, NP1989–NP1993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Mita, T.H.; Dermer, M.; Knight, J. Reversed facial images and the mere-exposure hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1977, 35, 597–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Armstrong, T.; Olatunji, B.O. Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: A meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2012, 32, 704–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Hayes, S.D.; Crocker, P.R.E.; Kowalski, K.C. Gender Differences in Physical Self-Perceptions, Global Self-Esteem and Physical Activity: Evaluation of the Physical Self-Perception Profile Model. J. Sport Behav. 1999, 22, 1–14. [Google Scholar]
  24. Mäkinen, M.; Puukko-Viertomies, L.-R.; Lindberg, N.; Siimes, M.A.; Aalberg, V. Body dissatisfaction and body mass in girls and boys transitioning from early to mid-adolescence: Additional role of self-esteem and eating habits. BMC Psychiatry 2012, 12, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Raskin, R.; Hall, C.S. The Narcissistic Personality Inventory: Alternative form reliability and further evidence of construct validity. J. Pers. Assess. 1981, 45, 159–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. von Collani, G. Deutsche Versionen Des Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI-d). Zusammenstellung sozialwissenschaftlicher Items und Skalen (ZIS) 2008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Schienle, A.; Schwab, D.; Höfler, C.; Freudenthaler, H.H. Self-Disgust and Its Relationship With Lifetime Suicidal Ideation and Behavior. Crisis 2020, 41, 344–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Rosenberg, M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1965. [Google Scholar]
  29. Ferring, D.; Filipp, S.-H. Messung des Selbstwertgefühls: Befunde zu Reliabilität, Validität und Stabilität der Rosenberg-Skala. [Measuring self-esteem: Reliability, validity, and stability of the Rosenberg scale.]. Diagnostica 1996, 42, 284–292. [Google Scholar]
  30. von Collani, G.; Herzberg, P.Y. Eine revidierte Fassung der deutschsprachigen Skala zum Selbstwertgefühl von Rosenberg. Z. Für Differ. Und Diagn. Psychol. 2003, 24, 3–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Zeigler-Hill, V. Self-Esteem; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2013; ISBN 9781135068745. [Google Scholar]
  32. Garofalo, C.; Holden, C.J.; Zeigler-Hill, V.; Velotti, P. Understanding the connection between self-esteem and aggression: The mediating role of emotion dysregulation. Aggress. Behav. 2016, 42, 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Schienle, A.; Potthoff, J.; Schönthaler, E.; Schlintl, C. Disgust-Related Memory Bias in Children and Adults. Evol. Psychol. 2021, 19, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Koepernik, T.; Jauk, E.; Kanske, P. Lay theories of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 31, 346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Jauk, E.; Weigle, E.; Lehmann, K.; Benedek, M.; Neubauer, A.C. The Relationship between Grandiose and Vulnerable (Hypersensitive) Narcissism. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Jauk, E.; Kanske, P. Can neuroscience help to understand narcissism? A systematic review of an emerging field. Personal. Neurosci. 2021, 4, e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Back, M.D.; Küfner, A.C.P.; Dufner, M.; Gerlach, T.M.; Rauthmann, J.F.; Denissen, J.J.A. Narcissistic admiration and rivalry: Disentangling the bright and dark sides of narcissism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 105, 1013–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Zajenkowski, M.; Gignac, G.E. Telling people they are intelligent correlates with the feeling of narcissistic uniqueness: The influence of IQ feedback on temporary state narcissism. Intelligence 2021, 89, 101595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Jordan, D.G.; Winer, E.S.; Zeigler-Hill, V.; Marcus, D.K. A Network Approach to Understanding Narcissistic Grandiosity via the Narcissistic Admiration and Rivalry Questionnaire and the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Self Identity 2021, 21, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Leckelt, M.; Wetzel, E.; Gerlach, T.M.; Ackerman, R.A.; Miller, J.D.; Chopik, W.J.; Penke, L.; Geukes, K.; Küfner, A.C.P.; Hutteman, R.; et al. Validation of the Narcissistic Admiration and Rivalry Questionnaire Short Scale (NARQ-S) in convenience and representative samples. Psychol. Assess. 2018, 30, 86–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Ille, R.; Schöggl, H.; Kapfhammer, H.-P.; Arendasy, M.; Sommer, M.; Schienle, A. Self-disgust in mental disorders—symptom-related or disorder-specific? Compr. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 938–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ypsilanti, A.; Lazuras, L.; Powell, P.; Overton, P. Self-disgust as a potential mechanism explaining the association between loneliness and depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 243, 108–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Moncrieff-Boyd, J.; Allen, K.; Byrne, S.; Nunn, K. The Self-Disgust Scale Revised Version: Validation and relationships with eating disorder symptomatology. J. Eat. Disord. 2014, 2, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Bell, K.; Coulthard, H.; Wildbur, D. Self-Disgust within Eating Disordered Groups: Associations with Anxiety, Disgust Sensitivity and Sensory Processing. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2017, 25, 373–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Veale, D.; Riley, S. Mirror, mirror on the wall, who is the ugliest of them all? The psychopathology of mirror gazing in body dysmorphic disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 2001, 39, 1381–1393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Spreckelsen, P.V.; Glashouwer, K.A.; Bennik, E.C.; Wessel, I.; de Jong, P.J. Negative body image: Relationships with heightened disgust propensity, disgust sensitivity, and self-directed disgust. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0198532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Stasik-O’Brien, S.M.; Schmidt, J. The role of disgust in body image disturbance: Incremental predictive power of self-disgust. Body Image 2018, 27, 128–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Griffen, T.C.; Naumann, E.; Hildebrandt, T. Mirror exposure therapy for body image disturbances and eating disorders: A review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 65, 163–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Coutrot, A.; Binetti, N.; Harrison, C.; Mareschal, I.; Johnston, A. Face exploration dynamics differentiate men and women. J. Vis. 2016, 16, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Shen, J.; Itti, L. Top-down influences on visual attention during listening are modulated by observer sex. Vision Res. 2012, 65, 62–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Yi, A.; Wong, W.; Eizenman, M. Gaze Patterns and Audiovisual Speech Enhancement. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2013, 56, 471–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Tsang, V. Eye-tracking study on facial emotion recognition tasks in individuals with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism 2018, 22, 161–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Peterson, M.F.; Eckstein, M.P. Individual differences in eye movements during face identification reflect observer-specific optimal points of fixation. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 1216–1225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Hessels, R.S. How does gaze to faces support face-to-face interaction? A review and perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2020, 27, 856–881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Hardware setup with mirror attached to the computer screen. Blue lines indicate Area of Interest (AOI) determination for self-face and other-face.
Figure 1. Hardware setup with mirror attached to the computer screen. Blue lines indicate Area of Interest (AOI) determination for self-face and other-face.
Behavsci 11 00164 g001
Table 1. Pearson correlations between gaze duration and personality traits.
Table 1. Pearson correlations between gaze duration and personality traits.
Gaze DurationFixation Count
Rprp
Self-face
NPI−0.0800.5170.1500.222
QASD0.1140.3560.0420.734
RSES−0.365 *0.0020.1350.272
Other-face
NPI−0.1710.166−0.0430.729
QASD0.0920.458−0.0940.449
RSES−0.307 *00.0120.251 *0.040
NPI: Narcissistic Personality Inventory; QASD: Questionnaire for the Assessment of Self-Disgust; RSES: Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale; * p < 0.05.
Table 2. Regression analyses for gaze duration.
Table 2. Regression analyses for gaze duration.
B (CI)SE Bβp
Self-face
NPI−0.30 (−2.62, 2.02)1.16−0.0330.796
QASD−2.87 (−6.87, 1.14)2.01−0.2540.157
RSES−7.06 (−11.32, −2.80)2.13−0.5450.002
Other-face
NPI−1.23 (−3.42, 0.96)1.10−0.1480.266
QASD−1.40 (−5.05, 2.25)1.83−0.1410.445
RSES−4.75 (−8.63, −0.87)1.94−0.4160.017
NPI: Narcissistic Personality Inventory; QASD: Questionnaire for the Assessment of Self-Disgust; RSES: Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale; B: unstandardized beta; CI: 95% confidence interval; SE: standard error; β: standardized beta.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and gender comparisons.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and gender comparisons.
All
M (SD)
Males
M (SD)
Females
M (SD)
T66 (p)d
RSES2.12 (0.51)2.04 (0.54)2.18 (0.48)1.11 (0.812)0.27
NPI2.48 (0.72)2.69 (0.73)2.32 (0.69)2.09 (0.204)0.51
QASD0.66 (0.58)0.74 (0.63)0.60 (0.54)0.94 (0.699)0.23
Self-face
Gaze duration77.75 (6.58)80.70 (5.27)75.56 (6.66)3.43 (0.006) *0.84
Fixation counts127.49 (36.65)134.45 (41.46)122.31 (32.21)1.36 (0.715)0.33
Other-face
Gaze duration79.37 (5.83)82.05 (4.77)77.44 (5.80)3.45 (0.007) *0.86
Fixation counts129.75 (44.36)124.71 (44.87)133.36 (44.21)0.79 (0.436)0.19
NPI: Narcissistic Personality Inventory; QASD: Questionnaire for the Assessment of Self-Disgust; RSES: Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale; gaze duration in seconds; d: Cohen’s d; Asterisks: Bonferroni–Holm corrected p < 0.05.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Potthoff, J.; Schienle, A. Effects of Self-Esteem on Self-Viewing: An Eye-Tracking Investigation on Mirror Gazing. Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 164. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11120164

AMA Style

Potthoff J, Schienle A. Effects of Self-Esteem on Self-Viewing: An Eye-Tracking Investigation on Mirror Gazing. Behavioral Sciences. 2021; 11(12):164. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11120164

Chicago/Turabian Style

Potthoff, Jonas, and Anne Schienle. 2021. "Effects of Self-Esteem on Self-Viewing: An Eye-Tracking Investigation on Mirror Gazing" Behavioral Sciences 11, no. 12: 164. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11120164

APA Style

Potthoff, J., & Schienle, A. (2021). Effects of Self-Esteem on Self-Viewing: An Eye-Tracking Investigation on Mirror Gazing. Behavioral Sciences, 11(12), 164. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11120164

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop