Next Article in Journal
Defense-Oriented Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy as a Tailored Treatment for Boys: Neurobiological Underpinnings to Male-Specific Response Tested in Regulation-Focused Psychotherapy for Children
Previous Article in Journal
Personality, Intervention and Psychological Treatment: Untangling and Explaining New Horizons and Perspectives
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale: An Item Response Theory Analysis

1
Workera.ai, Palo Alto, CA 94306, USA
2
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC 26170, USA
3
Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61801, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2022, 12(8), 247; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080247
Submission received: 15 June 2022 / Revised: 19 July 2022 / Accepted: 21 July 2022 / Published: 22 July 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Psychiatric, Emotional and Behavioral Disorders)

Abstract

:
Ambivalence has a prominent role in the historical formulations of schizotypy and schizophrenia, as well as borderline personality disorder. However, it has been overlooked by our current diagnostic nomenclature. The Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale (SAS) is a 19-item self-report scale developed to examine ambivalence relevant to schizotypy and schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Questionnaire, interview, and ambulatory assessment studies support the construct validity of the measure as a predictor of schizophrenia-spectrum and borderline psychopathology. However, studies have not adequately examined the item properties and factor structure of the scale. To examine the psychometric features of the SAS, the present research applied item response theory and differential item functioning methods using a large sample of adults (n = 7096). Analyses of dimensionality were consistent with essential unidimensionality, and a 2PL IRT model found good item discrimination, an appropriate range of item difficulty, minimal local dependence, and excellent item fit. Analyses of differential item functioning found essentially no bias for gender on any items and very small effects for two items for racial/ethnic identity. Overall, the analyses reveal many psychometric strengths of the Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale and support its use a single-factor instrument for assessing ambivalence in diverse subgroups of adults.

1. Introduction

Schizotypy represents the phenotypic expression of the underlying vulnerability for schizophrenia-spectrum disorders that is expressed across a broad range of clinical and subclinical impairment [1]. Schizotypy offers a useful construct for understanding the risk for and development of schizophrenia-spectrum psychopathology because it encompasses subclinical manifestations, the psychosis prodrome, schizophrenia-spectrum personality disorders, and full-blown psychosis. Schizotypy is a multidimensional construct with positive (psychotic-like), negative (deficit), and disorganized dimensions. The construct involves disruptions in cognition (e.g., magical beliefs and cognitive slippage), perception (e.g., illusions and bodily aberrations), the experience and expression of emotion (e.g., flattened or dysregulated affect), and social functioning (e.g., suspiciousness, social disinterest, and social aversiveness).
Scholars have also suggested that ambivalence characterizes subclinical and clinical manifestations of schizotypy. The term ambivalence was originally coined by Bleuler [2] to represent the simultaneous experience of both positive and negative emotions and the inability to integrate these emotions. He viewed ambivalence as a manifestation of thought disorder and also considered it to be one of the core components of schizophrenia. Meehl [3] initially proposed that ambivalence was one of four core symptoms of schizotypy; however, he subsequently revised the role of ambivalence as a potentiating factor in schizotypy that increased the likelihood of developing schizophrenia [4]. Despite its prominence in Bleuler’s historical formulation of schizophrenia and Meehl’s model of schizotypy, the concept of ambivalence has subsequently received relatively little attention in the research and clinical literature regarding schizophrenia-spectrum psychopathology [5], and it is not part of the diagnostic formulation of any of the DSM-5-TR [6] schizophrenia spectrum or other psychotic disorders. The lack of inclusion likely resulted from a failure to adequately operationalize the construct, as well as its inclusion in psychoanalytic formulations of borderline personality (e.g., [7]).
The Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale (SAS; [8]) was developed to assess ambivalence relevant to schizotypy and the schizophrenia spectrum. Specifically, the 19 true–false items (see Table 1) emphasize the simultaneous experience of contradictory emotions or the rapid and almost random change of emotions over time. Sample items include, “My thoughts and feelings always seem to be contradictory”, and “Often I feel like I hate even my favorite activities”. The scale has good internal consistency (coefficient alpha = 0.84 in 1798 young adults) and test–retest (0.74 across nine weeks in 166 young adults) reliabilities [9].
Questionnaire, interview, and ambulatory assessment studies support the construct validity of the SAS. Multiple studies indicate that the SAS is associated with other measures of schizotypy (e.g., [9,10]). Kwapil et al. [11] reported that SAS scores correlated 0.49 with the positive schizotypy factor, 0.38 with the negative schizotypy factor, and 0.66 with the disorganized schizotypy factor of the Multidimensional Schizotypy Scale [12]. Burgin et al. [13] used experience sampling methodology to demonstrate that schizotypal ambivalence was associated with diminished positive affect, increased negative affect, as well as cognitive and social impairment in daily life. Using a French translation of the SAS [14], Loas et al. [15] found that first-degree relatives of people with schizophrenia had higher schizotypal ambivalence than first-degree relatives of people with non-psychotic psychiatric disorders. Finally, five interview studies with non-clinically ascertained young adults have provided converging evidence that the SAS is robustly associated with impaired functioning, schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms and personality disorder traits, and borderline personality disorder traits [9,11,16].
The empirical literature has supported the validity of the SAS. However, the scale was developed prior to the widespread availability of modern measurement tools such as item response theory (IRT) and differential item functioning (DIF), and the scale and its items have not undergone rigorous psychometric analysis. Furthermore, only one study, to our knowledge, has investigated the internal structure of the SAS. MacAulay et al. [10] examined the dimensional structure of the SAS in 334 participants. Using principal components analysis with an oblique rotation, they reported three underlying factors: interpersonal ambivalence, indecision/insecurity, and contradictory feelings. However, they used a Likert response format from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), and it is unclear if a multidimensional structure would be found using the original dichotomous scoring format.

Goals and Hypotheses of the Present Study

Given the historical importance of ambivalence and the empirical promise demonstrated by the SAS, the present study provided a comprehensive assessment of the scale’s psychometric properties in a large sample of non-clinically ascertained young adults. The study examined the dimensionality of the SAS using the traditional dichotomous response option. Next, it employed classical test theory (CTT) and IRT metrics to examine key item properties (e.g., item difficulty, discrimination, and fit) and test properties (e.g., test information). Finally, we examined the possibility of item bias in the SAS with analyses of differential item functioning based on self-reported gender and racial-ethnic categories. We concluded with an overall evaluation of the scale’s psychometric features.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

The sample consisted of 7096 adults enrolled at the University of North Carolina at Greensboro (UNCG) who completed the scale as part of larger research projects or mass screening sessions. All participants provided informed consent, and the research was approved by the UNCG Institutional Review Board. The sample was predominantly female (5466 women, 1622 men) and young (M = 19.60 years, SD = 3.51, Mdn = 18.8, range = 18 to 64; age was available for approximately half of the sample). For self-reported racial and ethnic identification, the sample largely consisted of European American (n = 4861), African American (n = 1521), Asian (n = 199) Hispanic/Latinx (n = 148), Native American (n = 39), and people who endorsed other or no categories (n = 152).

2.2. Analytic Approach

The data were analyzed in R 4.2 [17] using the packages psych 2.2.3 [18], TAM 4.0.1 [19], and lordif 0.3.3 [20]. The IRT models were conducted using marginal maximum likelihood and were case centered for identification, yielding a trait theta score centered on zero. The raw data and R code are available at Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/ztycp/, accessed on 22 July 2021) for researchers who would like to duplicate the analyses and explore the data further.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics, Reliability, and Dimensionality

Table 1 displays the descriptive statistics for each item, including the means (percent of participants who endorsed the binary item), standard deviations, and item-scale correlations, which ranged from 0.46 to 0.57. The scale exhibited good internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.84). Analysis of coefficient Omega (estimated with tetrachoric correlations) found that Omega-total was high (ωT = 0.93); omega-hierarchical, which reflects the item saturation of the general factor, was lower but nevertheless good (ωH = 0.71).
For dimensionality, we examined essential unidimensionality—a looser standard than strict unidimensionality that is commonly applied to psychological constructs [21]—using multiple criteria [22]. One of the more accepted methods is the ratio of the first and second eigenvalues, specifically a ratio of 4:1 [23] or 3:1 [21]. We also evaluated the minimum average partial (MAP) criterion and the scree plot from a parallel analysis [24]. The factor analyses were conducted in psych using maximum-likelihood factor analysis and tetrachoric correlations because of the dichotomous response scale.
The parallel analysis suggested seven factors based on the resampled values, but there was clearly one dominant factor and, at most, one minor secondary factor (see Figure 1). The MAP criterion suggested two factors, and the eigenvalue-ratio criterion suggested essential unidimensionality (a ratio of 8.4:1). To explore the small, secondary factor in more detail, we conducted an exploratory factor analysis with a bifactor rotation, which extracts a common, general factor and then specific orthogonal factors. All items had standardized loadings of at least 0.52–0.68 on the general factor, and no item had a larger loading on a specific factor than on the general factor. The specific factors appeared to reflect local dependence—pairs or small subsets of items that covaried with each other after accounting for the general factor—rather than substantive facets, a view that is supported by analyses of local independence in the next section. Taken together, the dominant first factor, high loadings of all items on the common factor, ratio of eigenvalues, and lack of substantive meaning of the specific minor factors suggest (1) good evidence for essential but not strict unidimensionality, and (2) that unidimensionality could probably be improved by trimming overlapping, partially redundant items that demonstrate local dependence.

3.2. IRT Model Fit and Local Independence

We estimated the IRT models in TAM using marginal maximum likelihood and case centering, which centers the trait theta score at 0. Two models were compared: a Rasch model that estimated item difficulty (b) parameters and a 2PL model that estimated both item discrimination (a) and difficulty (b) parameters. The models were compared using information theory metrics: the Akaike information criterion (AIC), Bayesian information criterion (BIC), and the Gilula–Haberman log penalty (GHP). All three criteria penalize model complexity to varying degrees and favor models with lower values. The 2PL model evidenced better fit than the Rasch model for the AIC (142,018 vs. 142,271), the BIC (142,279 vs. 142,408), and the GHP (0.527 vs. 0.528), suggesting the model fit improvement of the 2PL was sufficient to favor it despite including additional model parameters. We thus retained the 2PL model for the remaining analyses. Reliability for the estimated 2PL trait score was good (expected a posteriori reliability = 0.82).
The last step to evaluate the degree to which the 2PL model explained the data was to analyze whether the assumption of local independence holds. We evaluated the presence of locally dependent pairs using the adjusted Q3 statistic (aQ3), which corrects for bias in Yen’s [25] Q3 values by centering them on the average value [26]. Values of |0.20| (in the r metric) are common cut-offs for flagging locally dependent pairs [27]. No item pairs had aQ3 values greater than |0.20|, so local dependence was overall modest. We nevertheless explored the pairs with the highest aQ3 values to gain insight into the small, secondary factor found in the parallel analysis and the specific factors found in the exploratory bifactor analysis. The largest values were for items 14 and 16 (aQ3 = 0.18), items 11 and 15 (aQ3 = 0.15), and items 13 and 16 (aQ3 = 0.15); all remaining pairs were below 0.13.
Although local dependence was overall low, with all aQ3 values being under |0.20|, the items with the highest values were the ones that formed specific factors in the bifactor analysis, and they have highly similar item wording or meaning that created relatively redundant pairs. In these cases, many of the items overlapped in using the word “love”, and they refer to ambivalence in the context of close, emotionally intimate relationships (see Table 1). A possible source of this local dependence is that the young adults in this sample likely interpret these items in terms of close romantic relationships (vs. parental or sibling relationships), so people with limited experience with such relationships (common among young adults high in negative schizotypy [28]) are less likely to endorse them.

3.3. Item Characteristics

The 2PL model provides estimates of the values for the items’ difficulty (b) and discrimination (a) parameters (see Table 1). The item difficulty values, reflecting the trait level at which someone has a 50:50 chance of endorsing the item, ranged from −0.78 to 1.51 (see Figure 2, top panel). Four items were relatively easy. Participants endorsed items 4, 8, 10, and 15 at a higher rate, and these four items had b-values less than 0. The remaining items had difficulty values roughly between 0.50 and 1.50, so most items were on the harder end of the scale, but no item had a b value greater than two logits.
The discrimination (a) parameters, shown in the bottom panel of Figure 2, showed good discrimination overall. The a-values ranged from 1.01 to 1.64, so even the lowest value is reasonable for a self-report measure of individual differences.

3.4. Test Information

Consistent with the profile of moderate item difficulty, the SAS had a test information function that peaked at θ = 0.78, the higher end of the trait scale (see Figure 3). Possessing more ambivalence indicates a higher risk for poor functioning, so it is thus apt for the scale to provide the most information at the higher end of the underlying trait.

3.5. Item Fit

To evaluate item fit, we considered Infit and Outfit, common item fit statistics based on mean-square residuals [29]. The expected value is 1, and values greater than 1 reflect underfitting items (i.e., noisier than expected). We applied a common threshold of 1.15 to flag underfitting items in this large sample. As Figure 4 shows, the values rarely exceeded 1, and never by very much.
A limitation of Infit and Outfit statistics, however, is that they can become insensitive in large samples [30]. We thus also evaluated item RMSD, a fit statistic that evaluates deviations between the true and fitted item response functions. In their work, Köhler et al. [31] suggested RMSD misfit benchmarks of negligible (RMSD < 0.02), small (0.02 ≤ RMSD < 0.05), medium (0.05 ≤ RMSD < 0.08), and large (RMSD ≥ 0.08). As Figure 5 illustrates, item fit was excellent for all 19 SAS items. RMSD values were below 0.02 for all items, suggesting that item misfit was negligible.

3.6. Differential Item Functioning

DIF was investigated to see if subgroups with the same true trait level varied in their likely responses. Such differences would reflect item bias—the operation of nuisance or construct-irrelevant factors—rather than true group differences in the underlying trait [32]. We used the logistic ordinal regression method implemented in lordif [33], which uses IRT-based trait scores and iterative purification methods to identify items showing uniform and non-uniform DIF [33,34]. A virtue of this approach is that it does not require specifying anchor items known to be DIF-free, which is important when relatively little is known about a scale’s item behavior. Because of our large sample size, we evaluated DIF via effect size statistics [35,36], particularly McFadden’s R2 [37], using a threshold of R2 = 0.02 (2% of the variance) to flag items for total DIF (i.e., uniform plus non-uniform DIF). Because R2 = 0.02 is a common benchmark for a “small effect size” in the R2 metric, it is a lenient threshold for an initial evaluation of items for possible DIF.
For gender, a comparison of women (n = 5466) and men (n = 1622) indicated that none of the 19 items was flagged for DIF using a R2 = 0.02 threshold, so any gender-based DIF is at most very small (see Table 1). To explore DIF further, we reduced the threshold to R2 = 0.01. Even at that level, no items were flagged were gender-based DIF. We thus conclude that the SAS items show essentially no gender-based DIF, inasmuch as an item bias effect less than R2 = 0.01 is too negligible to warrant attention.
For race and ethnicity, we recoded the categories into European-American participants (n = 4861) and participants of color (n = 1907). Participants in the declined-to-endorse/endorsed-another-category group (n = 152) were omitted for this analysis. Granted, this is a broad-brush approach that is limited by what was recorded in the original data. However, none of the 19 items was flagged for DIF using a R2 = 0.02 threshold, so DIF based on racial-ethnic identification is at most small for these items (see Table 1). To explore DIF further, we found that reducing the threshold to 0.01 yielded two items flagged for DIF: item 13 (“Everyone has a lot of hidden resentment toward his loved one”; R2 = 0.017) and item 16 (“I usually find that feelings of hate will interfere when I have grown to love someone”; R2 = 0.017). For both items, participants of color were more likely than European-American participants to endorse the item, given the same true trait level. There’s no apparent interpretation for these very small DIF effects, but it’s notable that these two items appeared among the handful of items with relatively larger local dependence.

4. Discussion

The SAS is one of the few self-report tools available for measuring ambivalence within the context of schizotypy and psychopathology more generally. We conducted a detailed psychometric evaluation of the SAS to discern its relative strengths and weaknesses and to examine lingering questions about dimensionality. Using a large sample of nearly 7100 adults, we applied IRT methods to examine the scale and item features and to determine whether items showed differential item functioning. We believe that the sample was appropriate for evaluating the SAS given that studies of schizotypy often focus on young adults who fall in an age range of greatest risk for developing schizophrenia-spectrum disorders.

4.1. Dimensionality

Based on a group of criteria, the SAS appears to be essentially unidimensional. According to parallel analysis and MAP, there were at most two noteworthy factors, but the first was clearly dominant and greatly exceeded the 4:1 guidelines for eigenvalue ratios used for essential unidimensionality [21]. Subsequent exploratory bifactor analysis and local dependence statistics found evidence for a strong common factor and indicated that any minor factors were largely driven by locally dependent item pairs containing overlapping wordings or redundant meaning.
Our finding of essential unidimensionality conflicts with MacAuley et al. [10], who found a three-factor structure using exploratory procedures. However, one must consider the sample size, population, and item types. While the sample used in the present study included nearly 7100 respondents, the MacAuley et al. study included only 334 participants. While MacAuley et al. removed participants whose ages were outside of the range of 18–25 years, the current study did not exclude data based on participants’ ages, although the sample was predominantly within that age range. Finally, MacAuley et al. transformed the dichotomous SAS items into Likert-style items, which precludes a direct comparison of results, whereas our analyses used the original binary response scale and statistical models appropriate for such responses (e.g., parallel and factor analyses based on tetrachoric correlations). Taken together, given our much larger sample size and our administration of the scale in the format in which it was originally developed, we are inclined toward the evidence in favor of essential unidimensionality.

4.2. Item and Test Features

The SAS items fit a 2PL IRT model and showed limited evidence for item misfit. All items showed acceptable levels of item discrimination, so they all were effective at differentiating between people with different trait levels. Concerning item difficulty, on the whole, the items lean toward the “hard” end—it takes a relatively high level of the underlying schizotypal ambivalence trait to be likely to endorse them. The test information function is centered around 0.80, so the scale’s scores are most reliable in that region. This seems appropriate for a scale that is predominantly used in general samples, where there is more interest in reliably discriminating between participants on the higher end of the trait, and it follows from schizotypy scale development procedures in Kwapil et al. [12] and Gross et al. [38].

4.3. Differential Item Functioning

A notable finding was the lack of evidence for item bias. Analysis of differential item functioning found, at most, very small effect sizes for gender and for racial-ethnic identity. Given the large sample size, it’s reasonable to conclude that gender-based DIF is essentially zero for the SAS. This indicates that researchers can have confidence in any gender differences that are observed using the scale. Likewise, DIF based on racial and ethnic identification was at most very small. Only two items had R2 values greater than 1%, so DIF based on this categorization appears minimal. Because the underlying items are free of DIF, any group differences are likely to reflect true differences in the underlying trait, not specious differences driven by nuisance factors. Nevertheless, this issue deserves continued attention in future research, especially in light of the imbalanced group sizes and the relatively coarse classification of racial and ethnic identities that was afforded by the existing dataset.

4.4. Limitations and Conclusions

Several limitations of the present research should be noted. First, our sample consisted of adults who were not selected based on clinical features. The assessment of schizotypy has critical relevance for studies of at-risk and clinical samples (e.g., [12,39]), and the SAS has been used successfully in a small number of studies that recruited based on clinical criteria (e.g., psychiatric hospitalization or family history [14,15]) or that applied structured clinical interviews to assess a range of clinical disorders [9,11,16]. Nevertheless, it is not yet known how well the psychometric features of the SAS illustrated in the present sample would replicate in high-risk and clinical samples, and we believe that studying the scale’s performance in such samples is a major long-term goal for future research. Second, the present sample, while large, was nevertheless notably imbalanced regarding gender, and it was unable to provide fine differentiation for aspects of race and ethnicity. The large absolute numbers of men and women should suggest that the estimates are stable, but a key goal for future research should be to examine the psychometric qualities of the SAS in large samples that offer a more detailed look at the psychometric equivalence of the SAS across racial and ethnic identities. Finally, given the age of the SAS and subsequent advances in scale development, it is worth considering how these and future psychometric analyses could inform avenues for future revisions, such as rewording, adding, or omitting items.
The goal of the present study was to provide a comprehensive assessment of the psychometric properties of the SAS in a large sample of non-clinically ascertained young adults. Overall, the analyses support the psychometric features of the SAS: it appears to be essentially unidimensional, the items have an appropriate range of difficulty and good item discrimination, the items fit a 2PL model well, and DIF based on gender and racial and ethnic identity is at most minimal. The present psychometric findings, along with the growing empirical support from questionnaire, interview, and ambulatory assessment studies, support the continued use of the SAS. Furthermore, its relatively brief and non-invasive format makes it ideal for screening purposes and for inclusion in laboratory protocols.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.B.D. and T.R.K.; Formal Analysis, L.B.D. and P.J.S.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, L.B.D., P.J.S. and T.R.K.; Writing—Review and Editing, L.B.D., P.J.S. and T.R.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of North Carolina at Greensboro (protocol #17-0182, approval date 8 May 2017).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all research participants in this study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data and R files used in the analyses are publicly available at the project’s Open Science Framework archive (https://osf.io/ztycp/, accessed on 22 July 2021).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Kwapil, T.R.; Barrantes-Vidal, N. Schizotypy: Looking Back and Moving Forward. Schizophr. Bull. 2014, 41 (Suppl. S2), S366–S373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Bleuler, E.P. Dementia Praecox of the Group of Schizophrenias (J. Zinkin, Trans.); International Universities Press: New York, NY, USA, 1950. [Google Scholar]
  3. Meehl, P.E. Schizotaxia, Schizotypy, Schizophrenia. Am. Psychol. 1962, 17, 827–838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Meehl, P.E. Toward an Integrated Theory of Schizotaxia, Schizotypy, and Schizophrenia. J. Pers. Disord. 1990, 4, 1–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Raulin, M.L.; Brenner, V. Ambivalence. In Symptoms of Schizophrenia; Costello, C.G., Ed.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1993; pp. 201–226. [Google Scholar]
  6. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  7. Kernberg, O. The Structural Diagnosis of Borderline Personality Organization. In Borderline Personality Disorders: The Concept, the Syndrome, the Patient; International Universities Press: New York, NY, USA, 1977; pp. 87–121. [Google Scholar]
  8. Raulin, M.L. Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale. 1986; Unpublished test. [Google Scholar]
  9. Mann, M.C.; Vaughn, A.G.; Barrantes-Vidal, N.; Raulin, M.L.; Kwapil, T.R. Social ambivalence scale as a marker of schizotypy. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2008, 196, 399–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. MacAulay, R.K.; Brown, L.S.; Minor, K.S.; Cohen, A.S. Conceptualizing Schizotypal Ambivalence: Factor Structure and Its Relationships. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2014, 202, 793–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kwapil, T.R.; Mann, M.C.; Raulin, M.L. Psychometric Properties and Concurrent Validity of the Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2002, 190, 290–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Kwapil, T.R.; Gross, G.M.; Silvia, P.J.; Raulin, M.L.; Barrantes-Vidal, N. Development and psychometric properties of the Multidimensional Schizotypy Scale: A new measure for assessing positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypy. Schizophr. Res. 2018, 193, 209–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Burgin, C.J.; Chun, C.; Horton, L.E.; Barrantes-Vidal, N.; Kwapil, T.R. Splitting of Associative Threads: The Expression of Schizotypal Ambivalence in Daily Life. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2015, 37, 349–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Yon, V.; Loas, G.; Monestes, J.-L.; Verrier, A.; Deligne, H. Psychometric Properties of the French Versions of the Cognitive Slippage Scale and Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale in 340 Subjects. L’encephale 2007, 33, 249–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Loas, G.; Dimassi, H.; Monestes, J.L.; Yon, V. Criterion Validity of the Cognitive Slippage and Schizotypal Ambivalence Scales. Psychol. Rep. 2013, 113, 930–934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Kwapil, T.R.; Edmundson, M.S.; Hernandez, L.M.; Kemp, K.C.; Rbeiz, K.S.; Clark, H.E.; Bathery, A.J.; Raulin, M.L.; Barrantes-Vidal, N. Schizotypal Ambivalence as a Predictor of Schizophrenia-Spectrum and Borderline Personality Traits in Young Adults: Converging Results from Three Interview Studies. 2022; Unpublished Work. [Google Scholar]
  17. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Core Team: Vienna, Austria, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  18. Revelle, W. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. R Package Version 2.2.3. 2022. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych (accessed on 22 July 2021).
  19. Robitzsch, A.; Kiefer, T.; Wu, M. TAM: Test Analysis Modules. R package Version 4.0.1. 2021. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=TAM (accessed on 22 July 2021).
  20. Choi, S.W.; Gibbons, L.E.; Crane, P.K. lordif: Logistic Ordinal Regression Differential Item Functioning Using IRT. R Package Version 0.3.3. 2016. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lordif (accessed on 22 July 2021).
  21. Slocum-Gori, S.L.; Zumbo, B.D. Assessing the Unidimensionality of Psychological Scales: Using Multiple Criteria from Factor Analysis. Soc. Indic. Res. 2010, 102, 443–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Silvia, P.J.; Rodriguez, R.M. Time to Renovate the Humor Styles Questionnaire? An Item Response Theory Analysis of the HSQ. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Lord, F.M. Applications of Item Response Theory to Practical Testing Problems; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Taylor & Francis: Abingdon, UK, 1980. [Google Scholar]
  24. Zwick, W.; Velicer, W. Comparison of Five Rules for Determining the Number of Components to Retain. Psychol. Bull. 1986, 99, 432–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Yen, W.M. Effects of Local Item Dependence on the Fit and Equating Performance of the Three-Parameter Logistic Model. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1984, 8, 125–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Marais, I. Local Dependence. In Rasch Models in Health; Christensen, K.B., Kreiner, S., Mesbah, M., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2013; pp. 111–130. [Google Scholar]
  27. Christensen, K.B.; Makransky, G.; Horton, M. Critical Values for Yen’s Q3: Identification of Local Dependence in the Rasch Model Using Residual Correlations. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 2017, 41, 178–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Kemp, K.C.; Bathery, A.J.; Barrantes-Vidal, N.; Kwapil, T.R. Positive, Negative, and Disorganized Schizotypy Predict Differential Patterns of Interview-Rated Schizophrenia-Spectrum Symptoms and Impairment. Assessment 2021, 28, 141–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Bond, T.G.; Yan, Z.; Heine, M. Applying the Rasch Model: Fundamental Measurement in the Human Sciences, 4th ed.; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  30. Wu, M.; Adams, R.J. Properties of Rasch residual fit statistics. J. Appl. Meas. 2013, 14, 339–355. [Google Scholar]
  31. Köhler, C.; Robitzsch, A.; Hartig, J. A Bias-Corrected RMSD Item Fit Statistic: An Evaluation and Comparison to Alternatives. J. Educ. Behav. Stat. 2020, 45, 251–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Osterlind, S.J.; Everson, H.T. Differential Item Functioning; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  33. Choi, S.W.; Gibbons, L.E.; Crane, P.K. Lordif: An R Package for Detecting Differential Item Functioning Using Iterative Hybrid Ordinal Logistic Regression/Item Response Theory and Monte Carlo Simulations. J. Stat. Softw. 2011, 39, 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Crane, P.K.; Gibbons, L.E.; Ocepek-Welikson, K.; Cook, K.; Cella, D.; Narasimhalu, K.; Hays, R.D.; Teresi, J.A. A comparison of three sets of criteria for determining the presence of differential item functioning using ordinal logistic regression. Qual. Life Res. 2007, 16, 69–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Jodoin, M.G.; Gierl, M.J. Evaluating Type I Error and Power Rates Using an Effect Size Measure with the Logistic Regression Procedure for DIF Detection. Appl. Meas. Educ. 2001, 14, 329–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Meade, A.W. A taxonomy of effect size measures for the differential functioning of items and scales. J. Appl. Psychol. 2010, 95, 728–743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Menard, S. Coefficients of Determination for Multiple Logistic Regression Analysis. Am. Stat. 2000, 54, 17–24. [Google Scholar]
  38. Gross, G.M.; Kwapil, T.R.; Raulin, M.L.; Silvia, P.J.; Barrantes-Vidal, N. The multidimensional schizotypy scale-brief: Scale development and psychometric properties. Psychiatry Res. 2018, 261, 7–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Mason, O.J. The Assessment of Schizotypy and Its Clinical Relevance. Schizophr. Bull. 2015, 41, S374–S385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Actual and resampled eigenvalues from the parallel analysis. Note: Only the first 7 factors are shown for clarity.
Figure 1. Actual and resampled eigenvalues from the parallel analysis. Note: Only the first 7 factors are shown for clarity.
Behavsci 12 00247 g001
Figure 2. Item difficulty (b, top panel) and discrimination (a, bottom panel) values from the 2PL model, sorted from low to high.
Figure 2. Item difficulty (b, top panel) and discrimination (a, bottom panel) values from the 2PL model, sorted from low to high.
Behavsci 12 00247 g002
Figure 3. Test information function for the SAS.
Figure 3. Test information function for the SAS.
Behavsci 12 00247 g003
Figure 4. Infit and Outfit statistics for the SAS items.
Figure 4. Infit and Outfit statistics for the SAS items.
Behavsci 12 00247 g004
Figure 5. RMSD item fit for the SAS items.
Figure 5. RMSD item fit for the SAS items.
Behavsci 12 00247 g005
Table 1. SAS items and descriptive statistics.
Table 1. SAS items and descriptive statistics.
ItemTextMean (Percent Endorsed)SDItem-Scale Correlation (r)IRT DifficultyIRT DiscriminationGender DIF (R2)Race-Ethnicity DIF (R2)
1Often I feel like I hate even my favorite activities.0.180.380.471.451.440.0000.002
2My thoughts and feelings always seem to be contradictory.0.280.450.560.841.640.0000.001
3My feelings about my own worth as a person are constantly changing back and forth.0.380.490.570.451.570.0000.006
4Very often when I feel like doing something, at the same time I don’t feel like doing it.0.680.460.49−0.781.30.0030.000
5When I am trying to make a decision, it almost feels like I am physically switching from side to side.0.290.450.480.991.140.0000.000
6It’s impossible to know how you feel because the people around you are constantly changing.0.170.380.461.511.40.0000.002
7I always seem to be the most unsure of myself at the same time that I am most confident of myself.0.370.480.540.521.390.0010.004
8I always seem to have difficulty deciding what I would like to do.0.520.50.5−0.071.150.0010.004
9Most people seem to know what they’re feeling more easily than I do.0.360.480.570.551.580.0000.000
10Love and hate tend to go together.0.590.490.47−0.421.010.0010.006
11Love never seems to last very long.0.190.40.461.451.260.0010.007
12The closer I get to people, the more I am annoyed by their faults.0.290.450.471.011.090.0010.000
13Everyone has a lot of hidden resentment toward his loved one.0.360.480.540.61.330.0000.017
14I have noticed that feelings of tenderness often turn into feelings of anger.0.180.380.51.371.540.0010.005
15My experiences with love have always been muddled with great frustrations.0.580.490.53−0.331.330.0000.000
16I usually find that feelings of hate will interfere when I have grown to love someone.0.270.450.510.991.30.0000.017
17A sense of shame has often interfered with my accepting words of praise from others.0.350.480.550.621.430.0000.002
18I usually experience doubt when I have accomplished something that I have worked on for a long time.0.380.490.510.521.20.0000.002
19I doubt if I can ever be sure exactly what my true interests are.0.230.420.531.11.560.0020.000
Note: n = 7096. For DIF, McFadden R2 values for total DIF are reported (e.g., a value of 0.017 is R2 = 0.017, or 1.7% of the variance).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Deters, L.B.; Silvia, P.J.; Kwapil, T.R. The Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale: An Item Response Theory Analysis. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 247. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080247

AMA Style

Deters LB, Silvia PJ, Kwapil TR. The Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale: An Item Response Theory Analysis. Behavioral Sciences. 2022; 12(8):247. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080247

Chicago/Turabian Style

Deters, Lauren B., Paul J. Silvia, and Thomas R. Kwapil. 2022. "The Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale: An Item Response Theory Analysis" Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 8: 247. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080247

APA Style

Deters, L. B., Silvia, P. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2022). The Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale: An Item Response Theory Analysis. Behavioral Sciences, 12(8), 247. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080247

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop