Do Dark Humour Users Have Dark Tendencies? Relationships between Dark Humour, the Dark Tetrad, and Online Trolling
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Dark Tetrad
- Psychopathy can be summarised by high thrill-seeking behaviour and low empathy [18], and it has also been associated with externalising behaviours [19]. Ref. [18] identified a four-factor structure of psychopathy, consisting of callous affect, criminal tendencies, erratic lifestyle, and interpersonal manipulation.
- Narcissism can be described as self-absorption and feelings of superiority and entitlement, which can also be subdivided into four scales [20], namely, exploitativeness/entitlement, leadership/authority, superiority/arrogance, and self-absorption/self-admiration.
- Sadism is defined as callousness, deriving pleasure from the suffering of others, and the enjoyment of cruelty [15].
1.2. Dark Humour
1.3. Online Trolling
1.4. The Present Study
- Sadism is positively related to dark humour trait, enjoyment, and ability (large effect).
- Psychopathy is positively related to dark humour trait, enjoyment, and ability (medium effect).
- Machiavellianism is positively related to dark humour trait, enjoyment, and ability (small effect)
- Narcissism is positively related to dark humour trait, enjoyment, and ability (small effect).
- Dark humour trait is positively related to online trolling trait.
- Dark humour enjoyment is positively related to online trolling enjoyment.
- Dark humour ability is positively related to online trolling ability.
- Dark Tetrad traits will predict the ability to troll.
- Trolls will be better at trolling than non-trolls.
- Online trolls will perceive trolling attempts more positively than non-trolls.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. The Dark Tetrad
2.2.2. Online Trolling
- Trait: The Global Assessment of Internet Trolling-Revised (GAIT-R) [43] is an 8-item self-report used to assess online trolling, including items such as “I have disrupted people in comment sections of websites” (α = 0.78). Items were measured on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 “strongly disagree” to 5 “strongly agree”.
- Enjoyment: Participants were presented with the definition of online trolling [9] and an online trolling scenario where they were trolled by two players in an online shooter game (receiving condition). They were asked to rate how funny, boring, offensive, and enjoyable (adapted from [48]) they would find being trolled in this way. Each item was measured on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (e.g., “very unfunny” or “very unenjoyable”) to 6 (e.g., “very funny” or “very enjoyable”). Two open-ended questions were then asked for exploratory purposes: “How would being trolled in this way make you feel, in your own words?” and “If you had the opportunity, would you troll these players back? Why?”
- Ability: Participants were presented with an online trolling definition [9] and a second online trolling scenario where they encountered the same two players as in the previous scenario, and now could troll them back (sender condition). Participants were instructed to write down ways they could troll these players back effectively (maximum of 15 ways). They were asked to rate how funny, boring, offensive, and enjoyable they would find trolling someone back on the same 6-point Likert scale. An additional open-ended question was asked: “How would trolling someone back make you feel, in your own words?” The detailed task instructions are available on OSF (https://osf.io/qghyk/?view_only=e3b1819e307f403a8991c20a66b26a47, accessed on 30 April 2024).
2.2.3. Dark Humour
- Trait: The Dark Humour Scale [49] is an 8-item self-report used to measure the dark humour trait, including items like “I have fun confronting others with macabre and morbid jokes and banter” (α = 0.91). Items were measured on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 “strongly disagree” to 7 “strongly agree”.
- Enjoyment: Dark humour enjoyment was measured through the judgement of memes obtained from the internet. Seven different memes were presented and rated according to how funny, boring, offensive, and enjoyable participants found each one (α = 0.88). Each item was measured on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (e.g., “very unfunny” or “very unenjoyable”) to 6 (e.g., “very funny” or “very enjoyable”).
- Ability: Dark humour ability was measured through participants’ ability to produce punchlines for seven blank meme templates obtained from the internet. The definition of dark humour was presented [26,27,29,30], followed by seven different blank meme templates. Participants were asked to think of dark humour punchlines which could fit each template, with a maximum of five punchlines per meme. The resulting punchlines were scored for quantity (i.e., total number of punchlines; α = 0.95) and quality. The latter consisted of the authors rating the wittiness (based on the guidelines of [50]) and dark humour (based on the definition of dark humour) of the best punchline per meme from 1 to 5. The authors’ ratings correlated 0.30–0.59 (p ≤ 0.001; Mdn = 0.43) per meme for wittiness (α = 0.80) and 0.59–0.77 (p < 0.001; Mdn = 0.66) per meme for dark humour (α = 0.88). The memes employed in this study are available on OSF (https://osf.io/qghyk/?view_only=e3b1819e307f403a8991c20a66b26a47, accessed on 30 April 2024).
2.2.4. Humorous Temperament
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Dark Humour and the Dark Tetrad
3.2. Dark Humour and Online Trolling
3.3. Dark Tetrad and Online Trolling
3.4. Supplementary Analyses
3.4.1. Trolling Enjoyment
3.4.2. Online trolling and the Dark Tetrad
3.4.3. Humorous Temperament, Dark Humour, Online Trolling, and the Dark Tetrad
4. Discussion
4.1. Hypothesis 1
4.2. Hypotheses 2 and 3
4.3. General Limitations and Future Research
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Dixon, S. Types of Online Abuse Experienced by Victims in the United Kingdom as of February 2022. Statista. 13 July 2022. Available online: https://www.statista.com/statistics/1319815/uk-online-abuse-experienced/ (accessed on 30 April 2024).
- YPulse. 3 Stats That Show What Memes Mean to Gen Z & Millennials. 5 March 2019. Available online: https://www.ypulse.com/article/2019/03/05/3-stats-that-show-what-memes-mean-to-gen-z-millennials/ (accessed on 30 April 2024).
- Oxford Learners Dictionaries. Meme Noun. Available online: https://www.oxfordlearnersdictionaries.com/definition/english/meme (accessed on 17 April 2023).
- Obrdlik, A.J. “Gallows humor”—A sociological phenomenon. Am. J. Sociol. 1942, 47, 709–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dionigi, A.; Duradoni, M.; Vagnoli, L. Humor and the dark triad: Relationships among narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy and comic styles. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2022, 197, 111766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, R.A.; Lastuk, J.M.; Jeffery, J.; Vernon, P.A.; Veselka, L. Relationships between the dark triad and humor styles: A replication and extension. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 52, 178–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torres-Marín, J.; Navarro-Carrillo, G.; Carretero-Dios, H. Differentiating the traits of the Dark Tetrad in their linkages with humor styles, dispositions toward ridicule and laughter, and comic styles. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2022, 185, 111281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veselka, L.; Schermer, J.A.; Martin, R.A.; Vernon, P.A. Relations between humor styles and the dark triad traits of personality. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2010, 48, 772–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buckels, E.E.; Trapnell, P.D.; Paulhus, D.L. Trolls just want to have fun. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2014, 67, 97–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Navarro-Carrillo, G.; Torres-Marín, J.; Carretero-Dios, H. Do trolls just want to have fun? Assessing the role of humor-related traits in online trolling behavior. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2021, 114, 106551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Funder, D.C. The Personality Puzzle, 6th ed.; W. W. Norton & Company: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Costa, P.T., Jr.; McCrae, R.R. The five-factor model of personality and its relevance to personality disorders. J. Personal. Disord. 1992, 6, 343–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paulhus, D.L.; Williams, K.M. The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. J. Res. Personal. 2002, 36, 556–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodboy, A.K.; Martin, M.M. The personality profile of a cyberbully: Examining the dark triad. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 49, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buckels, E.E.; Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. Behavioral confirmation of everyday sadism. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 2201–2209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chabrol, H.; Van Leeuwen, N.; Rodgers, R.; Séjourné, N. Contributions of psychopathic, narcissistic, Machiavellian, and sadistic personality traits to juvenile delinquency. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2009, 47, 734–739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christie, R.; Geis, F.L. Studies in Machiavellianism; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1970. [Google Scholar]
- Hare, R.D. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R), 2nd ed.; Multi-Health Systems, Inc.: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Seibert, L.A.; Miller, J.D.; Few, L.R.; Zeichner, A.; Lynam, D.R. An examination of the structure of self-report psychopathy measures and their relations with general traits and externalizing behaviors. Personal. Disord. Theory Res. Treat. 2011, 2, 193–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Emmons, R.A. Factor analysis and construct validity of the narcissistic personality inventory. J. Personal. Assess. 1984, 48, 291–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. Introducing the short dark triad (SD3). Assessment 2014, 21, 28–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paulhus, D.L.; Buckels, E.E.; Trapnell, P.D.; Jones, D.N. Screening for dark personalities: The short dark tetrad (SD4). Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2021, 37, 208–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moor, L.; Anderson, J.R. A systematic literature review of the relationship between dark personality traits and antisocial online behaviours. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2019, 144, 40–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruch, W.; Heintz, S.; Platt, T.; Wagner, L.; Proyer, R.T. Broadening humor: Comic styles differentially tap into temperament, character, and ability. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 309565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, C.; Lau, C.; Yosopov, L.; Saklofske, D.H. The seriousness of humour: Examining the relationship and pathways between sexist humour and the Dark Tetrad traits. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 42, 32030–32053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baldick, C. The Concise Oxford Dictionary of Literary Terms; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Mindess, H.; Miller, C.; Turek, J.; Bender, A.; Corbin, S. The Antioch Humor Test: Making Sense of Humor; Avon Books: New York, NY, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Willinger, U.; Hergovich, A.; Schmoeger, M.; Deckert, M.; Stoettner, S.; Bunda, I.; Witting, A.; Seidler, M.; Moser, R.; Kacena, S.; et al. Cognitive and emotional demands of black humour processing: The role of intelligence, aggressiveness and mood. Cogn. Process. 2017, 18, 159–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cambridge Dictionary. Black Humour. Available online: https://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/english/black-humour (accessed on 30 April 2024).
- Aillaud, M.; Piolat, A. Influence of gender on judgments of dark and nondark humor. Individ. Differ. Res. 2012, 10, 211–222. [Google Scholar]
- Freud, S.; Strachey, J.; Tyson, A.; Strachey, A.; Freud, A. The future of an illusion, Civilization and its discontents and other works (1927–1931). In The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud; Original work published 1927; Hogarth Press: London, UK, 2001; Volume XXI. [Google Scholar]
- Demjén, Z. Laughing at cancer: Humour, empowerment, solidarity and coping online. J. Pragmat. 2016, 101, 18–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Felton, J.S. Burnout as a clinical entity—Its importance in health care workers. Occup. Med. 1998, 48, 237–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moran, C.; Massam, M. An evaluation of humour in emergency work. Australas. J. Disaster Trauma Stud. 1997, 3, 26–38. [Google Scholar]
- Ostrower, C. Humor as a defense mechanism during the holocaust. Interpret. J. Bible Theol. 2015, 69, 183–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bischetti, L.; Canal, P.; Bambini, V. Funny but aversive: A large-scale survey of the emotional response to COVID-19 humor in the Italian population during the lockdown. Lingua 2021, 249, 102963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Skórka, P.; Grzywacz, B.; Moroń, D.; Lenda, M. COVID-19 in Memes: The Adaptive Response of Societies to the Pandemic? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 12969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nicol, S. Cyber-bullying and trolling. Youth Stud. Austrailia 2012, 31, 3–4. [Google Scholar]
- Cook, C.; Schaafsma, J.; Antheunis, M. Under the bridge: An in-depth examination of online trolling in the gaming context. New Media Soc. 2017, 20, 3323–3340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manuoğlu, E.; Öner-Özkan, B. Sarcastic and deviant trolling in Turkey: Associations with dark triad and aggression. Soc. Media+ Soc. 2022, 8, 20563051221126053. [Google Scholar]
- Leite, Â.; Cardoso, S.; Monteiro, A.P. Dark personality traits and online behaviors: Portuguese versions of cyberstalking, online harassment, flaming and trolling scales. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 6136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Volkmer, S.A.; Gaube, S.; Raue, M.; Lermer, E. Troll story: The dark tetrad and online trolling revisited with a glance at humor. PLoS ONE 2023, 18, e0280271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sest, N.; March, E. Constructing the cyber-troll: Psychopathy, sadism, and empathy. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017, 119, 69–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, R.; Puhlik-Doris, P.; Larsen, G.; Gray, J.; Weir, K. Individual differences in uses of humor and their relation to psychological well-being: Development of the humor styles questionnaire. J. Res. Personal. 2003, 37, 48–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lau, C.; Chiesi, F.; Hofmann, J.; Saklofske, D.H.; Ruch, W. Development and linguistic cue analysis of the state-trait cheerfulness inventory–Short form. J. Personal. Assess. 2021, 103, 547–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruch, W.; Köhler, G.; Van Thriel, C. Assessing the “humorous temperament”: Construction of the facet and standard trait forms of the State-Trait-Cheerfulness-Inventory—STCI. Humor 1996, 9, 303–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erdfelder, E.; Faul, F.; Buchner, A. GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 1996, 28, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruch, W.; Rath, S. The nature of humor appreciation: Toward an integration of perception of stimulus properties and affective experience. Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 1993, 6, 363–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heintz, S.; Ruch, W. Dark Humour Scale; Unpublished questionnaire; University of Zurich: Zurich, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Ruch, W.; Heintz, S. Humor production and creativity: Overview and recommendations. In Creativity and Humor; Luria, S.R., Baer, J., Kaufman, J.C., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2019; pp. 1–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2022; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 30 April 2024).
- Wickham, H.; Averick, M.; Bryan, J.; Chang, W.; McGowan, L.D.; François, R.; Grolemund, G.; Hayes, A.; Henry, L.; Hester, J.; et al. Welcome to the tidyverse. J. Open Source Softw. 2019, 4, 1686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morey, R.; Rouder, J. _BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes Factors for Common Designs_. (R Package Version 0.9.12-4.4) [Computer Software]. 2022. Available online: https://cloud.r-project.org/web/packages/BayesFactor/index.html (accessed on 30 April 2024).
- Revelle, W. psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research. (R Package Version 2.2.5) [Computer software]. 2022. Available online: https://cloud.r-project.org/web/packages/psych/index.html (accessed on 30 April 2024).
- Schloerke, B.; Cook, D.; Larmarange, J.; Briatte, F.; Marbach, M.; Thoen, E.; Elberg, A.; Crowley, J. _GGally: Extension to ‘ggplot2’_. (R Package Version 2.1.2) [Computer software]. 2021. Available online: https://cloud.r-project.org/web/packages/GGally/index.html (accessed on 30 April 2024).
- Funder, D.C.; Ozer, D.J. Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 2019, 2, 156–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gignac, G.E.; Szodorai, E.T. Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2016, 102, 74–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wetzels, R.; Matzke, D.; Lee, M.D.; Rouder, J.N.; Iverson, G.J.; Wagenmakers, E.-J. Statistical evidence in experimental psychology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 6, 291–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pabel, A.; Pearce, P.L. Developing the humour repertoire concept to guide future tourism-humour research. Eur. J. Humour Res. 2019, 7, 120–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofmann, J.; Platt, T.; Lau, C.; Torres-Marín, J. Gender differences in humor-related traits, humor appreciation, production, comprehension, (neural) responses, use, and correlates: A systematic review. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 42, 16451–16464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bischetti, L.; Attardo, S. From mode adoption to saluting a dead kitten. Reactions to a humorous tweet by Ricky Gervais. In The Pragmatics of Humour in Interactive Contexts; Bernabéu, E.L., Ed.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2023; pp. 65–86. [Google Scholar]
- Chovanec, J.; Tsakona, V. The girl is on fire! Interactional humour in YouTube comments on the Notre Dame disaster. In The Pragmatics of Humour in Interactive Contexts; Bernabéu, E.L., Ed.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2023; pp. 87–107. [Google Scholar]
- Dynel, M. On being roasted, toasted and burned: (Meta) pragmatics of Wendy’s Twitter humour. J. Pragmat. 2020, 166, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dynel, M. Irony, Deception and Humour: Seeking the Truth about Overt and Covert Untruthfulness; Walter de Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2018. [Google Scholar]
Variables | M | SD | Range | N |
---|---|---|---|---|
Dark Tetrad | ||||
Psychopathy | 14.82 | 4.40 | 7–28 | 160 |
Machiavellianism | 23.77 | 3.92 | 9–32 | 160 |
Narcissism | 19.35 | 4.43 | 7–32 | 160 |
Sadism | 18.79 | 5.27 | 8–34 | 160 |
Online Trolling | ||||
Trait | 15.30 | 5.36 | 8–32 | 160 |
Enjoyment | 11.69 | 3.93 | 4–24 | 159 |
Ability | 3.46 | 2.17 | 0–15 | 160 |
Dark humour | ||||
Trait | 35.22 | 10.86 | 8–56 | 160 |
Enjoyment | 111.13 | 23.89 | 28–167 | 158 |
Ability quantity | 10.32 | 9.32 | 0–35 | 160 |
Ability wittiness | 2.10 | 0.56 | 1.00–4.00 | 140 |
Ability darkness | 2.39 | 0.67 | 1.08–4.21 | 140 |
Humour temperament | ||||
Cheerfulness | 27.00 | 4.22 | 17–36 | 160 |
Seriousness | 26.51 | 4.82 | 14–39 | 160 |
Bad mood | 21.84 | 5.88 | 10–39 | 160 |
Dark Humour | Psychopathy | Machiavellianism | Narcissism | Sadism | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | BF | r | p | BF | r | p | BF | r | p | BF | |
Trait | 0.40 | <0.001 | >100 | 0.53 | <0.001 | >100 | 0.25 | <0.001 | 21.64 | 0.56 | <0.001 | >100 |
Enjoyment | 0.18 | 0.010 | 2.40 | 0.29 | <0.001 | >100 | 0.19 | 0.020 | 2.85 | 0.36 | <0.001 | >100 |
Ability quantity | 0.14 | 0.090 | 0.80 | 0.18 | 0.030 | 2.11 | 0.11 | 0.200 | 0.47 | 0.17 | 0.060 | 1.52 |
Ability wittiness | 0.17 | 0.050 | 1.31 | 0.28 | <0.001 | 45.06 | 0.25 | <0.001 | 12.28 | 0.23 | <0.001 | 8.24 |
Ability darkness | 0.20 | 0.010 | 2.69 | 0.19 | 0.020 | 2.52 | 0.27 | <0.001 | 34.70 | 0.34 | <0.001 | >100 |
Dark Humour | Online Trolling Trait | Online Trolling Enjoyment | Online Trolling Ability | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | BF | r | p | BF | r | p | BF | |
Trait | 0.57 | <0.001 | >100 | 0.17 | 0.020 | 1.60 | 0.34 | <0.001 | >100 |
Enjoyment | 0.32 | <0.001 | >100 | 0.14 | 0.060 | 0.82 | 0.34 | <0.001 | >100 |
Ability quantity | 0.22 | 0.010 | 9.29 | 0.15 | 0.150 | 1.10 | 0.43 | <0.001 | >100 |
Ability wittiness | 0.22 | 0.020 | 4.81 | 0.20 | 0.020 | 2.57 | 0.32 | <0.001 | >100 |
Ability darkness | 0.38 | <0.001 | >100 | 0.23 | 0.010 | 6.04 | 0.34 | <0.001 | >100 |
Variable | Cheerfulness | Seriousness | Bad Mood | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | BF | r | p | BF | r | p | BF | |
Dark Tetrad | |||||||||
Psychopathy | −0.03 | 0.724 | 0.19 | −0.13 | 0.104 | 0.66 | 0.17 | 0.035 | 1.58 |
Machiavellianism | −0.10 | 0.191 | 0.42 | −0.01 | 0.882 | 0.19 | 0.22 | 0.006 | 7.62 |
Narcissism | 0.25 | 0.001 | 28.39 | 0.13 | 0.103 | 0.66 | −0.11 | 0.171 | 0.45 |
Sadism | −0.16 | 0.039 | 1.44 | −0.18 | 0.020 | 2.58 | 0.25 | 0.001 | 29.51 |
Online trolling | |||||||||
Trait | −0.10 | 0.227 | 0.37 | −0.11 | 0.185 | 0.43 | 0.22 | 0.005 | 8.71 |
Enjoyment | −0.12 | 0.150 | 0.50 | −0.27 | <0.001 | 54.92 | 0.10 | 0.200 | 0.41 |
Ability | −0.04 | 0.580 | 0.21 | −0.10 | 0.219 | 0.38 | −0.03 | 0.673 | 0.20 |
Dark humour | |||||||||
Trait | −0.16 | 0.047 | 1.22 | −0.17 | 0.027 | 1.95 | 0.32 | <0.001 | >100 |
Enjoyment | 0.03 | 0.733 | 0.19 | −0.12 | 0.123 | 0.58 | 0.07 | 0.376 | 0.27 |
Ability quantity | −0.01 | 0.863 | 0.19 | −0.10 | 0.204 | 0.40 | 0.03 | 0.725 | 0.19 |
Ability wittiness | 0.04 | 0.628 | 0.22 | −0.14 | 0.100 | 0.73 | −0.12 | 0.150 | 0.53 |
Ability darkness | 0.01 | 0.883 | 0.20 | −0.17 | 0.050 | 1.23 | −0.03 | 0.753 | 0.21 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Voisey, S.; Heintz, S. Do Dark Humour Users Have Dark Tendencies? Relationships between Dark Humour, the Dark Tetrad, and Online Trolling. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 493. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060493
Voisey S, Heintz S. Do Dark Humour Users Have Dark Tendencies? Relationships between Dark Humour, the Dark Tetrad, and Online Trolling. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(6):493. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060493
Chicago/Turabian StyleVoisey, Sophie, and Sonja Heintz. 2024. "Do Dark Humour Users Have Dark Tendencies? Relationships between Dark Humour, the Dark Tetrad, and Online Trolling" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 6: 493. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060493
APA StyleVoisey, S., & Heintz, S. (2024). Do Dark Humour Users Have Dark Tendencies? Relationships between Dark Humour, the Dark Tetrad, and Online Trolling. Behavioral Sciences, 14(6), 493. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060493