Next Article in Journal
Potential Energy Recovery System for Electric Heavy Forklift Based on Double Hydraulic Motor-Generators
Next Article in Special Issue
A Unified Framework for Automatic Detection of Wound Infection with Artificial Intelligence
Previous Article in Journal
Investigation of Zero Moment Point in a Partially Filled Liquid Vessel Subjected to Roll Motion
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Analysis of a Wake-Up Task-Based Mobile Alarm App

1
Department of Human-Computer Interaction, Hanyang University, Ansan-si 15588, Korea
2
Research Institute of Engineering Technology, Hanyang ERICA, Ansan-si 15588, Korea
3
Department of Knowledge Service Engineering, KAIST, Daejeon 34141, Korea
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Appl. Sci. 2020, 10(11), 3993; https://doi.org/10.3390/app10113993
Submission received: 8 May 2020 / Revised: 1 June 2020 / Accepted: 2 June 2020 / Published: 9 June 2020

Abstract

:
The latest mobile alarm apps provide wake-up tasks (e.g., solving math problems) to dismiss the alarm, and many users willingly accept such an inconvenience in return for successfully waking up on time. However, there have been no studies that investigate how the wake-up tasks are used and their effects from a human–computer interaction perspective. This study aims to deepen our understanding of how users engage and utilize the task-based alarm app by (1) examining the characteristics of different wake-up tasks and (2) extracting usage factors of hard tasks which involve physical or cognitive task loads over a certain level. We developed and deployed Alarmy, which is a task-based mobile alarm app with four wake-up task features: touching a button, taking a picture, shaking the device, and solving math problems. We collected 42.9 million in situ usage data from 211,273 US users for five months. Their alarm app usage behaviors were measured in two folds: eight alarm-set variables and five alarm-dismiss variables. Our statistical test results reveal the significant differences in alarm usage behaviors depending on the wake-up task, and the multiple regression analysis results show key usage patterns that affect the frequent uses of hard tasks, which are late alarm hours, many snoozes, and relatively more use on weekends. Our study results provide theoretical implications on behavior change as well as practical implications for designing task-based mobile alarm.

1. Introduction

Many people have experienced forced awakening using mobile alarm applications. Kurniawan et al. [1] found that the alarm clock is one of the most crucial features of smartphones, and the most popular sleep-related apps were alarm apps [2]. Additionally, Park et al. [3] conducted surveys of mobile phone usage pattern, and the results showed that many participants used alarm applications very frequently.
The latest mobile alarm apps offer wake-up task alarms that do not allow the user to dismiss an alarm until they complete a specific task that we call a wake-up task in this study. According to relevant studies [4,5] and the 14 most downloaded alarm apps in Google Play (using the search keyword ”alarm”), including our app, Alarmy (deployed in 2013), most alarm apps provide alarms with wake-up tasks. The types of wake-up tasks are diverse; they include inputting text (e.g., Earlybird Alarm Clock), taking a picture (e.g., Alarmy and Mimicker Alarm), reading a given sentence aloud (e.g., EarlyBird Alarm Clock), solving a math problem (e.g., Alarmy), walking some steps [4], playing a game (e.g., Alarmmon), and haptic features such as following a particular pattern (e.g., FreeAlarm Clock). These tasks may aggravate users by enforcing higher cognitive and physical efforts relative to the general touch-to-dismiss type of alarm. Task-based alarm users need to go through these to achieve their initial interaction goal (i.e., dismissing an alarm), and this would increase cognitive load and frustration [6]. Despite such inconvenience, the large number of downloads and amount of usage shows that many users are willing to conduct the task to get up at the right time.
The principle behind task-based alarm apps is linked to the relationship between neural activation and wakefulness [7,8,9]. From the perspective of the systemic–structural activity theory (SSAT) [10], prior studies revealed that task complexity could be related to the activation level of the brain [8,9]. In particular, Bloch [7] pointed out that the higher the level of activation is, the higher the level of wakefulness. Based on these studies, performing wake-up tasks in alarm apps can be seen as activating neurons that help users to wake.
While SSAT is helpful in understanding the general working mechanism of wake-up tasks in mobile alarm apps, it rarely considers users’ task selection and specific usage contexts. In most task-based alarm apps, users can select one of the wake-up tasks according to their preferences and needs. Even though the particular tasks can increase wakefulness, users may rarely choose them due to the ultra high level of the task difficulty. Furthermore, wake-up alarms have been relatively under-studied in human–computer interaction research (HCI). Many HCI researchers have mainly focused on using mobile sensing technologies to support healthy sleep behaviors such as monitoring sleep states [11] and measuring sleep quality [12,13]. Particularly, mobile alarm usage behavior goes through a self-regulative process: setting up the alarm to plan one’s upcoming wake-up time and selecting a task to make oneself adhere to the mobile alarm’s ”instruction” to complete a task. This study was inspired by Fogg’s behavior model [14], which explains how one’s motivation, ability, and a prompt to a task initiate an action. When a person is motivated to wake up on time, their ability may not be sufficient at the time of waking up. Therefore, a certain prompt must exist that could actually lead to one’s action of waking up. Therefore, we aimed to analyze the different usage patterns and insights of the particular group of people whom we call hard-taskers, who actively use what could be inconvenient and frustrating task-based alarms. Understanding the in situ moments of awakening with task-based alarms would be of great value in the future design of mobile alarms that could be appropriate for users with different preferences and wake-up needs.
This paper aims to deepen our understanding of wake-up tasks, most distinguished from the conventional alarm-dismiss method (i.e., touching), by analyzing four wake-up tasks in the Alarmy app: (1) touching a button ( n o r m a l _ t a s k ), (2) taking a picture ( p i c t u r e _ t a s k ), (3) shaking the device ( s h a k e _ t a s k ), and (4) solving math problems ( m a t h _ t a s k ). Specifically, we addressed two research questions:
RQ 1. How does users’ alarm usage differ depending on the wake-up task?
RQ 2. How do hard-taskers, who use a higher proportion of the hard tasks with task load over a certain level, use task alarms, and in what contexts?
Regarding the first research question, we anticipated that the level of task loads will cause differences in alarm-set usage and alarm-dismiss usage. For example, it was expected that more difficult tasks are mostly used in earlier hours.
Hypothesis 1a (H1a).
The frequency of the alarm-set will be different among the four wake-up tasks.
Hypothesis 1b (H1b).
The time of the alarm-set will be different among the four wake-up tasks.
Hypothesis 1c (H1c).
The consistency of the alarm-set will be different among the four wake-up tasks.
Hypothesis 2a (H2a).
The time duration of the alarm-dismiss will be different among the four wake-up tasks.
Hypothesis 2b (H2b).
The use of snoozing will be different among the four wake-up tasks.
Next, for the second research question, it was expected that there will be alarm app usage factors for predicting the proportion of the individual’s hard-task uses. The following hypothesis aims to test that.
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
The particular alarm app usage will affect the proportion of hard-task uses.
The remainder of this paper is organized as follows. We first review the literature on human awakening and inconvenient interaction. We then explain the methodology to answer the two research questions and present the analysis results. Finally, we discuss the implications, including limitations of this study, and present the conclusions.

2. Literature Review

2.1. Task Capability Immediately after Awakening

Prior studies have examined task capability shortly after awakening [15]. Jeanneret and Webb [16] evaluated grip strength by squeezing a dynamometer after awakening compared to the normal state. Wilkinson and Stretton [17] carried out performance experiments after awakening at different times in the night. They evaluated several performance metrics such as adding numbers, reaction response to a specific sound tone, and coordinating a ball at a point. Åkerstedt and Gillberg [18] performed memory experiments where subjects learned four playing cards and were tested for retention of the previous cards.
Such studies could be useful references to design the wake-up tasks by specifying the task load, but these were mainly conducted in limited experimental settings (i.e., laboratories). For our first research question, we analyzed task performance based on alarm usage logs collected from an extensive number of real users. Therefore, quantitative results in this study can better clarify the characteristics of a different wake-up task, and this can lead us to practical implications for designing a task-based mobile alarm app.

2.2. Uncomfortable and Inconvenient Interaction

Ease of use has been a general principle of interactive system design in HCI [19]. This principle is to increase efficiency and convenience so that users do not have to struggle to achieve better results. However, Gilmore [20] addressed the question about the situation where guidelines and principles of interface design are applied uniformly to every information technology. Benford et al. [21] argued in favor of unconventional interaction design, which they called uncomfortable interaction. This tool is important to understand cultural experiences related to entertainment, enlightenment, and sociality. They investigated and reported multiple cases based on the uncomfortable system. For example, roller coasters and sports such as bungee jumping can cause physical discomfort such as tiredness and tension. However, people are pleased to accept this stress to feel the thrill and entertainment.
Rekimoto and Tsujita [22] proposed inconvenient interaction, which requires the user’s participation and effort to attain a long-term benefit with the short-term inconvenience. For example, they showed a microwave that requires the user to perform a step-aerobics exercise during operation. Another example of the inconvenient system was a refrigerator that contains a smile-awareness sensor so that the user has to smile if they want to open it [23]. These examples were based on the premise that laughing can help people to maintain long-term benefits of health. The Communication-Grill, for example, looks like an ordinary grill except that it cooks and keeps the meat warm only while people converse around it. This product encourages people to maintain a conversation with a positive effect.
Awakening from sleep is generally a difficult but indispensable behavior. In particular, people often need to get up at a challenging time such as the early morning hours, and that is why they use a traditional wake-up alarm. Needless to say, wake-up tasks impose inconvenience to users, so users have to increase their efforts to perform wake-up tasks. In this regard, using wake-up tasks can be understood as an empirical case for the principles of inconvenient interaction.
In this study, we examined hard-taskers who like to use such inconvenience to answer the second research question. The hard-tasker group is important and needs to be understood because the wake-up task, which causes task loads over a certain level, is the most distinguishing feature of the task-based alarm app. Based on these understandings, it would be possible to recommend a suitable wake-up task for a particular user and to manage the level of inconvenience.

3. Methodology

3.1. Alarmy Usage Data

To answer the research questions, we analyzed Alarmy usage logs. Alarmy is a task-based mobile app we developed in 2013 which currently provides four different wake-up tasks that involve various levels of physical or mental efforts: (1) touching a button ( n o r m a l _ t a s k ), (2) taking a picture ( p i c t u r e _ t a s k ), (3) shaking the device ( s h a k e _ t a s k ), and (4) solving math problems ( m a t h _ t a s k ). The following sections introduce the Alarmy app and describe the dataset for the analysis.

3.1.1. Alarmy App

In this work, we analyzed A l a r m y , a wake-up task-based mobile alarm app. We deployed Alarmy to the public via Google Play and the App Store. After a few years, the app became the most downloaded alarm clock app in 97 countries, and the number of its daily active users (DAU) is about three million.
Alarmy provides alarms with wake-up tasks, and the user is forced to perform a set task to turn off an alarm. This section explains how Alarmy users set and dismiss alarms with wake-up tasks. The whole alarm usage process in Alarmy is as shown in Figure 1.
Alarmy consists of four tab menus: (1) Alarm, (2) History, (3) Today’s Panel, and (4) Settings. First, the Alarm tab shows a list of alarms by displaying the set time and the task type for each. The user can add a new alarm by touching the button at the bottom of the screen. When touching one of the alarms in the list, the user can see and modify detailed settings. Additionally, this tab enables the user to set a timer called Quick Alarm. Secondly, the History tab visualizes the user’s previous alarm usage information such as alarm set time and the task type over time. Thirdly, the Today’s Panel tab contains the day’s weather, horoscope, and news. Finally, the Settings tab enables users to set details for the Alarmy app use, such as changing app theme, language, and so on.
To set the alarm, the user must specify detailed information. First, they must set when the alarm goes off. Further, the alarms can recur once the user has selected days of the week for recurrence (e.g., weekday or weekend). Next, the user can choose ringtone volume, type (e.g., sound or vibration), and sound (e.g., noisy sound or music in the device). Finally, the user must select one of five tasks for the alarm: (1) touching a button (conventional method), (2) taking a picture of a particular place, (3) shaking the device, (4) solving math problems of addition and multiplication, and (5) scanning a QR code.
These tasks certainly aggravate users by enforcing cognitive and physical efforts. Even so, our usage logs show that many users still conduct such tasks because they want to get up at the right time. In this paper, we refer to mobile alarm app tasks for waking people up wake-up tasks. We labeled each wake-up task as such: (1) touching a button ( n o r m a l _ t a s k ), (2) taking a picture ( p i c t u r e _ t a s k ), (3) shaking the device ( s h a k e _ t a s k ), and (4) solving math problems ( m a t h _ t a s k ). In this study, we excluded the scanning a QR code method because the number of users selecting the scanning a QR code method is very low, and the procedure for this task is quite similar to that for the p i c t u r e _ t a s k .
Touching a button is the conventional method for alarm dismissal, and its task load is relatively low. However, other wake-up tasks require relatively high physical or cognitive efforts, in which users must specify the detailed tasks in advance, called hard tasks. For example, the user can specify the difficulty of the math problems or the number of shakes. For the tasks of taking a picture and scanning a QR code, the user must register an image as a reference when setting the alarm.
Once an alarm goes off, the user must decide whether to delay the alarm (snooze) or perform the wake-up task. If the user decides to snooze, the alarm temporarily turns off, but it re-rings after the time the user previously specified. Otherwise, the user must perform a specific task to dismiss the alarm.

3.1.2. Dataset Overview

Table 1 shows the description of alarm usage variables and the statistics that we measured for each individual user. We collected Alarmy usage data from the real users who voluntarily downloaded and installed the app in the US from January to May 2018 (151 days). The collected usage log includes anonymized user ID, the time when the alarm was set, the time when the alarm was set, the details of the alarm-set (e.g., wake-up task type, set to recur), the time when the alarm was dismissed, and the number of snooze uses. The consent required for data collection from these users was exempt by the approval from the Hanyang Institutional Review Board (IRB No.: HYU-2019-06-010-1).
During the study period, a total of 581,712 users left 48,763,070 usage logs. To exclude trial users, we selected 211,273 Alarmy users who had used the app for more than 30 days and their 42,909,263 app usage logs. They were 38% of the total users, but their usage logs cover 88% of the entire dataset. From the usage logs, we measured alarm usage behaviors in two folds: alarm-set and alarm-dismiss. The alarm-set usage contains eight variables in three subcategories: (1) set frequency, (2) set time, and (3) set consistency. Alarm-dismiss usage includes four variables from the two subcategories, (1) dismiss time and (2) snooze.
The average number of the days that the users set the alarm was 74 days, and the users usually set 1.68 alarms per day. The alarms mostly occurred in the early morning hours (7.43 h) on weekdays (84%). Further, most of the alarms were set to recur (77%). The most common task was n o r m a l _ t a s k s (71%), followed by m a t h _ t a s k s (15%), s h a k e _ t a s k s (10%), and p i c t u r e _ t a s k s (4%). On average, it took about 19 s to dismiss an alarm (Normal: 14.1 s; Picture: 40.06 s; Shake: 26.62 s; Math: 40.81 s).

3.2. Analysis Methods

3.2.1. Comparing Alarm Usage among Wake-Up Tasks (RQ1)

To compare the alarm usage among the four wake-up tasks, each user’s usage variables were aggregated for each task. To check the existence of statistically significant differences in alarm usage among the wake-up task, we conducted a Kruskal–Wallis H test, a non-parametric hypothesis test for a multigroup comparison. By considering the large size of each group, we also calculated the effect sizes to evaluate the magnitude of the differences [24]. We considered ϵ 2 , the effect size for the Kruskal–Wallis H test. A prior study [25] suggested that ϵ 2 = 0.01 be considered a small effect size, 0.04 represents a moderate effect size, 0.16 a relatively strong effect size, and 0.46 a strong effect size.
In cases where the comparison test shows a moderate or strong effect size under the prior studies’ guideline [24], we conducted the Dunn test as a post-hoc test to identify the detailed relationship between the two tasks and calculated Cohen’s d as the effect size, which represent the magnitude of the mean difference. Cohen [26] provided rules of thumb for interpreting d, suggesting that an d of | 0.2 | represents a small size, | 0.5 | represents a medium size, and | 0.8 | represents a large effect size. We used 0.001 as the level of significance for all the hypothesis tests and conducted a Bonferroni correction method for the post-hoc tests.

3.2.2. Modeling Hard-Taskers’ Usage Patterns (RQ2)

For the second research question, we adopted a statistical modeling approach to explore relevant usage patterns of hard-taskers, who show a relatively high proportion of non-normal tasks with cognitive and physical task loads (i.e., m a t h _ t a s k , s h a k e _ t a s k , and p i c t u r e _ t a s k ). Specifically, we conducted a multiple linear regression analysis, which builds a linear model of predicting the proportion of the hard task uses ( p r o p _ h a r d _ t a s k ) based on the alarm-set and alarm-dismiss usage variables of each user. We used the above twelve independent usage variables, including eight alarm-set variables and four alarm-dismiss variables from Table 1.
For this analysis, all the variables were measured for each of the 211,273 users. We found several independent variables that were correlated. For example, the two highest correlations were observed between a v g _ d a i l y _ a l a r m and s d _ d a i l y _ a l a r m , and between a v g _ f r e q _ s n o o z e and s d _ f r e q _ s n o o z e . We excluded a v g _ d a i l y _ a l a r m and s d _ f r e q _ s n o o z e to make all the pairs of the independent variables have a Pearson correlation value less than 0.7, and the variance inflation factor (VIF) for each independent variable was lower than the benchmark of 10 for multicollinearity [27]. The highest value was 1.757. Therefore, our regression models were safe from the multicollinearity effect.

4. Results

4.1. Different Alarm App Usage among Wake-Up Tasks (RQ1)

Table 2 and Table 3 show the task-wise comparison results by the Kruskal–Wallis H test, and Figure 2 and Figure 3 represent the magnitude of the mean differences among the wake-up tasks.

4.1.1. Alarm-Set Usage

Set Frequency. There are three variables in the set frequency, f r e q _ u s e _ d a y s , p r o p _ u s e _ d a y s , and a v g _ d a i l y _ a l a r m . First, the number of days of setting each wake-up task ( f r e q _ u s e _ d a y s ) was compared via the Kruskal–Wallis H test, The results show significant and moderate differences among the wake-up tasks [ H ( 3 ) = 10,884.67 , p < 0.001 , ϵ 2 = 0.036 ]. Post-hoc comparisons using the Dunn test also found significant differences in most of the pairs of the wake-up tasks, but their magnitudes of the difference differed depending on the task pair. N o r m a l _ t a s k was used 54 days on average, and this is much greater than those of others (vs. p i c t u r e _ t a s k : d = 0.491 , vs. s h a k e _ t a s k : d = 0.471 , vs. m a t h _ t a s k : d = 0.252 ). m a t h _ t a s k tended to have a moderately lower number of days than p i c t u r e _ t a s k ( d = 0.251 ) and m a t h _ t a s k ( d = 0.226 ). However, the difference in the frequencies of p i c t u r e _ t a s k uses and s h a k e _ t a s k uses was not significant and not considerable ( p = 0.278 , d = 0.023 ).
Next, there is a significant and moderate difference in the proportion of setting each wake-up task ( p r o p _ u s e _ d a y s ) [ H ( 3 ) = 15,704.64 , p < 0.001 , ϵ 2 = 0.052 ], and all the post-test results are also statistically significant. The effect sizes of the post-tests show the detailed relationship between the wake-up tasks. The proportion of days with n o r m a l _ t a s k is much greater than those of others (vs. p i c t u r e _ t a s k : d = 0.553 , vs. s h a k e _ t a s k : d = 0.585 , vs. m a t h _ t a s k : d = 0.337 ). s h a k e _ t a s k and p i c t u r e _ t a s k showed a similar number of uses ( d = 0.032 ), and m a t h _ t a s k was the least used (vs. s h a k e _ t a s k : d = 0.254 and vs. p i c t u r e _ t a s k : d = 0.222 ).
We also found a significant and moderate difference in the number of set alarms in a day ( a v g _ d a i l y _ a l a r m , multiple alarm-sets in a day) [ H ( 3 ) = 13,852.05 , p < 0.001 , ϵ 2 = 0.046 ]. What all tasks had in common was that users tended to set a single alarm for a day ( n o r m a l _ t a s k : 1.55, p i c t u r e _ t a s k : 1.16, s h a k e _ t a s k : 1.34, m a t h _ t a s k : 1.36). However, the effect sizes showed a large difference between particular task pairs. For example, the number of n o r m a l _ t a s k alarms tended to be greater than those of the other tasks, and its effect size also showed that the magnitude of their differences was considerably large (vs. p i c t u r e _ t a s k : d = 0.624 , vs. s h a k e _ t a s k : d = 0.351 , vs. m a t h _ t a s k : d = 0.314 ). Similarly to the p r o p _ u s e _ d a y s , the effect size of the difference between s h a k e _ t a s k and m a t h _ t a s k was relatively small ( d = 0.047 ), and they were frequently used in a day more than p i c t u r e _ t a s k (vs. s h a k e _ t a s k : d = 0.391 and vs. m a t h _ t a s k : d = 0.425 ).
Set Time. The set time category includes a v g _ r i n g _ h o u r and p r o p _ w e e k d a y . First, the Kruskal–Wallis H test on the set-alarm hours ( a v g _ r i n g _ h o u r ) reveals a significant difference among the tasks, but its effect size was small, indicating that most of the alarms were concentrated in the morning hours [ H ( 3 ) = 2558.71 , p < 0.001 , ϵ 2 = 0.008 ]. Even though the magnitude of the difference is not that large, we could find a tendency of the task preference in the set-time. n o r m a l _ t a s k ’s average hours (about 8 a.m.) tended to be slightly later than those of the other tasks (vs. p i c t u r e _ t a s k : d = 0.281 , vs. s h a k e _ t a s k : d = 0.119 , vs. m a t h _ t a s k : d = 0.181 ). Interestingly, hard tasks (i.e., p i c t u r e _ t a s k , s h a k e _ t a s k , and m a t h _ t a s k ) tended to be more concentrated at the earlier hours than n o r m a l _ t a s k .
Similarly, we also found a small effect size in the comparison of the weekday use proportion ( p r o p _ w e e k d a y ), indicating that most of the set alarms were concentrated on weekdays regardless of the wake-up task types [ H ( 3 ) = 1828.93 , p < 0.001 , ϵ 2 = 0.006 ]. Approximately 82–85% of the alarms went off on weekdays, and such a tendency was not entirely different depending on the task type.
Set Consistency. We compared three variables, s d _ d a i l y _ a l a r m , s d _ r i n g _ h o u r , and p r o p _ r e c u r , in terms of the set consistency. First, we found a significant difference in the variability of the number of a daily alarm-set ( s d _ d a i l y _ a l a r m ) and its moderate effect size [ H ( 3 ) = 12,485.94 , p < 0.001 , ϵ 2 = 0.041 ]. n o r m a l _ t a s k tended to be more inconsistent in the number of the daily set alarms (vs. p i c t u r e _ t a s k : d = 0.671 , vs. s h a k e _ t a s k : d = 0.354 , vs. m a t h _ t a s k : d = 0.302 ). p i c t u r e _ t a s k showed strong consistency in the number of the daily set alarms compared to the other tasks (vs. s h a k e _ t a s k : d = 0.371 and vs. m a t h _ t a s k : d = 0.428 ).
Similarly, there were significant and moderate differences in the variability of the ringing hours ( s d _ r i n g _ h o u r ) [ H ( 3 ) = 11,731.86 , p < 0.001 , ϵ 2 = 0.039 ]. It was found that the users set n o r m a l _ t a s k ’s ringing hour more inconsistently than the other tasks (vs. p i c t u r e _ t a s k : d = 0.482 , vs. s h a k e _ t a s k : d = 0.332 , vs. m a t h _ t a s k : d = 0.349 ).
Finally, we compared the proportion of weekly-recurrent alarms over wake-up tasks. The results showed a small effect size, indicating that most of the alarms were set to recur weekly regardless of the task type [ H ( 3 ) = 2736.596 , p < 0.001 , ϵ 2 = 0.009 ]. Approximately 73–76% of the alarms were set to recur.

4.1.2. Alarm-Dismiss Usage

Task Completion Time. We considered a v g _ t a s k _ c o m p l e t i o n _ t i m e and s d _ t a s k _ c o m p l e t i o n _ t i m e in the usage category of the task completion time. First, our Kruskal–Wallis H test results on the a v g _ t a s k _ c o m p l e t i o n _ t i m e revealed significant and strong differences among wake-up tasks [ H ( 3 ) = 14,4454.19 , p < 0.001 , ϵ 2 = 0.683 ]. The task completion time of n o r m a l _ t a s k was approximately 14 s on average, while those of p i c t u r e _ t a s k ( M e a n = 40.05 , S D = 15.63 ) and m a t h _ t a s k ( M e a n = 40.81 , S D = 14.67 ) were the longest. s h a k e _ t a s k ’s completion time was approximately 26 s.
There is also significant and moderate difference in the variability of the task completion time among the tasks ( s d _ t a s k _ c o m p l e t i o n _ t i m e ) [ H ( 3 ) = 14,281.84 , p < 0.001 , ϵ 2 = 0.067 ]. n o r m a l _ t a s k tended to be more consistent in the task completion time compared to the other tasks (vs. p i c t u r e _ t a s k : d = 0.594 , vs. s h a k e _ t a s k : d = 0.163 , vs. m a t h _ t a s k : d = 0.461 ). In the variability of the completion time, the magnitude of the difference between m a t h _ t a s k and p i c t u r e _ t a s k was not great ( d = 0.156 ).
Snooze. Regarding snooze use, a v g _ f r e q _ s n o o z e and s d _ f r e q _ s n o o z e were compared among the wake-up tasks. We found significant difference in the number of snoozes ( a v g _ f r e q _ s n o o z e ), but its magnitude was small [ H ( 3 ) = 277.76 , p < 0.001 , ϵ 2 = 0.001 ]. p i c t u r e _ t a s k has the largest number of snooze use per alarm ( M e a n = 0.332 and S D = 0.507 ) among the wake-up tasks. However, the standard deviation of snooze use with the p i c t u r e _ t a s k was too large, so the Kruskal–Wallis H test accepted the null hypothesis. Next, we found that there was a significant but weak difference in the consistency of snooze use ( s d _ f r e q _ s n o o z e ) [ H ( 3 ) = 16.18 , p = 0.001 , ϵ 2 < 0.001 ]. The standard deviation of the snooze use per alarm was not considerably different among the wake-up tasks.

4.2. Alarm Usage Factors for Predicting the Proportion of the Hard-Task Use (RQ2)

Table 4 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis. The regression model was statistically significant ( F ( 10,22,110 ) = 22,110 , p < 0.001 ) and explained the proportion of using hard tasks reasonably well ( R 2 of 0.511).
The results reveal significant usage patterns to understand hard-taskers. First, our regression analysis results reveal that hard-taskers experienced challenging situations such as awakening at an earlier time relatively less. The proportion of hard-task uses tends to be high when a v g _ r i n g _ h o u r was late, and the proportion of weekday use was low. Second, hard-taskers showed irregular usage behaviors, which possibly short-circuit habit-formation. Their number of daily alarms varied much (multiple alarms in a day), and they consistently delayed waking up by dismissing an alarm using the snooze feature. Additionally, hard-taskers’ alarm uses tended to be sparse over a longer period (positively correlated with f r e q _ u s e _ d a y s and negatively correlated with p r o p _ u s e _ d a y s ). Finally, hard-taskers tended to show a longer task completion time. This is obvious because they are the users who use hard tasks relatively often, which take a longer time to dismiss.

5. Discussion

5.1. Characteristics of Wake-Up Tasks

n o r m a l _ t a s k is the most casual task type. n o r m a l _ t a s k is the easiest and causes the lowest task load. The task completion time of n o r m a l _ t a s k was the shortest (14 s), and also the number of the snooze uses was the lowest (0.155 times) among the tasks. Such a low level of the task load possibly leads users to use n o r m a l _ t a s k frequently and casually in diverse situations. Almost half of the total alarm-sets were n o r m a l _ t a s k (48%), and the average number of days was the greatest among the wake-up tasks (54 days). Further, n o r m a l _ t a s k tended to be used for alarms in the late morning hours (7.72—equivalent to 7:43 a.m.), which are likely less challenging. Finally, the consistency of using n o r m a l _ t a s k tended to be lower than that of the other tasks in terms of both set and dismiss usage, possibly because of its frequent uses in diverse contexts.
p i c t u r e _ t a s k is the hardest task and mainly used for challenging situations. Dismissing p i c t u r e _ t a s k tends to take a longer time (40 s), and the number of snooze uses was the greatest, indicating that p i c t u r e _ t a s k is the most challenging task to be completed among the wake-up tasks. The higher level of the difficulty probably causes users to usually set p i c t u r e _ t a s k for challenging contexts. The average set-hour of p i c t u r e _ t a s k was the earliest (6.88 h), and its standard deviation was the lowest. Moreover, the overall frequency of the p i c t u r e _ t a s k use was the lowest. p i c t u r e _ t a s k is more valuable and useful when the user is desperate to awaken on time, especially in the earlier morning.
m a t h _ t a s k is also a difficult type, but its psychological barrier seems lower than that of p i c t u r e _ t a s k . Similar to p i c t u r e _ t a s k , m a t h _ t a s k takes a longer time to complete (40 s). However, the frequency of the m a t h _ t a s k use was greater than that of the p i c t u r e _ t a s k use by almost five times. Further, its number of snooze uses was much lower than that of p i c t u r e _ t a s k . Such differences between m a t h _ t a s k and p i c t u r e _ t a s k occur possibly because these tasks cause different types of task load. p i c t u r e _ t a s k causes a physical load, while m a t h _ t a s k a cognitive load. According to the results, it seems that users feel the burden of physical tasks more than that of cognitive tasks, even though the actual completion time of the tasks is the same.
s h a k e _ t a s k shows moderate usage tendencies in both alarm-set and alarm-dismiss. In alarm-dismiss, s h a k e _ t a s k showed a longer task completion time, and the higher number of the snooze uses compared to n o r a m l _ t a s k . However, its difficulty level did not reach that of the other two hard tasks, i.e., p i c t u r e _ t a s k and m a t h _ t a s k . On the other hand, the alarm-set variables of s h a k e _ t a s k tended to be similar to those of m a t h _ t a s k , except for the set frequency, even though s h a k e _ t a s k causes a physical load, unlike m a t h _ t a s k .

5.2. Physical Tasks vs. Cognitive Tasks

This study found the characteristics of the wake-up tasks by analyzing dismissal behaviors. The task of touching a button tends to require the shortest time to complete, while the tasks of taking a picture and solving math problems showed relatively long task completion times. Next, users tended to snooze more often when the wake-up task was set as taking a picture. Solving math problems had a significantly lower number of snoozes than taking a picture, which showed a similar task completion time. Due to the greater number of snoozes, taking a picture showed the longest time to dismiss from the first ring; this was followed by solving math problems, then shaking a device, and finally touching a button. Our results show that we should consider cognitive and physical loads of wake-up tasks in their design.
Though taking a picture and solving math problems showed similar task completion times, we could find significant differences in the number of snoozes and the frequency of setting the task. The task of solving math problems mainly causes a cognitive load, while taking a picture requires physical effort. The results may indicate that users tend to experience more psychological pressure from physical tasks than cognitive tasks. Requiring a physical load could lead to better performance in waking the user from sleep. However, this kind of task should be carefully designed because users would avoid using this task due to the pressure it causes. Therefore, recommending cognitive tasks can be relatively more effective in user retention, especially for those who are sensitive to psychological pressures.
As the design guideline from Rekimoto and Tsujita’s inconvenient interaction [22] suggests, the difficulty of inconvenience (i.e., physical or cognitive tasks) needs to be well balanced with the necessity (i.e., waking up on time), or else it may lead the user to abandon the use. Further, Kim et al.’s study [6] suggests that users engage in a cost–benefit analysis when given a mobile task (i.e., wake-up task) with a certain level of difficulty and weigh whether the outcome of the task (i.e., waking up) is worth not ignoring (i.e., snoozing) it. Therefore, our work complements prior studies by characterizing the mobile alarm tasks with physical and cognitive loads for future wake-up app design support.

5.3. Wake-Up Tasks and Fogg’s Motivation–Ability–Prompt (MAP) Model

The study results are well linked with Fogg’s MAP behavior model [14], which explains human behavior through three elements: (1) motivation, (2) ability, and (3) prompts, as shown in Figure 4. In this model, if at least one of the three elements is missing, the anticipated behavior is rarely triggered. This model can thoroughly explain mobile alarm users’ usage behavior. A user’s motivation can vary depending on the next morning’s schedule or their willingness to build the habit of waking up early. Furthermore, the ability in MAP corresponds to how much the user is confident in waking up on time. According to the MAP theory, the activation threshold for awakening on time can basically be determined by motivation and ability. Additionally, the wake-up tasks in the mobile alarm app can be seen as prompts.
For example, in the context when a user’s motivation is low, and their ability is high, the wake-up tasks do not need to be that hard. The study results show that most users frequently used the easiest task ( n o r m a l _ t a s k ) and mostly set it to recur. This probably indicates that motivation is usually lower than ability for most users. Even the easiest task can trigger the targeted behavior (awakening on time) because it may not be that important. However, the regression analysis results reveal that hard-taskers usually seem less pressured to wake up on time (e.g., late and irregular alarm hour, many snoozes, and relatively less use in weekdays), so they do not need to build the habit of waking up early (i.e., the ability is relatively low). However, probably, they occasionally face situations that require them to get up early (i.e., the motivation becomes higher), and the hard tasks become more valuable at this time.
Developing the ability requires time and effort, so diverse wake-up tasks in mobile alarm app scan be useful to deal with different activation levels depending on contextual motivation and individual ability. One further idea would be a wake-up task recommendation based on the user’s schedule. For example, a mobile alarm app can be connected with a calendar app and encourage the user to set a challenging wake-up task only if there is an important event early in the next morning. Such flexible interaction can reduce the overall pressure or burden on the app user and convince them to continue using the app.

5.4. Limitations

In this current study, we were not able to include a user profile for a detailed analysis (e.g., an individual’s level of motivation of use; the sensitivity to the alarm). Additionally, the detailed context of use was not available for analysis. For example, in the case of p i c t u r e _ t a s k , the target picture may be just near the bed, or even outside of the house, which could greatly influence usage behavior. Despite such limitations, the large dataset allowed us to observe a generalized behavior that partly justifies the extreme outliers. It would be interesting to conduct a qualitative study with a smaller number of datasets to uncover the hidden motivations behind the various wake-up tasks for future work.

6. Conclusions

This paper aimed to improve task-based mobile alarm app design by understanding the wake-up task uses in the Alarmy app. The results of this study reveal the prevalent use of the n o r m a l _ t a s k , the earlier and regular use of the p i c t u r e _ t a s k , and the longest task completion time of the m a t h _ t a s k . Further, our regression analysis results showed that hard-taskers experienced challenging situations such as awakening at an earlier time relatively less and mostly showed irregular behaviors such as a high number of snooze uses and an inconsistent number of alarms in a day, which possibly short-circuit habit-formation.
In future work, it will be interesting to design wake-up tasks for mobile crowdsourcing [28,29]. Our study results showed the potential of wake-up tasks for crowdsourcing. For example, we found that there are many users who willingly and consistently conduct cognitive or physical tasks in order to get out of bed. Moreover, task completion tended to be longer (50–70 s) than that in prior work, such as utilizing smartphone unlock moments (1–3 s) [30,31].
Furthermore, we believe that our study can be a pioneer in the HCI field of behavior change related to status right after awakening and encourage branching out in further studies. This study presents empirical cases under the principles of inconvenient interaction. HCI has mostly focused on increasing effectiveness in task performance, but like task-based alarms, users still decide to accept inconvenience in some domains. We believe that understanding such intended inconvenient experiences will expand the HCI research area.

Author Contributions

Methodology, M.K. and J.K.; software, J.S. and K.T.O.; validation, K.T.O. and J.K.; investigation, K.T.O. and M.K.; resources, J.S.; data curation, J.S.; writing—original draft preparation, K.T.O. and M.K.; writing—review and editing, J.K.; visualization, K.T.O.; supervision, M.K. and J.K.; project administration, M.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported in part by the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Korea and the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2017S1A5B8058870) and by a grant (19CTAP-C152247-01) from the Technology Advancement Research Program funded by the Ministry of Land, Infrastructure and Transport of the Korean government.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Kurniawan, S.; Mahmud, M.; Nugroho, Y. A Study of the Use of Mobile Phones by Older Persons. In Proceedings of the Extended Abstracts of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Montreal, QC, Canada, 22–27 April 2006; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 989–994. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ong, A.A.; Gillespie, M.B. Overview of Smartphone Applications for Sleep Analysis. World J. Otorhinolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2016, 2, 45–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Park, M.H.; Lee, S.T. A Study on User Needs Cognition on Functions of Mobile Phone-Focused on Use Frequency and Necessity on Functions of Age Group. In Proceedings of the HCI Korea, Pyeongchang-gun, Korea, 13–15 February 2008; The HCI Society of Korea: Seoul, Korea, 2008; pp. 465–469. [Google Scholar]
  4. Kasim, S.; Hafit, H.; Leong, T.H.; Hashim, R.; Ruslai, H.; Jahidin, K.; Arshad, M.S. SRC: Smart Reminder Clock. In IOP Conference Series: Materials Science and Engineering; IOP Publishing: Bristol, UK, 2016; Volume 160, p. 012101. [Google Scholar]
  5. Yeh, Y.H.; Lu, D.H.; Hung, J.C. Combining Fuzzy Systems and Social Networking Sites Design to Alarm Clocks Using the Android System. In Proceedings of the International Symposium on Computer, Consumer and Control, Taichung, Taiwan, 4–6 June 2012; IEEE: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2012; pp. 28–31. [Google Scholar]
  6. Kim, J.; Park, J.; Lee, H.; Ko, M.; Lee, U. LocknType: Lockout Task Intervention for Discouraging Smartphone App Use. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Glasgow, UK, 4–9 May 2019; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2019; pp. 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bloch, V. Level of Wakefulness and Attention. In Experimental Psychology; University Press of France: Paris, France, 1966; pp. 97–146. [Google Scholar]
  8. Lazareva, V.V.; Svederskaya, N.E.; Khomskaya, E.D. Electrical Activity of Brain During Mental Workload. In Neuropsychological Mechanisms of Attention; Science Publisher: Moscow, Russia, 1979; pp. 151–168. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bedny, G.Z.; Karwowski, W.; Bedny, I.S. Complexity Evaluation of Computer-Based Tasks. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 2012, 28, 236–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bedny, G.Z.; Karwowski, W. A Systemic-Structural Activity Approach to Design of Human–Computer Interaction Tasks. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 2003, 16, 235–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Choe, E.K.; Lee, B.; Kay, M.; Pratt, W.; Kientz, J.A. SleepTight: Low-Burden, Self-Monitoring Technology for Capturing and Reflecting on Sleep Behaviors. In Proceedings of the ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing, Osaka, Japan, 7–11 September 2015; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 121–132. [Google Scholar]
  12. Min, J.K.; Doryab, A.; Wiese, J.; Amini, S.; Zimmerman, J.; Hong, J.I. Toss’n’turn: Smartphone as Sleep and Sleep Quality Detector. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Toronto, ON, Canada, 26 April–1 May 2014; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 477–486. [Google Scholar]
  13. Ravichandran, R.; Sien, S.W.; Patel, S.N.; Kientz, J.A.; Pina, L.R. Making Sense of Sleep Sensors: How Sleep Sensing Technologies Support and Undermine Sleep Health. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Denver, CO, USA, 6–11 May 2017; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 6864–6875. [Google Scholar]
  14. Fogg, B.J. A Behavior Model for Persuasive Design. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Persuasive Technology, Claremont, CA, USA, 26–29 April 2009; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2009; p. 40. [Google Scholar]
  15. Dinges, D.F. Are You Awake? Cognitive Performance and Reverie during the Hypnopompic State. In Sleep and Cognition; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1990; pp. 159–175. [Google Scholar]
  16. Jeanneret, P.R.; Webb, W.B. Strength of Grip on Arousal from Full Night’s Sleep. Percept. Mot. Skills 1963, 17, 759–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Wilkinson, R.T.; Stretton, M. Performance after Awakening at Different Times of Night. Psychon. Sci. 1971, 23, 283–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. ÅKerstedt, T.; Gillberg, M. Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Memory and Sleep Latencies in Connection with Repeated Awakenings from Sleep. Psychophysiology 1979, 16, 49–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Ahtinen, A. Wellness Applications-UI Design to Support Long-Term Usage Motivation. In Proceedings of the Extended Abstracts of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Florence, Italy, 5–10 April 2008; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 2669–2672. [Google Scholar]
  20. Gilmore, D.J. Interface Design: Have we got it wrong? In Human—Computer Interaction; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1995; pp. 173–178. [Google Scholar]
  21. Benford, S.; Greenhalgh, C.; Giannachi, G.; Walker, B.; Marshall, J.; Rodden, T. Uncomfortable Interactions. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Austin, TX, USA, 5–10 May 2012; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 2005–2014. [Google Scholar]
  22. Rekimoto, J.; Tsujita, H. Inconvenient Interactions: An Alternative Interaction Design Approach to Enrich Our Daily Activities. In Proceedings of the International Working Conference on Advanced Visual Interfaces, Como, Italy, 27–30 May 2014; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 225–228. [Google Scholar]
  23. Tsujita, H.; Rekimoto, J. Smiling Makes Us Happier: Enhancing Positive Mood and Communication with Smile-Encouraging Digital Appliances. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Ubiquitous Computing, Beijing, China, 17–21 September 2011; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 1–10. [Google Scholar]
  24. Becker, L.A. Effect size (ES). Retrieved Sept. 2000, 9, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  25. Rea, L.M.; Parker, R.A. Designing and Conducting Survey Research: A Comprehensive Guide; Jossey-Bass Publishers: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  26. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Routledge Academic: New York, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  27. Hair, J.F.; Tatham, R.L.; Anderson, R.E.; Black, W. Multivariate Data Analysis, 5th ed.; Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  28. Kandappu, T.; Jaiman, N.; Tandriansyah, R.; Misra, A.; Cheng, S.F.; Chen, C.; Lau, H.C.; Chander, D.; Dasgupta, K. TASKer: Behavioral Insights via Campus-Based Experimental Mobile Crowd-Sourcing. In Proceedings of the ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing, Heidelberg, Germany, 12–16 September 2016; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 392–402. [Google Scholar]
  29. Truong, K.N.; Shihipar, T.; Wigdor, D.J. Slide to X: Unlocking the Potential of Smartphone Unlocking. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Toronto, ON, Canada, 26 April–1 May 2014; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 3635–3644. [Google Scholar]
  30. Vaish, R.; Wyngarden, K.; Chen, J.; Cheung, B.; Bernstein, M.S. Twitch Crowdsourcing: Crowd Contributions in Short Bursts of Time. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Toronto, ON, Canada, 26 April–1 May 2014; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 3645–3654. [Google Scholar]
  31. Cheng, J.; Teevan, J.; Iqbal, S.T.; Bernstein, M.S. Break It Down: A Comparison of Macro-And Microtasks. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Seoul, Korea, 18–23 April 2015; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 4061–4064. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Setting and dismissing an alarm in the Alarmy app.
Figure 1. Setting and dismissing an alarm in the Alarmy app.
Applsci 10 03993 g001
Figure 2. The magnitude of task-wise mean differences on alarm set-usage: Cohen’s d (col, row).
Figure 2. The magnitude of task-wise mean differences on alarm set-usage: Cohen’s d (col, row).
Applsci 10 03993 g002
Figure 3. The magnitude of task-wise mean differences on alarm dismiss-usage: Cohen’s d (col, row).
Figure 3. The magnitude of task-wise mean differences on alarm dismiss-usage: Cohen’s d (col, row).
Applsci 10 03993 g003
Figure 4. Fogg’s MAP behavior model (motivation, ability, and prompt).
Figure 4. Fogg’s MAP behavior model (motivation, ability, and prompt).
Applsci 10 03993 g004
Table 1. Alarm usage variables.
Table 1. Alarm usage variables.
CategoryVariableDescriptionMeanSD
Set Frequency f r e q _ u s e _ d a y s The number of days using the alarm73.6730.47
p r o p _ u s e _ d a y s Proportion of the days using the alarm0.660.18
a v g _ d a i l y _ a l a r m Average number of alarms in a day1.680.69
Set Time a v g _ r i n g _ h o u r Average of ringing hours7.432.72
p r o p _ w e e k d a y Proportion of alarms set for weekdays0.840.11
Set Consistency s d _ d a i l y _ a l a r m SD of the number of alarms in a day0.6760.50
s d _ r i n g _ h o u r SD of ringing hours2.141.68
p r o p _ r e c u r % of the alarms set to recur weekly0.770.35
Dismiss Time a v g _ t a s k _ c o m p l e t i o n _ t i m e Average time to dismiss the alarm after it went off (seconds)19.5912.21
s d _ t a s k _ c o m p l e t i o n _ t i m e SD of the seconds to dismiss the alarm after it went off (seconds)13.865.88
Snooze a v g _ f r e q _ s n o o z e Average number of delays to dismiss an alarm0.180.28
s d _ f r e q _ s n o o z e SD of delays to dismiss an alarm0.300.27
Table 2. Alarm set-usage comparison among the wake-up tasks (Kruskal–Wallis H test).
Table 2. Alarm set-usage comparison among the wake-up tasks (Kruskal–Wallis H test).
VariableWake-Up Task: Mean (SD)Hp-Value ϵ 2
NormalPictureShakeMath
Set Frequency
f r e q _ u s e _ d a y s 54.38
(38.92)
2.53
(12.85)
9.05
(23.51)
13.68
(28.95)
10884.67<0.0010.036
p r o p _ u s e _ d a y s 0.49
(0.30)
0.02
(0.11)
0.08
(0.20)
0.12
(0.24)
15704.64<0.0010.052
a v g _ d a i l y _ a l a r m 1.56
(0.64)
1.17
(0.31)
1.34
(0.48)
1.37
(0.49)
13852.05<0.0010.046
Set Time
a v g _ r i n g _ h o u r 7.73
(3.02)
6.89
(2.65)
7.37
(2.89)
7.20
(2.62)
2558.71<0.0010.008
p r o p _ w e e k d a y 0.82
(0.17)
0.86
(0.17)
0.83
(0.19)
0.84
(0.16)
1828.93<0.0010.006
Set Consistency
s d _ d a i l y _ a l a r m 0.58
(0.48)
0.26
(0.32)
0.41
(0.43)
0.44
(0.43)
12485.94<0.0010.041
s d _ r i n g _ h o u r 2.16
(1.80)
1.30
(1.50)
1.57
(1.67)
1.55
(1.55)
11731.86<0.0010.039
p r o p _ r e c u r 0.73
(0.38)
0.76
(0.38)
0.74
(0.39)
0.77
(0.38)
2736.60<0.0010.009
Table 3. Alarm dismiss-usage comparison among the wake-up tasks (Kruskal–Wallis H test).
Table 3. Alarm dismiss-usage comparison among the wake-up tasks (Kruskal–Wallis H test).
VariableWake-Up Task: Mean (SD)Hp-Value ϵ 2
NormalPictureShakeMath
Dismiss Time Duration
a v g _ t a s k _ c o m p l e t i o n _ t i m e 14.10
(7.18)
40.06
(15.09)
26.62
(12.34)
40.81
(15.49)
144,454.19<0.0010.683
s d _ t a s k _ c o m p l e t i o n _ t i m e 11.86
(6.22)
15.63
(8.01)
12.89
(6.39)
14.67
(5.61)
14,281.84<0.0010.067
Snooze
a v g _ f r e q _ s n o o z e 0.16
(0.25)
0.33
(0.51)
0.20
(0.32)
0.19
(0.33)
277.76<0.0010.001
s d _ f r e q _ s n o o z e 0.27
(0.27)
0.30
(0.31)
0.28
(0.30)
0.28
(0.28)
16.18<0.0010.001
Table 4. Multiple linear regression for identifying hard-taskers.
Table 4. Multiple linear regression for identifying hard-taskers.
Variable Beta S.E. t P > | t | 95% CI Exp (Beta) VIF
LowerUpper
( c o n s t a n t )−0.1640.007−23.5800.000−0.178−0.151126.656
f r e q _ u s e _ d a y s 0.0040.00015.3020.0000.0000.0001.371
p r o p _ u s e _ d a y s −0.0400.004−9.350.000−0.049−0.0321.499
a v g _ r i n g _ h o u r 0.0050.00016.4220.0000.0040.0051.464
s d _ r i n g _ h o u r 0.0010.0000.4510.652−0.0010.0011.541
p r o p _ w e e k d a y −0.0410.006−6.7220.000−0.053−0.0291.170
s d _ d a i l y _ a l a r m 0.0650.00145.9270.000−0.0620.0671.269
p r o p _ r e c u r 0.0170.0028.8860.0000.0130.0211.158
a v g _ t a s k _ c o m p l e t i o n _ t i m e 0.0270.000401.4450.0000.0270.0271.745
s d _ t a s k _ c o m p l e t i o n _ t i m e −0.0110.000−76.4790.000−0.011−0.0101.757
a v g _ f r e q _ s n o o z e 0.0980.00242.7680.0000.0940.1031.041
F ( 10,211,262 ) = 22,110 ,   p < 0.001 ; R 2 = 0.511 .

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Oh, K.T.; Shin, J.; Kim, J.; Ko, M. Analysis of a Wake-Up Task-Based Mobile Alarm App. Appl. Sci. 2020, 10, 3993. https://doi.org/10.3390/app10113993

AMA Style

Oh KT, Shin J, Kim J, Ko M. Analysis of a Wake-Up Task-Based Mobile Alarm App. Applied Sciences. 2020; 10(11):3993. https://doi.org/10.3390/app10113993

Chicago/Turabian Style

Oh, Kyue Taek, Jaemyung Shin, Jaejeung Kim, and Minsam Ko. 2020. "Analysis of a Wake-Up Task-Based Mobile Alarm App" Applied Sciences 10, no. 11: 3993. https://doi.org/10.3390/app10113993

APA Style

Oh, K. T., Shin, J., Kim, J., & Ko, M. (2020). Analysis of a Wake-Up Task-Based Mobile Alarm App. Applied Sciences, 10(11), 3993. https://doi.org/10.3390/app10113993

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop