Next Article in Journal
Enzyme Catalysis: Advances, Techniques, and Outlooks
Next Article in Special Issue
Functional and Sensory Properties of Gingerbread Enriched with the Addition of Vegetables
Previous Article in Journal
Presenting a Human Pupil Orbit Model (HPOM) for Eye-Gaze Tracking
Previous Article in Special Issue
Effect of Scents on Gazing Behavior and Choice
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Linking the Sensory Taste Properties of Chocolate-Based Biscuits to Consumers’ Emotions: A Cross-Cultural Study

by
Annchen Mielmann
1,* and
Thomas A. Brunner
2
1
The Africa Unit for Transdisciplinary Health Research (AUTHeR), School of Physiology, Nutrition and Consumer Sciences, North-West University, Potchefstroom 2531, South Africa
2
Food Science and Management, School of Agricultural, Forest and Food Sciences (HAFL), Bern University of Applied Sciences, Länggasse 85, 3052 Zollikofen, Switzerland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Appl. Sci. 2022, 12(16), 8038; https://doi.org/10.3390/app12168038
Submission received: 20 June 2022 / Revised: 26 July 2022 / Accepted: 26 July 2022 / Published: 11 August 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sensory Characteristics and Consumers Acceptance of Food Products)

Abstract

:
This research measured consumers’ emotions and change in emotion to the specific sensory taste properties and attitudes of chocolate-based biscuits. The sample size involved 216 respondents from South Africa (n = 106) and Switzerland (n = 110). Respondents tasted chocolate-based biscuits and completed an online questionnaire. The increase in consumers’ levels of guilt after chocolate-based biscuit consumption and the contribution of a chocolate taste and craving attitude to consumers’ subsequent positive emotions and change in positive emotions could help food and consumer scientists to understand the link between emotions and the sensory descriptors of chocolate-based biscuits. Investigating the association between the emotional responses and sensory attributes of sweet baked products could benefit product developers when formulating food products for specific target markets and aid in the understanding of the emotional profile of food products.

1. Introduction

Ultra-processed food products include food-derived substances (e.g., starch, sugars, oils and fats) that include ingredients such as artificial additives, colourants, flavourants and emulsifiers [1]. Sweet baked products may include a variety of pastries, cakes, biscuits, breads and other sweetened snacks classified as ultra-processed food. Current trends in consumers’ eating habits include a higher consumption of energy-dense snacks, leading to less energy consumed at breakfast, lunch and dinner [2]. The snacking trend is contributing to the growth of the global bakery products market, which is driven by consumers’ choices of sweet baked products that are easily accessible, resulting in a range of ready-to-eat baked goods.
Globally, confections, cakes and biscuits (sweet baked products) categories hold a large share of snack sales [3]. It is estimated that the market for global bakery products will reach USD 457.4 billion, and it is projected to reach a compound annual growth rate (CAGR) of 3.9% by 2027. Due to the unforeseen economic implications caused by the COVID-19 pandemic, growth in the sweet baked products segment has been revised to 3.8% CAGR for the next 7 years [4]. South Africa showed a strong increase of 8.47% in 2020 [5], and in 2021, the category value share for sweet biscuits was the second highest (23.1%), with chocolate having the highest share at 23.8% [6].
Comparably in Switzerland, biscuits are a popular sweet baked product and regarded as a convenient snack. For Switzerland, it is projected that the market will grow by 3.34% (CAGR 2021–2026) [7]. It is predicted that by the end of 2024, chocolate-based biscuits will dominate the overall market with a share of 39.4% [8].

1.1. Emotional Response

Emotional response is defined as an adjustable reaction to an evoked stimulus, which includes bodily or behavioural responses and a change in subjective feeling [9]. The main factors affecting emotional responses towards foods include the type of food product and its sensory attributes, and the consumers’ characteristics. Emotional eating may increase the consumption and preference for unhealthy food products with a high sugar and fat content [10]. Therefore, emotional responses are often the deciding component explaining respective variations in the preference for sweet-tasting products [11]. However, a consumer’s perception towards a food product is influenced by the emotion evoked by the product (at that moment), and their preference, emotional response and attitudes towards the product. When considering the emotional responses evoked by different food types, it is evident that some foods are considered more emotional than others. For example, fried chicken, pizza and ice cream are labelled as more emotional, whilst carrots and oatmeal are classified as less emotional [12]. Generally, emotion-laden foods are consumed to satisfy cravings and not for nutritional benefit [13]. Reasons for consuming emotion-laden food products could include temptations, an impulsive motivator and the desire to improve ones’ general emotional state of well-being [14].

1.2. Emotional Response to Sensory Properties

More researchers are investigating the relationship between a food products’ sensory attributes and its emotional profile [15]. Emotional aspects are connected to consumers’ sensory experiences—this could influence their food preference [16]. Researchers such as Robin have aimed to link specific emotions to sensory properties; respondents had to link the emotions with the basic tastes (bitter, sweet, salty, sour) [17]. The emotion disgust was associated with a bitter taste, happiness with sweet and the emotions of surprise, happiness, fear, sadness, anger and disgust with sour and salty tastes.
Previous research has indicated sensory attributes in driving emotions for an array of foods, such as wine [18], coffee [19], alcoholic cocktails [20] and chocolates [21]. For example, tasting energy-dense food high in fat or sugar elicits positive affective reactions that encourage ingestion, while the tasting of bitter foods elicits negative affective reactions that promote rejection [22]. Researchers stated that the sensory characteristics of chocolate served as a driver for emotional response and confirmed an association between specific sensory characteristics and emotional perceptions [23]. Researchers developed a single-response emotion word to measure to associate emotional responses to peanut butter chocolate chip biscuits and chocolate chip biscuits. While the peanut butter chocolate chip biscuits were more strongly associated with blue/uninspired and unhappy/dissatisfied emotions than the biscuit containing chocolate chips, the emotions of secure/at ease were more strongly associated with the regular chocolate chip biscuits [24].

1.3. Emotional Response to Attitudes

Emotional responses depend on consumers’ attitudes toward food. Researchers clustered attitude into two distinct clusters, craving and guilty attitude. While those with a craving attitude seek comfort from foods under emotionally stressful conditions, a guilty attitude is linked with dissatisfaction with one’s body image and weight [25]. Consumers do not necessarily eat emotion-laden foods for their nutritional benefits, but rather to satisfy cravings or fulfil an emotional need [13]. Researchers found that high-attribute chocolate cravers indicated more positive implicit attitudinal behaviour towards chocolate [26]. Chocolate could arouse one’s sensory pleasure and provide emotional comfort [27] as it contributes positively to one’s emotional state by providing immediate pleasure [28]. Chocolate consumption has been associated with both positive and negative emotions (guilt is associated with weight gain) [29]. For example, a study on temporal dominance of emotions revealed that for plain chocolate, the sensory attributes crunchy, dry, bitter and cocoa were associated with more negative emotions, such as calm and bored (low arousal) and aggressive (high arousal). For flavoured chocolate, the sensory attributes fruity, sweet, crunchy and melting were linked to positive emotions such as happy, energetic and loving [30].

1.4. A Cultural Perspective

Food culture refers to a specific country’s cultural traditions, agricultural and trade practices, food processing and purchase and consumption habits, which significantly influence consumers’ food choices [31]. Therefore, consumers’ cultural (anthropology) origins may influence their food choices [32]. South Africa is classified as an emerging country due to its large markets for goods and services, and entrepreneurs that can compete globally if given adequate support [33]. A transition in food consumption behaviour has led to an increased intake of processed foods high in carbohydrates [34]. These changes in a country’s food choices negatively affect public health as ultra-processed foods contribute to increased obesity and non-communicable diseases (NCDs) [35]. Switzerland is listed among the top high-income countries with a very high life expectancy and indicates very low prevalence rates of obesity [36]. Notable differences exist between socio-demographic clusters with a possible risk of developing NCDs [37]. Ultra-processed foods could therefore transform dietary patterns, food culture and supplies as for many high-income countries, such as Switzerland, the supply of ultra-processed foods is starting to dominate the market [38], therefore increasing the consumption of these products in low-/middle-income countries [39] such as South Africa [40].
Whilst various studies [29] have investigated emotional responses to chocolate products, measuring the link between the sensory characteristics and the emotions food products evoke remains unsatisfactory in a cross-cultural context. Furthermore, most of these studies aim to describe emotional responses’ commercial application and marketing function, providing value and insight to the industry [41], without considering it from a cultural perspective, which would benefit the consumer. Cross-cultural research is necessary to understand consumers’ affective responses and emotions in different countries. Research has indicated that different cultures understand the same set of emotions but could express them differently [42].
A comparison between South Africa and Switzerland was made as consumers from emerging countries, such as South Africa, and high-income countries, such as Switzerland, all have different learning experiences with sweet baked products, causing different emotional responses to sensory properties and attitudes. Research into consumers’ emotional responses towards emotion-laden foods across cultures is still limited. These emotional and attitudinal responses can aid the food industry in identifying product-specific characteristics to develop and promote the consumption of chocolate-based biscuits, which can be used to understand the link between emotions, sensory descriptors and attitudes. Furthermore, emotional responses can provide insight into consumers’ food choices and can be used to market the processing of more health attributes.
Therefore, from a cultural perspective, the aim of this paper was to determine consumers’ emotions and changes in emotion to the specific sensory taste properties and attitudes of chocolate-based biscuits.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Respondents

The responses were collected from respondents in South Africa (n = 106) and Switzerland (n = 110). Respondents received a link for the electronic screening questionnaire that determined whether they were (a) above the age of 18 years; (b) citizens of the particular country; (c) not allergic to bovine milk, gluten, wheat and/or soy; and (d) consumers of chocolate-based biscuits. All qualified respondents received a link containing the final electronic questionnaire. Biscuits were delivered to a South African retail company, and in Switzerland, the biscuits were sent by standard mail. Table 1 provides the demographic characteristics of the South African and Swiss samples.

2.2. Chocolate-Based Biscuits

Biscuits were chosen for this study because of their familiarity and practicality in the food sector and acceptable sensory features such as taste and texture [43]. Chocolate-based biscuits were chosen as baked products containing chocolate are one of the most popular and acceptable products owing to their longer shelf life, versatility and pleasant sensory attributes [44]. Each respondent was provided with one snack pack containing four biscuits for the home-use test from a well-known brand in South Africa and Switzerland, respectively. Both biscuits contained similar sugar (>30 g/100 g), salt (>0.30 g/100 g) and fat (>20 g/100 g) contents (Table A1). It was required of the respondents to taste the biscuits and complete the questionnaire where consumption usually occurs e.g., work, home…

2.3. Questionnaire

Data gathering was conducted using an online questionnaire [45] operated by QuestionPro (v20.4, Seattle, WA, USA). The screening and final questionnaires were translated from English to German and verified by a native German speaker. The questionnaires were back-translated to English by a qualified language translator. The questionnaire consisted of five sections. Appendix A reveals the scale and items used in the study (Table A2).
In the first and third sections, respondents’ emotional responses (positive and negative emotions) was recorded before and after evaluating the sensory taste properties of the biscuits, as one’s emotional response to food is often sensitive to expectations based on the sensory attributes of a single taste [46], and the sensory attributes have a strong connection with consumers’ emotions. Emotions are viewed based on two aspects: positive versus negative and pleasure versus displeasure. The EsSense25 Profile is a questionnaire that contains a list of single words that describe emotional responses to foods and was validated against the EsSense list (39 attributes) by [47], displaying a balance between positive and negative words. The EsSense25 Profile was consulted to measure emotional responses (5-point Likert scale with 1 = not at all and 5 = extremely) [47].
In the second section, the rate-all-that-apply (RATA) tool measured the intensity of sensory taste properties using a 5-point unipolar verbal scale with 1 = not at all and 5 = extremely [48]. As a starting point for the sensory taste properties, a list of 12 sensory descriptors for the biscuits were retrieved from the study by Heenan and colleagues [49]. A trained panel reduced the sensory descriptor list from 12 taste descriptors to 6 taste descriptors (chocolate, vanilla, buttery, sweet, caramel, nutty) which best described the two chocolate-based biscuits and the discrepancies between the biscuits in the market due to their sensory variation [50]. In addition, the list of sensory descriptors was reduced to prevent loss of interest and respondent fatigue.
The fourth section applied the Attitudes to Chocolate Questionnaire (ACQ) to determine respondents’ attitudes towards the sweet baked biscuits. The ACQ consists of 20 statements [25] and implements a 5-point scale with 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree. To gain more insight into the reasons for consuming chocolates, [25] developed the ACQ to evaluate consumers’ cravings for chocolate and guilt when consuming chocolate. Due to attitude’s evaluative element [51], consumers’ attitude of sweet baked products was determined after emotional response after the tasting of the chocolate-based biscuits, and reporting on emotional response before and after consuming the biscuits can create assumptions that may be triggered by the biscuits [23].
In the final section, respondents answered demographic questions, including those regarding age, gender, education level, home language, monthly household income and marital and occupational status (Table 1).

2.4. Statistical Analysis

IBM SPSS Statistics (version 24) was used for all statistical analyses. The internal coherence of the seven indicators and four variables that determined respondents’ emotions towards the biscuits were measured with Cronbach’s alpha coefficient. For the demographic characteristics, emotions, sensory taste properties, attitudes and descriptive statistics were applied.
For the association between sensory terms, Pearson’s correlations were implemented. To determine the indicators of consumers’ positive and negative emotions before and after consuming the biscuits, multiple linear regression analyses were implemented. To indicate the significance of consumers’ changes in positive and negative emotional states after completing the survey, a t-test was run.
Differences were assigned to multiple linear regression analysis (MLRA). Duplicate statistical methods and indices were used to show the factors contributing to the change in emotion. Proof of homoscedasticity, normality and collinearity diagnostics showed the appropriateness for regression analyses. For this study, an enter technique with the use of an experimental setting of the study was applied.
Four regression models, all subjected to MLRA, explained the dependent variable, “positive/negative emotion direct measurement” or “positive/negative emotional change direct measure”, with three other predictor variables, i.e., sensory properties, attitude (craving) and attitude (guilty).

3. Results

3.1. Mean Values for Emotional Response, Sensory Properties and Attitudes

Respondents’ emotional responses before and after biscuit consumption are indicated in Table 2 and Table 3, respectively. Before biscuit consumption, for the positive emotions, the South Africans felt happy (3.52 ± 1.09), and the Swiss respondents felt satisfied (3.64 ± 0.84). For the negative emotions, both samples felt worried (2.02 ± 1.34; 1.98 ± 1.02) (Table 2).
The South African and Swiss respondents felt good (3.06 ± 1.19) and calm (3.27 ± 0.99) after consuming the biscuits. Consumers’ positive and negative emotions decreased after consuming the biscuits (2.54 ± 1.14; 1.38 ± 0.75), except for the negative emotion guilt, which increased after biscuit consumption (1.35 ± 0.85; 1.47 ± 0.93) (Table 3).
Mean values with applicable standard deviation scores for the sensory properties and respondents’ attitudes are indicated in Table 4 and Table 5, respectively. A chocolate and sweet taste was the most intense taste experienced by South African (3.80 ± 1.06; 3.65 ± 0.93) and Swiss (3.68 ± 0.82; 3.69 ± 0.82) respondents.
For respondents’ craving attitudes to sweet baked products, South Africans indicated that “my desire for sweet baked products often seems overpowering” (3.43 ± 1.07), while the Swiss respondents revealed that “even when I do not really want any sweeter baked products, I will often carry on eating it” (3.35 ± 1.02). For respondents’ guilty attitudes, both the countries indicated that “I consider sweet baked products to be high in fat and of poor nutritional value” (South Africa: 3.32 ± 1.23; Swiss: 3.65 ± 1.15).

3.2. Emotional Responses after Biscuit Consumption

For this study, the researchers focused on the respondents’ emotional responses after biscuit consumption. Table A3 provides a summary of regression analysis for the variables predicting consumers’ emotions before biscuit consumption.
The results of respondents’ subsequent positive and negative emotions are provided in Table 6. South Africans regarded the sensory taste variables vanilla, chocolate and caramel as positive predictors influencing their positive emotions towards chocolate-based biscuits. For South African respondents, a craving attitude implied a positive outlook, while a guilty attitude suggested a negative outlook on consumers’ subsequent positive emotions.
Unsurprisingly, Swiss consumers indicated that a chocolate taste increased their subsequent positive emotions. Respondents’ positive emotions after biscuit consumption were explained by the regression models: 54% (South Africa) and 10% (Swiss). Therefore, it can be confirmed that for both countries, the sensory property, chocolate taste, positively affected consumers’ subsequent positive emotions.
For South Africans, mostly females (above the age of 35 years) with an education higher than Grade 12, the sensory taste variables, caramel and sweet, increased their negative emotions towards chocolate-based biscuits. A craving attitude was positively related to negative emotions after consuming the biscuits. Swiss consumers, predominantly females earning an income of more than CHF 6000 per month, regarded a vanilla taste as a positive indicator towards their level of negative emotions.
For both populations, a nutty taste, guilty attitude and the demographic variable gender were positively related to their negative emotions towards chocolate-based biscuits. Interestingly, a buttery and chocolate taste decreased both groups’ negative emotions. For both consumer groups, a negative emotion was more positively influenced by a guilty attitude.
Consumers’ negative emotions after biscuit consumption were explained by the regression models: 40% (South Africa) and 20% (Swiss). It is evident that for both countries, the sensory property nutty taste positively affect consumers’ subsequent negative emotions, and a guilty attitude increased consumers’ subsequent negative emotions.
In relation to explicit attributes, food products containing bitter and sweet attributes are seen as more complex. Attributes such as nutty, coffee, cocoa, tart and bitter are associated with the bitter attribute segment, while chocolate and vanilla are associated with the sweet attribute segment [52].
Researchers determined the associations between the sensory characteristics of milk chocolate and consumers’ emotional responses and how specific internal consumer behaviour variables (e.g., attitudes) influenced this response. Results reveal that bitterness influenced negative emotion, while sweetness had no effect on either positive or negative emotion [53]. These findings are supported by [54], as pleasant emotion categories received high scores for high sweetness and low bitterness (and vice versa), meaning that both the sweet and bitter attributes influenced emotion segments.

3.3. Change in Emotions towards Biscuit Consumption

Table 7 predicts consumers’ positive and negative emotional changes. For South Africans, the taste variables chocolate and vanilla increased the change in positive emotion towards chocolate-based biscuits. For both population groups, a craving attitude suggested an increase towards positive emotions. For Swiss respondents who were mostly living in the city, a chocolate taste increased the change in positive emotion for South Africans; however, South Africans’ cravings towards chocolate-based biscuits was higher.
The regression models for respondents from South Africa and Switzerland’s change in positive emotions explained 39% and 24%, respectively. Based on the results, it can be established that for both samples, the sensory property, chocolate taste, increased consumers’ positive emotional changes and a craving attitude increased consumers’ positive emotional changes.
South African respondents (older than 35 years) earning an income of more than ZAR 20,000 per month, indicated that a buttery and vanilla taste increased the change in negative emotions towards chocolate-based biscuits. The negative contribution of South Africans’ craving attitudes to a change in negative emotions was confirmed, as discussed in the previous section.
As predicted, a chocolate taste decreased the change in negative emotions for both groups. Swiss consumers’ (who mainly lived in the city and were not working) attitudes towards guilt increased their change in negative emotions more than those of South Africans. Income and occupational status increased South Africans’ and Swiss respondents’ changes in negative emotions, respectively. The regression model explained 21% (South Africa) and 14% (Swiss), respectively. It can therefore be confirmed that for both samples, the sensory properties nutty and chocolate taste decreased consumers’ negative emotional changes and a guilty attitude increased consumers’ negative emotional changes.

4. Discussion

This study reported an overall decrease in consumers’ positive and negative emotions after biscuit consumption, except for the negative emotion guilt. Researchers examined consumers’ emotional changes after eating chocolate and found that chocolate could evoke responses of guilt in healthy/normal-weight women. These emotional responses are likely to be prompted by negative food-related perceptions dependent on culturally determined attitudes to slimness/body weight [55].
Respondents experienced guilt that resulted in lower positive emotions. Due to chocolate’s sensory pleasure, it evoked positive emotions during and after consumption, but the level of guilt increased thereafter due to the respondents’ negative thoughts. Kim and co-workers investigated emotions toward sweet-tasting foods according to sweet-liker status and mentioned that a possible reason that emotional responses varied between respondents who like or dislike sweet foods in a non-consistent manner with the sweetness level of sweet-tasting foods is that independent pathways determine development of liking, and varied emotional responses are evoked for that food product [11]. This raised the possibility that, for respondents who liked sweet foods, emotional responses to sweet foods were more influenced by sweetness, while the emotional responses of respondents who disliked sweet foods to sweet foods were more linked with how much the sweet food was liked.
Researchers reported that there are cases where a single sensory attribute was associated with different emotional terms, characterized by different arousal and/or valence properties. In as study where chocolate was used as the subject, sweetness was significantly linked with emotional responses that expressed pleasant activation and both pleasant and unpleasant deactivation, confirming that consumers can be segmented on their emotional responses to the sensory properties of food products [30]. Researchers confirmed in their study that sweet-tasting chocolates increased the positive emotional response and reduced the negative emotional response, but bitter chocolates increased the negative emotional response and reduced positive emotions [56].
Furthermore, respondents’ guilty attitudes increased their negative emotions and changes in negative emotions towards chocolate-based biscuits, as they regarded these to be of poor nutritional value and high in fat. Researchers confirmed that the two major ingredients in biscuits are usually sugar and fat [57]. Biscuits are regarded as high-reward food items—they lead consumers to act impulsively, to seek variety in their choice of biscuits and influence them by positive hedonic affect. It could be foreseen that positive characteristics may thus outweigh the negative ones in choice cues in the form of an approach–avoidance imbalance or cognitive suspension process [58].
A chocolate taste increased respondents’ positive emotions, but a guilty attitude increased the change in their negative emotions. Differences in emotional responses experienced by women after consuming chocolate in everyday life increased their emotions of joy and guilt. While the sensory enjoyment when consuming chocolates evoked joy, negative thoughts (e.g., effect on body weight) linked with chocolate consumption evoked guilt.
A recent study found that more than 50% of Swiss consumers regarded taste as important when purchasing food products [59]. Research revealed that for South African consumers, the sweetness of baked products was an important driver of choice of whether the sweet food was found to be pleasurable [60]. Milk chocolates are currently the most preferred type as plain milk chocolate is preferred over dark chocolate [61].
In Switzerland, milk chocolate forms an integral part of Swiss culture, and the Swiss are regarded as chocolate connoisseurs [62]. Part of both countries’ growing diversification into culinary tourism is the offering of food and beverages, which includes chocolate products, paired with wine and coffee [63,64].
The differences in the regression models of emotions and changes in emotion between the South African and Swiss samples could possibly be described by different influential factors, such as South Africans’ higher attitudes towards the craving of chocolate-based biscuits; for instance, one respondent stated “my desire for sweet baked products often seems overpowering”. Food cravings are an important predictor of food consumption. Increased food cravings are linked with consuming craved foods, such as chocolate, more habitually [65]. Chocolate can act as a mood booster and may also increase cravings or impose negative emotions, such as guilt [66] when considered as an unhealthy snack. Negative emotional responses such as tiredness, depression and boredom are associated with food cravings [25].
Swiss consumers’ craving attitudes can mainly be ascribed to their lack of self-control; for instance, one respondent reported that “even when I do not really want any more chocolate-based biscuits, I will often carry on eating them”. Different styles of overeating have been reported; for example, external eating (e.g., responding to a pleasant taste) has been linked with an increase in consumption of high-calorie foods [67]; however, research has indicated that gaining weight for some time was not affected [68]. Furthermore, emotional eating (e.g., responding to negative emotional responses), was positively linked with eating too much calorie-dense food [66] and was linked with weight gain in the long term [69]. In a longitudinal study by [68], high indecisiveness towards palatable foods was linked with more extreme weight variations among females, implying that indecisiveness towards palatable food may result in a habit of overeating [68,69]. These results suggest that initiating interventions to help consumers improve their self-control to restrict their cravings towards chocolate-based biscuits may prevent the overconsumption of high-calorie, energy-dense foods.

4.1. Value of the Study

Two questions emerge from this study. Firstly, how can positive emotions, such as the level of goodness for the South African sample and calmness for the Swiss group, be maintained or increased, and negative emotions, such as the level of guilt and boredom, be reduced when introducing healthier sweet baked product options to consumers? Secondly, how can South Africans’ overpowering desire and Swiss consumers’ lack of self-control in overeating be addressed to assist them in controlling their craving attitudes towards sweet baked products?
The knowledge gained from comparing South Africa and Swiss consumers’ emotions and changes in emotions towards chocolate-based biscuits will help researchers to link the sensory taste properties of chocolate-based biscuits and attitudes to respondents’ emotions. This study creates a starting point for future research, presenting variables that positively/negatively contribute to consumers’ emotions and changes in emotions towards chocolate-based biscuits so that healthier sweet baked products may be developed. Therefore, emotional responses can be applied as a segmentation, differentiation and product development [70] measure, as the association between food products and consumers’ emotions is essential to ensure a positive product experience.
Understanding the emotional responses among consumers from different cultures remains an important issue in sensory and consumer sciences. Investigating the association between the sensory attributes of foods and consumers’ emotions could help product developers to formulate food products for specific markets and assist in the understanding of the emotional profile of food products. Furthermore, if consumers’ emotions and attitudes towards chocolate-based biscuits are incorporated from a cultural perspective, their feedback and product acceptance could then be predicted; communicating emotional responses to consumers or food product developers could aid in the development of sensory–emotion lexicons for the chocolate-based biscuit category, for example, labelling healthy chocolate-based biscuits with the emotions they evoke. More research on emotion testing before and after the consumption of individual sweet baked products is required to understand the link between sensory properties and emotional responses.

4.2. Limitations and Recommendations

Measuring only one type of biscuit from one brand must be noted and considered when interpreting the data. Due to cultural differences in the interpretations associated with processed foods [71], replicating findings in different cultures would explain the extent to which the findings are applicable to other cultures and sweet baked products.
Furthermore, the same sensory taste descriptors were used for the two biscuits. Researchers reported that European consumers revealed that country of residence with variation in dietary habits was strongly associated with their preferences for the basic tastes of sweet, salty, bitter and umami, suggesting that food culture plays a significant role in food preference and taste perception [72].
Therefore, for a clearer understanding of cross-cultural variability in the sensory properties of chocolate-based biscuits, future research should include different taste, olfactory and visual descriptors for each product. Further studies will need to examine variables such as including a beverage, e.g., coffee or tea, and the social context (e.g., alone, with family), as comfort foods are usually linked with acceptance and security (positive emotional responses) [73]; these food-related emotions may originate from pairing with relatives or cultural contexts [11].
Future researchers may investigate by which processes the sensory attributes, emotions and attitude correspond with foods. Classical or Pavlovian conditioning [74] could be applicable; from the repeated reappearance of a food product with sensory–emotional–attitudinal-associated stimuli, consuming this food product may emerge as a conditioned stimulus influencing emotional and attitude (e.g., craving) states; for example, when energy-dense food products reappear with pictures or descriptions of obese silhouettes in the media, consuming similar food products may evoke anxiety regarding weight management. Therefore, any attempt to change consumers’ detrimental choices should also consider their emotional response towards food, for example, exploring emotions evoked by food products and determining whether they prevent with choosing and eating healthier foods.

Author Contributions

A.M. was responsible for the conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, investigation and writing—original draft; T.A.B. was responsible for the conceptualization, validation, resources, supervision and writing—review and editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by The Scientific Committee of School of Agricultural, Forest, and Food Sciences, Bern University of Applied Sciences, approved the study (2016.08).

Informed Consent Statement

All the respondents were required to give informed consent, and participation in this study was completely voluntary. The respondents were reassured of their confidentiality and the privacy of the information that they supplied.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Eugenia Harms for her assistance with the sensory descriptors and Franziska Götze for her assistance with the translation of the questionnaires.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Nutritional value of chocolate-based biscuits per 100g.
Table A1. Nutritional value of chocolate-based biscuits per 100g.
DescriptionSouth AfricaSwitzerland
Kilojoules (kJ)20102110
Kilocalories (kcal)480504
Carbohydrates (g)6967
Carbohydrates of which are sugar (g)3832
Protein (g)55
Fat (g)2023
Fiber (g)2.52.5
Salt (g)0.610.31
Table A2. Scales and items used for cluster analysis, including internal consistency analyses.
Table A2. Scales and items used for cluster analysis, including internal consistency analyses.
ItemsFactor LoadingsCronbach’s Alpha
Positive emotions
 Adventurous0.780
 Calm0.812
 Enthusiastic0.716
 Free0.842
 Good0.867
 Happy0.641
 Joyful0.631
 Nostalgic0.354
 Satisfied0.814
 Secure0.722
 Positive emotions before eating 0.834
 Positive emotion after eating 0.900
Negative emotions
 Aggressive0.760
 Guilty0.682
 Bored0.612
 Worried0.836
 Negative emotion before eating 0.664
 Negative emotion after eating 0.562
Sensory taste properties 0.707
 Chocolate0.930
 Vanilla0.804
 Buttery0.518
 Sweet0.379
 Caramel0.826
 Nutty0.719
Attitude: craving 0.872
 I eat sweet baked products to cheer me up when I am down0.575
 I often eat sweet baked products when I am bored0.429
 I like to indulge in sweet baked products0.590
 My desire for sweet baked products often seems overpowering0.673
 The thought of sweet baked products often distracts me from what I am doing (e.g., watching TV) 0.685
 I usually find myself wanting sweet baked products during the afternoon0.685
 I often go into a shop for something else and end up buying sweet baked products0.692
 Sweet baked products often prey on my mind0.397
 Nothing else but sweet baked products will satisfy my cravings0.699
 Even when I do not really want any more sweet baked Products I will often carry on eating them0.752
Attitude: guilt 0.905
 I feel unattractive after I have eaten sweet baked products0.817
 I often feel sick after eating sweet baked products0.724
 I am often on one kind of diet or another0.617
 I consider sweet baked products to be high in fat and of poor nutritional value0.590
 After eating sweet baked products, I often wish I hadn’t0.868
 I feel guilty after eating sweet baked products0.789
 I feel depressed and dissatisfied with life after eating sweet baked products0.769
 I feel unhealthy after I have eaten sweet baked products0.867
 I always look at the calorie value of a sweet baked product before I eat it0.648
 If I resist the temptation to eat sweet baked products, I feel more in control of my life0.640
Table A3. Summary of regression analysis for variables predicting consumers’ emotions before biscuit consumption.
Table A3. Summary of regression analysis for variables predicting consumers’ emotions before biscuit consumption.
BSE Bβp
Positive emotions
SA (R2 = 0.33)
 Constant4.160.34 0.00 **
 Caramel taste0.270.060.380.00 **
 Attitude: guilt−0.370.07−0.450.00 **
 Gender−0.570.16−0.310.00 **
 Occupational status−0.590.28−0.170.04 *
Swiss (R2 = 0.10)
 Constant3.160.21 0.00 **
 Attitude: guilt−0.190.08−0.260.01 *
 People >16 years in household0.140.070.210.05
Negative emotions
SA (R2 = 0.21)
Aggressive
 Constant2.010.43 0.00 **
 Attitude: guilt0.290.090.280.00 **
 Single−0.840.22−0.410.00 **
 Income−0.540.21−0.260.01 *
 People >16 years in household−0.210.10−0.210.03 *
Bored (R2 = 0.22)
 Constant1.980.33 0.00 **
 Attitude: guilt0.280.100.260.00 **
 Age−0.570.19−0.280.00 **
 Income−0.680.22−0.320.00 **
 Single−0.500.22−0.240.02 *
Guilty (R2 = 0.10)
 Constant0.520.37 0.16
 Attitude: craving0.350.120.270.01 *
Worried (R2 = 0.21)
 Constant1.030.51 0.05
 Nutty taste0.500.120.400.00 **
 Vanilla taste−0.330.12−0.280.01 *
 Attitude: craving.360.15.210.02 *
Swiss
Aggressive (R2 = 0.19)
 Constant0.610.15 0.00 **
 Vanilla taste0.210.070.300.00 **
 Residence area0.270.110.240.02 *
 Gender0.230.110.200.04 *
Bored (R2 = 0.23)
 Constant1.810.37 0.00 **
 Attitude: guilt0.400.100.360.00 **
 Age−1.150.32−0.330.00 **
Guilty (R2 = 0.20)
 Constant0.470.18 0.01 *
 Caramel taste0.220.060.370.00 **
 Attitude: craving0.140.070.200.04 *
Worried (R2 = 0.16)
 Constant1.190.21 0.00 **
 Caramel taste0.450.110.400.00 **
Total population
Aggressive (R2 = 0.13)
 Constant0.450.21 0.04 *
 Attitude: guilt0.150.070.150.04 *
 Attitude: craving0.170.080.170.03 *
 Residence area0.340.140.170.01 *
 Single−0.330.13−0.170.01 *
Bored (R2 = 0.16)
 Constant1.450.21 0.00 **
 Attitude: guilt0.280.070.260.00 **
 Age−0.580.14−0.270.00 **
Guilty (R2 = 0.13)
 Constant0.320.20 0.12
 Nutty taste0.190.060.210.00 **
 Attitude: craving0.290.070.270.00 **
Worried (R2 = 0.21)
 Constant0.610.27 0.02 *
 Nutty taste0.340.080.270.00 **
 Attitude: guilt0.390.090.280.00 **
 Gender0.570.170.230.00 **
 Income−0.370.17−0.150.03 *
Note: SA = South Africa; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.001. B = unstandardized coefficient; SE B = standard error; β = standardized coefficient; p = significance level.

References

  1. Monteiro, C.A.; Cannon, G.; Levy, R.B.; Moubarac, J.-C.; Lc Louzada, M.; Rauber, F.; Khandpur, N.; Cediel, G.; Neri, D.; Martinez-Steele, E.; et al. Ultra-processed foods: What they are and how to identify them. Public Health Nutr. 2019, 22, 936–941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Kant, A.K.; Graubard, B.I. 40-year trends in meal and snack eating behaviors of American adults. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2015, 115, 50–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Peckenpaugh, D.J. 2022 Snack & Bakery Industry Outlook. Available online: https://www.preparedfoods.com/articles/126673-2022-snack-and-bakery-industry-outlook (accessed on 7 June 2022).
  4. ReportLinker. Global Bakery Products Industry. 2021. Available online: https://www.reportlinker.com/p05069906/Global-Bakery-Products-Industry.html (accessed on 7 June 2022).
  5. MarketResearch.com. Cakes, Pastries and Sweet Pies (Bakery and Cereals) Market in South Africa—Outlook to 2025; Market Size, Growth and Forecast Analytics. Available online: https://www.marketresearch.com/GlobalData-v3648/Cakes-Pastries-Sweet-Pies-Bakery-30328458/ (accessed on 7 June 2022).
  6. IPSOS. South Africans Spending More on Snacks. Available online: https://www.ipsos.com/en-za/south-africans-spending-more-snacks (accessed on 7 June 2022).
  7. Statista. Bread & Cereal Products. Switzerland. Available online: https://www.statista.com/outlook/cmo/food/bread-cereal-products/switzerland (accessed on 7 June 2022).
  8. Transparency Market Research. Factors Driving Switzerland Biscuits Market through 2024 and beyond. Available online: https://www.transparencymarketresearch.com/pressrelease/switzerland-biscuits-market.htm (accessed on 7 June 2022).
  9. Brosch, T.; Pourtois, G.; Sander, D. The perception and categorisation of emotional stimuli: A review. Cogn Emot. 2010, 24, 377–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bui, C.; Lin, L.-Y.; Wu, C.-Y.; Chiu, Y.-W.; Chiou, H.-Y. Association between Emotional Eating and Frequency of Unhealthy Food Consumption among Taiwanese Adolescents. Nutrients 2021, 13, 2739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Kim, J.-Y.; Prescott, J.; Kim, K.-O. Emotional responses to sweet foods according to sweet liker status. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 59, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Cardello, A.V.; Meiselman, H.L.; Schutz, H.G.; Craig, C.; Given, Z.; Lesher, L.L.; Eicher, S. Measuring emotional responses to foods and food names using questionnaires. Food Qual. Prefer. 2012, 24, 243–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Parker, G.; Parker, I.; Brotchie, H. Mood state effects of chocolate. J. Affect. Disord. 2006, 92, 149–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Jaeger, S.R. The effect of product–context appropriateness on emotion associations in evoked eating occasions. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 40, 49–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Lagast, S.; De Steur, H.; Schouteten, J.J.; Gellynck, X. A comparison of two low-calorie sweeteners and sugar in dark chocolate on sensory attributes and emotional conceptualisations. Int. J. Food Sci. Nutr. 2018, 69, 344–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Köster, E.P.; Mojet, J. From mood to food and from food to mood: A psychological perspective on the measurement of food-related emotions in consumer research. Food Res. Int. 2015, 76, 180–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Robin, O.; Rousmans, S.; Dittmar, A.; Vernet-Maury, E. Autonomic estimated basic emotions induced by primary tastes. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2000, 54, S14–S15. [Google Scholar]
  18. Mora, M.; Urdaneta, E.; Chaya, C. Emotional response to wine: Sensory properties, age and gender as drivers of consumers’ preferences. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 66, 19–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Pinsuwan, A.; Suwonsichon, S.; Chompreeda, P.; Prinyawiwatkul, W. Sensory Drivers of Consumer Acceptance, Purchase Intent and Emotions toward Brewed Black Coffee. Foods 2022, 11, 180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Pierguidi, L.; Spinelli, S.; Dinnella, C.; Prescott, J.; Monteleone, E. Liking patterns moderate the relationship between sensory, emotional and context appropriateness profiles: Evidences from a Global Profile study on alcoholic cocktails. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 83, 103904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. De Pelsmaeker, S.; Gellynck, X.; Dewettinck, K.; Schouteten, J. Effect of tasting and flavour on chocolate-evoked emotions by consumers. Br. Food J. 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Dubovski, N.; Ert, E.; Niv, M.Y. Bitter mouth-rinse affects emotions. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 60, 154–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Thomson, D.M.; Crocker, C.; Marketo, C.G. Linking sensory characteristics to emotions: An example using dark chocolate. Food Qual. Prefer. 2010, 21, 1117–1125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Jaeger, S.R.; Roigard, C.M.; Jin, D.; Xia, Y.; Zhong, F.; Hedderley, D.I. A single-response emotion word questionnaire for measuring product-related emotional associations inspired by a circumplex model of core affect: Method characterisation with an applied focus. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 83, 103805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Benton, D.; Greenfield, K.; Morgan, M. The development of the attitudes to chocolate questionnaire. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1998, 24, 513–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Richard, A.; Meule, A.; Friese, M.; Blechert, J. Effects of chocolate deprivation on implicit and explicit evaluation of chocolate in high and low trait chocolate cravers. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Jeon, H.M.; Yoo, S.R.; Kim, S.H. The Relationships among Experience, Delight, and Recollection for Revisit Intention in Chocolate Exposition. Sustainability 2020, 12, 8644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Paoletti, R.; Poli, A.; Conti, A.; Visioli, F. Chocolate and Health; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  29. Merlino, V.M.; Mota-Gutierrez, J.; Borra, D.; Brun, F.; Cocolin, L.; Blanc, S.; Massaglia, S. Chocolate culture: Preferences, emotional implications and awareness of Italian consumers. Int. J. Gastron. Food Sci. 2021, 25, 100374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Spinelli, S.; Jaeger, S.R. What do we know about the sensory drivers of emotions in foods and beverages? Curr. Opin. Food Sci. 2019, 27, 82–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Yin, J.; Yang, D.; Zhang, X.; Zhang, Y.; Cai, T.; Hao, Y.; Shenghui, C.; Chen, Y. Diet shift: Considering environment, health and food culture. Sci. Total Environ. 2020, 719, 137484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Rozin, P. The integration of biological, social, cultural and psychological influences on food choice. Front. Nutr. Sci. 2006, 3, 19. [Google Scholar]
  33. Akinemi, F.; Adejumo, O. Entrepreneurial motives and challenges of SMEs owners in emerging economies: Nigeria & South Africa. Adv. Economics Bus. Manag. 2017, 5, 624–633. [Google Scholar]
  34. Ronquest-Ross, L.C.; Vink, N.; Sigge, G.O. Food consumption changes in South Africa since 1994. S. Afr. J. Sci. 2015, 111, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. WHO (World Health Organisation). World Health Statistics 2018: Monitoring Health for the SDGs, Sustainable Development Goals; World Health Organisation: Geneva, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  36. Chatelan, A.; Gaillard, P.; Kruseman, M.; Keller, A. Total, Added, and Free Sugar Consumption and Adherence to Guidelines in Switzerland: Results from the First National Nutrition Survey menuCH. Nutrients 2019, 11, 1117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Krieger, J.-P.; Pestoni, G.; Cabaset, S.; Brombach, C.; Sych, J.; Schader, C.; Faeh, D.; Rohrmann, S. Dietary Patterns and Their Sociodemographic and Lifestyle Determinants in Switzerland: Results from the National Nutrition Survey menuCH. Nutrients 2019, 11, 62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Baker, P.; Machado, P.; Santos, T.; Sievert, K.; Backholer, K.; Hadjikakou, M.; Russell, C.; Huse, O.; Bell, C.; Scrinis, G.; et al. Ultra-processed foods and the nutrition transition: Global, regional and national trends, food systems transformations and political economy drivers. Obes. Rev. 2020, 21, e13126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Monteiro, C.A.; Cannon, G.J. The role of the transnational ultra-processed food industry in the pandemic of obesity and its associated diseases: Problems and solutions. World Nutr. J. 2019, 10, 89–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Stacey, N.; Tugendhaft, A.; Hofman, K. Sugary beverage taxation in South Africa: Household expenditure, demand system elasticities, and policy implications. Prev. Med. 2017, 105, S26–S31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Songa, G.; Slabbinck, H.; Vermeir, I.; Russo, V. How do implicit/explicit attitudes and emotional reactions to sustainable logo relate? A neurophysiological study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 71, 485–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Gupta, M.; Torrico, D.D.; Hepworth, G.; Gras, S.L.; Ong, L.; Cottrell, J.J.; Dunshea, F.R. Differences in Hedonic Responses, Facial Expressions and Self-Reported Emotions of Consumers Using Commercial Yogurts: A Cross-Cultural Study. Foods 2021, 10, 1237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Arepally, D.; Reddy, R.S.; Goswami, T.K.; Datta, A.K. Biscuit baking: A review. LWT 2020, 113, 109726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Omeroglu, P.; Ozdal, T. Fatty acid composition of sweet bakery goods and chocolate products and evaluation of overall nutritional quality in relation to the food label information. J. Food Compos. Anal. 2020, 88, 103438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Mielmann, A.; Brunner, T.A. Consumers’ Motives for Eating and Choosing Sweet Baked Products: A Cross-Cultural Segmentation Study. Foods 2020, 9, 1811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Gibson, E.L. Emotional influences on food choice: Sensory, physiological and psychological pathways. Physiol. Behav. 2006, 89, 53–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Nestrud, M.A.; Meiselman, H.L.; King, S.C.; Lesher, L.L.; Cardello, A.V. Development of EsSense25, a shorter version of the EsSense Profile®. Food Qual Pref. 2016, 48, 107–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Schouteten, J.J.; De Steur, H.; De Pelsmaeker, S.; Lagast, S.; De Bourdeaudhuij, I.; Gellynck, X. An integrated method for the emotional conceptualization and sensory characterization of food products: The EmoSensory® Wheel. Food Res. Int. 2015, 78, 96–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Heenan, S.P.; Hamid, N.; Dufour, J.-P.; Harvey, W.; Delahunty, C.M. Consumer freshness perceptions of breads, biscuits and cakes. Food Qual. Pref. 2009, 20, 380–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Sipos, L.; Nyitrai, Á.; Hitka, G.; Friedrich, L.F.; Kókai, Z. Sensory Panel Performance Evaluation—Comprehensive Review of Practical Approaches. Appl. Sci. 2021, 11, 11977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Hanks, L.; Line, N.D. The restaurant social servicescape: Establishing a nomological framework. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2018, 74, 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Thamke, I.; Dürrschmid, K.; Rohm, H. Sensory description of dark chocolates by consumers. LWT-Food Sci. Technol. 2009, 42, 534–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Taljaard, I. Exploring the Relationship between Selected Internal Consumer Behaviour Variables and the Emotional Response to the Sensory Properties of Chocolate. Ph.D. Thesis, North-West University, Potchefstroom, South Africa, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  54. Chaya, C.; Eaton, C.; Hewson, L.; Vázquez, R.F.; Fernández-Ruiz, V.; Smart, K.A.; Hort, J. Developing a reduced consumer-led lexicon to measure emotional response to beer. Food Qual. Pref. 2015, 45, 100–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Macht, M.; Dettmer, D. Everyday mood and emotions after eating a chocolate bar or an apple. Appetite 2006, 46, 332–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Zhou, Y.; Tse, C.-S. Sweet taste brings happiness, but happiness does not taste sweet: The unidirectionality of taste-emotion metaphoric association. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2022, 34, 339–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Biguzzi, C.; Schlich, P.; Lange, C. The impact of sugar and fat reduction on perception and liking of biscuits. Food Qual. Pref. 2014, 35, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. McIntyre, C.; Schwanke, B. Biscuit (cookie) consumption. Cognitive suspension to experience moments of perfection in another world than this! Br. Food J. 2010, 112, 853–870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Danone Report. Food Waste. The large-Scale Consumer Study for Germany, Austria and Switzerland. 2021. Available online: https://www.danone.de/content/dam/danone-corp/danone-dach/bilder-en/danone-weltweit/danone-und-too-good-to-go-food-waste-studie-chart-report-eng.pdf (accessed on 15 June 2022).
  60. DuPont. Sweet Baked Goods Consumption in South Africa. Available online: https://www.food.dupont.com/content/dam/dupont/amer/us/en/nutrition-health/general/documents/South_Africa_infographics.pdf (accessed on 15 June 2022).
  61. Hamilton, R. Favourite South African Chocolate: The Sweet Taste of Home. 2020. Available online: https://www.africanvibe.com.au/blog/favourite-south-african-chocolate-the-sweet-taste-of-home (accessed on 15 June 2022).
  62. Leroy, P.; Varga, P. The symbolic understanding of milk in Swiss gastronomy. Res. Hosp. Manag. 2019, 9, 25–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Ferreira, S.L.; Muller, R. Innovating the wine tourism product: Food-and-wine pairing in Stellenbosch wine routes. Afr. J. Phys. Health Educ. Recreat. Dance 2013, 19, 72–85. [Google Scholar]
  64. Bender, J.; Gidlow, B.; Fisher, D. National stereotypes in tourist guidebooks: An Analysis of Auto- and Hetero-stereotypes in Different Language Guidebooks about Switzerland. Ann. Tour. Res. 2013, 40, 331–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Meule, A.; Hormes, J.M. Chocolate versions of the Food Cravings Questionnaires. Associations with chocolate exposure-induced salivary flow and ad libitum chocolate consumption. Appetite 2015, 91, 256–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Chao, A.; Grilo, C.M.; White, M.A.; Sinha, R. Food cravings, food intake, and weight status in a community-based sample. Eat. Behav. 2014, 15, 478–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Keller, C.; Siegrist, M. Does personality influence eating styles and food choices? Direct and indirect effects. Appetite 2015, 84, 128–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Keller, C.; Siegrist, M. Ambivalence toward palatable food and emotional eating predict weight fluctuations. Results of a longitudinal study with four waves. Appetite 2015, 85, 138–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Keller, C.; Hartmann, C. Not merely a question of self-control: The longitudinal effects of overeating behaviors, diet quality and physical activity on dieters’ perceived diet success. Appetite 2016, 107, 213–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Dalenberg, J.; Gutjar, S.; ter Horst, G.; de Graaf, K.; Renken, R.; Friese, M. Evoked emotions predict food choice. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e115388. [Google Scholar]
  71. Banna, J.C.; Gilliland, B.; Keefe, M.; Zheng, D. Cross-cultural comparison of perspectives on healthy eating among Chinese and American undergraduate students. BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Ahrens, W. Sensory taste preferences and taste sensitivity and the association of unhealthy food patterns with overweight and obesity in primary school children in Europe—A synthesis of data from the IDEFICS study. Flavour 2015, 4, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Troisi, J.D.; Gabriel, S. Chicken soup really is good for the soul: “Comfort food” fulfills the need to belong. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 22, 747–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Akpan, B. Classical and Operant Conditioning—Ivan Pavlov; Burrhus Skinner. In Science Education in Theory and Practice; Springer Texts in Education; Akpan, B., Kennedy, T.J., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2020. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Demographical characteristics of the study sample (n = 216).
Table 1. Demographical characteristics of the study sample (n = 216).
CharacteristicsSouth Africa Switzerland
Gender
 Male24.5%46.4%
 Female75.5%53.6%
Age
 Under 35 years24.1%25.0%
 Above 35 years81.9%85.0%
Education level SA
 Grade 12 or below22.6%
 Above Grade 12 77.4%
Education level Swiss
 Middle school or below 41.8%
 Above middle school 58.2%
Monthly income
 Less than R20,00037.7%
 More than R20,00047.2%
 Preference not to disclose information13.2%
Monthly income
 Less than CHF 6000 20.9%
 More than CHF 6000 57.3%
 Preference not to disclose information 23.6%
Marital status
 Single/widowed/divorced 38.7%19.1%
 Married/living with a partner 61.3%80.9%
Occupational status
 Working94.3%30.9%
 Not working/on pension 5.7%69.1%
Geographic region
 City 85.4%54.5%
 Countryside 14.6%45.5%
Table 2. Mean and standard deviation (m ± sd) values of consumers’ emotions before biscuit consumption.
Table 2. Mean and standard deviation (m ± sd) values of consumers’ emotions before biscuit consumption.
ItemsSouth AfricaSwitzerland
Positive emotions
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.834)
3.03 ± 1.173.02 ± 0.94
 Adventurous2.49 ± 1.202.01 ± 1.04
 Calm3.49 ± 1.063.49 ± 0.96
 Enthusiastic2.91 ± 1.102.02 ± 1.04
 Free2.78 ± 1.393.37 ± 1.07
 Good3.32 ± 1.103.63 ± 0.82
 Happy3.52 ± 1.093.36 ± 0.88
 Joyful3.29 ± 1.273.07 ± 0.98
 Nostalgic2.05 ± 1.151.86 ± 0.95
 Satisfied3.12 ± 1.143.64 ± 0.84
 Secure3.33 ± 1.203.72 ± 0.78
Negative emotions
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.664)
1.71 ± 1.101.50 ± 0.73
 Aggressive1.56 ± 1.001.23 ± 0.57
 Guilty1.52 ± 1.041.18 ± 0.56
 Bored1.74 ± 1.031.59 ± 0.76
 Worried2.02 ± 1.341.98 ± 1.02
Note: SA = South Africa; measured on a five-point Likert-type scale; m = mean; sd = standard deviation.
Table 3. Mean and standard deviation (m ± sd) values of consumers’ emotions after biscuit consumption.
Table 3. Mean and standard deviation (m ± sd) values of consumers’ emotions after biscuit consumption.
ItemsSouth AfricaSwitzerland
Positive emotions
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.900)
2.57 ± 1.202.52 ± 1.04
 Adventurous1.90 ± 1.291.46 ± 0.81
 Calm3.00 ± 1.173.27 ± 0.99
 Enthusiastic2.38 ± 1.321.62 ± 0.88
 Free2.52 ± 1.272.65 ± 1.22
 Good3.06 ± 1.193.25 ± 0.98
 Happy2.97 ± 1.172.66 ± 1.15
 Joyful2.75 ± 1.232.48 ± 1.11
 Nostalgic1.77 ± 0.991.82 ± 1.05
 Satisfied2.92 ± 1.073.10 ± 1.00
 Secure2.42 ± 1.342.92 ± 1.24
Negative emotions
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.562)
1.42 ± 0.821.34 ± 0.65
 Aggressive1.24 ± 0.681.11 ± 0.39
 Guilty1.69 ± 1.121.26 ± 0.66
 Bored1.43 ± 0.701.51 ± 0.78
 Worried1.30 ± 0.761.47 ± 0.76
Note: SA = South Africa; measured on a five-point Likert-type scale; m = mean; sd = standard deviation.
Table 4. Mean and standard deviation (m ± sd) values of the sensory properties of biscuits.
Table 4. Mean and standard deviation (m ± sd) values of the sensory properties of biscuits.
ItemsSouth AfricaSwitzerland
Sensory taste properties (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.707)2.69 ± 1.072.46 ± 0.87
 Chocolate3.80 ± 1.063.49 ± 0.82
 Vanilla2.48 ± 1.111.80 ± 0.87
 Buttery2.82 ± 1.132.37 ± 0.94
 Sweet3.65 ± 0.933.68 ± 0.82
 Caramel1.81 ± 1.111.77 ± 0.91
 Nutty1.58 ± 1.071.63 ± 0.86
Note: SA = South Africa; measured on a five-point Likert-type scale; m = mean; sd = standard deviation.
Table 5. Mean and standard deviation (m ± sd) values of consumers’ attitudes towards biscuits.
Table 5. Mean and standard deviation (m ± sd) values of consumers’ attitudes towards biscuits.
ItemsSouth AfricaSwitzerland
Attitude Craving (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.872)2.85 ± 1.172.26 ± 1.09
 I eat sweet baked products to cheer me up when I am down2.91 ± 1.322.34 ± 1.22
 I often eat sweet baked products when I am bored2.83 ± 1.272.37 ± 1.19
 I like to indulge in sweet baked products2.33 ± 1.091.72 ± 0.99
 My desire for sweet baked products often seems overpowering3.43 ± 1.072.69 ± 1.32
 The thought of sweet baked products often distracts me from what I am doing (e.g., watching TV) 2.42 ± 1.222.38 ± 1.15
 I usually find myself wanting sweet baked products during the afternoon2.36 ± 1.241.50 ± 0.75
 I often go into a shop for something else and end up buying sweet baked products2.82 ± 1.172.09 ± 1.08
 Sweet baked products often prey on my mind3.29 ± 1.062.05 ± 1.12
 Even when I do not really want any more sweet baked products I will often carry on eating it3.22 ± 1.153.35 ± 1.02
 Attitude Guilt (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.905)2.50 ± 1.232.07 ± 1.01
 I feel unattractive after I have eaten sweet baked products2.36 ± 1.271.78 ± 1.03
 I often feel sick after eating sweet baked products2.26 ± 1.211.48 ± 0.73
 I am often on one kind of diet or another2.52 ± 1.271.62 ± 1.03
 I consider sweet baked products to be high in fat and of poor nutritional value3.32 ± 1.233.65 ± 1.15
 After eating sweet baked products, I often wish I hadn’t2.75 ± 1.452.37 ± 1.15
 I feel guilty after eating sweet baked products2.41 ± 1.311.90 ± 1.03
 I feel depressed and dissatisfied with life after eating sweet baked products2.61 ± 1.281.96 ± 0.97
 I feel unhealthy after I have eaten sweet baked products2.11 ± 0.971.93 ± 1.05
 I always look at the calorie value of a sweet baked product before I eat it2.63 ± 1.232.30 ± 1.11
 If I resist the temptation to eat sweet baked Products, I feel more in control of my life2.08 ± 1.131.74 ± 0.88
Note: SA = South Africa; measured on a five-point Likert-type scale; sd = standard deviation.
Table 6. Summary of regression analysis for variables predicting consumers’ emotions after biscuit consumption.
Table 6. Summary of regression analysis for variables predicting consumers’ emotions after biscuit consumption.
BSE Bβp
Positive emotions
SA (R2 = 0.54)
 Constant−0.100.37 0.79
 Vanilla taste0.270.070.320.00 **
 Chocolate taste0.250.070.290.00 **
 Caramel taste0.230.070.270.00 **
 Attitude: craving0.370.090.310.00 **
 Attitude: guilt−0.170.08−0.170.03 *
Swiss (R2 = 0.10)
 Constant1.640.33 0.00 **
 Chocolate taste0.240.09.270.01 *
Negative emotions
SA (R2 = 0.40)
 Constant0.250.26 0.22
 Nutty taste0.340.060.440.00 **
 Caramel taste0.270.060.420.00 **
 Buttery taste−0.170.06−0.270.00 **
 Sweet taste0.190.060.240.00 **
 Attitude: craving0.240.070.270.00 **
 Attitude: guilt0.270.070.250.00 **
 Gender0.250.130.130.01 *
 Age−0.390.13−0.230.01 *
 Education0.520.120.320.00 **
Swiss (R2 = 0.20)
 Constant0.870.23 0.00 **
 Vanilla taste0.140.040.310.00 **
 Chocolate taste−0.170.08−0.200.03 *
 Nutty taste0.230.090.250.01 *
 Attitude: guilt0.350.100.350.00 **
 Gender0.180.070.240.01 *
 Income−0.330.13−0.260.01 *
Note: SA = South Africa; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.001. B = unstandardized coefficient; SE B = standard error; β = standardized coefficient; p = significance level.
Table 7. Summary of regression analysis for the variables predicting consumers’ emotional changes.
Table 7. Summary of regression analysis for the variables predicting consumers’ emotional changes.
BSE Bβp
Positive change
SA (R2 = 0.39)
 Constant−3.680.42 0.00 **
 Chocolate taste 0.240.070.270.00 **
 Vanilla taste0.200.070.230.01 *
 Attitude: craving0.530.100.430.00 **
Swiss (R2 = 0.24)
 Constant−1.730.34 0.00 **
 Chocolate taste0.300.090.330.00 **
 Attitude: craving0.220.090.230.02 *
 Residence area−0.310.13−0.220.02 *
Negative change
SA (R2 = 0.21)
 Constant−0.680.430.000.21
 Buttery taste0.290.100.270.03 *
 Nutty taste−0.070.11−0.030.01 *
 Vanilla taste0.330.120.270.01 *
 Chocolate taste−0.360.11−0.280.00 **
 Attitude: guilt0.190.120.090.00 **
 Attitude: craving−0.430.16−0.230.01 *
 Age−0.590.25−0.200.02 *
 Income0.780.25 0.280.00 **
Swiss (R2 = 0.14)
 Constant0.150.21 0.000.12
 Nutty taste−0.280.10−0.270.01 *
 Chocolate taste−0.180.07−0.240.01 *
 Caramel taste−0.210.08−0.250.02 *
 Attitude: guilt0.310.080.380.00 **
 Income−0.330.10−0.310.00 **
 Residence area−0.250.09−0.280.01 *
 Occupational status0.440.19 0.230.02 *
Note: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.001; B = unstandardized coefficient; SE B = standard error; β = standardized coefficient; p = significance level.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mielmann, A.; Brunner, T.A. Linking the Sensory Taste Properties of Chocolate-Based Biscuits to Consumers’ Emotions: A Cross-Cultural Study. Appl. Sci. 2022, 12, 8038. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12168038

AMA Style

Mielmann A, Brunner TA. Linking the Sensory Taste Properties of Chocolate-Based Biscuits to Consumers’ Emotions: A Cross-Cultural Study. Applied Sciences. 2022; 12(16):8038. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12168038

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mielmann, Annchen, and Thomas A. Brunner. 2022. "Linking the Sensory Taste Properties of Chocolate-Based Biscuits to Consumers’ Emotions: A Cross-Cultural Study" Applied Sciences 12, no. 16: 8038. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12168038

APA Style

Mielmann, A., & Brunner, T. A. (2022). Linking the Sensory Taste Properties of Chocolate-Based Biscuits to Consumers’ Emotions: A Cross-Cultural Study. Applied Sciences, 12(16), 8038. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12168038

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop