Next Article in Journal
Early Mortality of Brain Infarction Patients and Red Blood Cell Distribution Width
Next Article in Special Issue
7,8-Dihydroxyflavone Enhances Cue-Conditioned Alcohol Reinstatement in Rats
Previous Article in Journal
Effects of Oral Contraceptive Androgenicity on Visuospatial and Social-Emotional Cognition: A Prospective Observational Trial
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

The BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism Modulates Resilience of Neurological Functioning to Brain Ageing and Dementia: A Narrative Review

by
Donnamay T. Brown
1,
James C. Vickers
1,
Kimberley E. Stuart
1,
Katerina Cechova
2,3 and
David D. Ward
1,4,5,*
1
Wicking Dementia Research and Education Centre, College of Health and Medicine, University of Tasmania, Hobart 7001, Tasmania, Australia
2
Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, 2nd Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and Motol University Hospital, 15006 Prague, Czech Republic
3
International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne’s University Hospital Brno, 65691 Brno, Czech Republic
4
Division of Geriatric Medicine, Department of Medicine, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS B3H 2E1, Canada
5
Centre for Health Care of the Elderly, QEII Health Sciences Centre, Nova Scotia Health Authority, Halifax, NS B3H 2E1, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2020, 10(4), 195; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10040195
Submission received: 26 February 2020 / Revised: 19 March 2020 / Accepted: 24 March 2020 / Published: 25 March 2020

Abstract

:
Brain-derived neurotropic factor (BDNF) is an abundant and multi-function neurotrophin in the brain. It is released following neuronal activity and is believed to be particularly important in strengthening neural networks. A common variation in the BDNF gene, a valine to methionine substitution at codon 66 (Val66Met), has been linked to differential expression of BDNF associated with experience-dependent plasticity. The Met allele has been associated with reduced production of BDNF following neuronal stimulation, which suggests a potential role of this variation with respect to how the nervous system may respond to challenges, such as brain ageing and related neurodegenerative conditions (e.g., dementia and Alzheimer’s disease). The current review examines the potential of the BDNF Val66Met variation to modulate an individual’s susceptibility and trajectory through cognitive changes associated with ageing and dementia. On balance, research to date indicates that the BDNF Met allele at this codon is potentially associated with a detrimental influence on the level of cognitive functioning in older adults and may also impart increased risk of progression to dementia. Furthermore, recent studies also show that this genetic variation may modulate an individual’s response to interventions targeted at building cognitive resilience to conditions that cause dementia.

1. Introduction

Neurotrophins are critical for cellular development, connectivity, plasticity and maintenance in the brain. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), the most abundant neurotrophin, has received significant attention due to a variety of important roles in axonal and dendritic growth and guidance, synaptogenesis, as well as experience-dependent plasticity in adult animal models [1,2,3]. BDNF has been observed secreting from dendrite to axon, from axon to dendrite, in autocrine loops, in paracrine interactions with neighbouring cells and across long distances through neural circuits [2,4]. BDNF may also have a role in the refinement of active neural pathways through activity-dependent strengthening of co-active synapse terminals and elimination of inactive terminals [3]. Temporally, BDNF works within seconds of release to influence synaptic function, over minutes to modify synaptic structure, and over hours to days to change genetic expression and protein synthesis [3]. In this regard, BDNF has a broad influence on both functional and structural forms of neural plasticity throughout the life course (for review, see [5]).
The ‘mature’ BDNF protein is derived from a larger pro-BDNF form and it is yet uncertain whether both the mature and the immature forms of this protein are secreted as a consequence of neuronal activity [6]. Recently, it has been shown that BDNF maturation is a key process for synaptic plasticity. BDNF binds to TrkB receptor and via this pathway promotes dendritic growth, spine density and long-term potentiation [5]. Activation of the pro-BDNF p75NTR receptor reduces neuroplasticity and facilitates long-term depression [7,8]. In this regard, BDNF appears to have a strong mediating role in both long-term potentiation and long-term depression, as well as in influencing broader morphological changes in neurons, likely by signalling changes in actin to effect remodelling [5]. BDNF-immunolabelled cells are present in all cortical regions in both neurons and glia, with a relatively higher density in the insular and temporal cortices, including the hippocampus, basal ganglia and amygdala, in addition to certain frontal regions, and have been observed in white matter adjacent to the cortex [2,9].
The gene for BDNF is located on chromosome 11p14.1 and demonstrates a variety of natural variations in the human population. Of particular focus for this review is the single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) at nucleotide 196 in exon 5 of the human BDNF gene, involving a guanine to adenine variation that results in the substitution of valine (Val) to methionine (Met). This SNP (rs6265), widely known as the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism, has received substantial attention, as hippocampal neurons transfected with the Met version of the SNP express 30% less secretion of BDNF protein upon stimulation than neurons transfected with the Val version, with no differences in constitutive release between these variants [10]. Early reports indicated that BDNF Met was associated with reduced localization with synaptic markers and reduced trafficking in transfected cells, lower levels of the metabolic marker n-acetyl aspartate in the human hippocampus, and abnormal medial temporal lobe activation in the N-back working memory task [10,11]. A range of functional deficits and increased anxiety behaviours have been demonstrated in transgenic animal models where the Met version of the BDNF gene is expressed [12,13,14]. Studies have demonstrated that mice transfected with two BDNF Met alleles show reduced N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor-mediated long-term potentiation and long-term depression in hippocampal and infralimbic medial prefrontal cortex synapses [14,15]. The function of the NMDA receptor has been associated with memory and cognition, particularly within the ageing brain [16,17].
Overall, it is assumed that the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism leads to a deficit in normal BDNF expression and impaired synaptic plasticity through multiple pathways, particularly in the context of activity-dependent functions. Given the critical role of BDNF in neural processes, BDNF Val66Met may affect brain and cognitive functioning in the context of ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Furthermore, given its ties with activity-dependent secretion of BDNF, it is possible that physical activity and cognitive activity may be important modulators to consider. The aim of this review was to examine the potential of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism to alter an individual’s susceptibility to brain ageing and dementia and to further identify pathways through which these effects may be conferred.

2. Methods

Articles published up until January 2020 were considered for inclusion. Literature searches were conducted using the PubMed and Google Scholar databases. Key search queries included combinations of the following terms: ‘BDNF’, ‘Val66Met’, ‘A196G’, ‘cognition’, ‘cognitive function’, ‘cognitive functioning’, ‘cognitive decline’, ‘Alzheimer’s disease’, ‘dementia’, ‘cognitive reserve’, ‘physical activity’, ‘exercise’, ‘brain structure’, ‘brain volume’, ‘brain function’, ‘APOE’, and ‘apolipoprotein E’. In addition to these key words, additional search terms of possible relevance were continuously identified from articles identified through database searches. We included articles published in English that focused on both in vivo and in vitro studies of humans and animals. Reference lists of identified articles were also searched to aid in the identification of additional articles of high relevance to the topic.

3. Influence of the BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism on Brain Structure and Function

Reduced [18] and less efficient [10] hippocampal activation has been reported during memory tasks in healthy young adults who carry BDNF Met. Later studies of healthy older adults identified that the presence of BDNF Met was associated with a significant reduction in medial temporal lobe activation during the encoding phase of a memory task [19]. Relative to the Val allele, the Met allele has also been linked with reduced neuronal activity in the medial prefrontal cortex in both rodent and human studies [14,20], as well as increased amygdala activation in response to emotional stimuli in human fMRI investigations [21].
In humans, possession of the Met allele has been associated with reduced grey matter in areas of the mid-frontal regions of the brain, fusiform gyrus, amygdala and thalamus [22,23]. In addition, meta-analyses have linked the Met allele to reduced grey matter volume of the hippocampus [24,25]. However, a more recent and larger meta-analysis did not support a link between the polymorphism and hippocampal volume in healthy individuals, indicating that previous associations may have been limited by imaging volume technique and sample size [26]. Ultimately, any association of BDNF Val66Met variants with grey matter volume may be, in part, due to developmental effects exerted by the polymorphism, with a recent longitudinal investigation identifying subtle and differential effects of the Met and Val variants on the maturation of grey matter volume in children and young adults [27].
Mixed findings have been reported regarding the relationship between BDNF Val66Met and white matter volume and activity, with most studies limited by small to moderate sample sizes. One of the larger studies of 455 adult twins and non-twin siblings reported that the Val allele was associated with a reduction in microstructural integrity of major fibre tracks in various frontal, midbrain and occipital regions, measured by fractional anisotropy [28]. This finding was replicated and further developed in a study that also included radial diffusivity: lower diffusivity was linked to reduced myelination, which was observed in healthy, young Val homozygotes [29]. Notably, these authors suggested that reduced fractional anisotropy and diffusivity can be linked to reduced activity, as well as increased efficiency of these brain regions, thus implying the results may not actually be of a detrimental effect for Val homozygotes. Associations of BDNF Val66Met and markers of white matter health or development are, however, inconsistent, with another study finding no relationship between BDNF Val66Met and white matter volume or integrity in a sample of 99 young adults [30]. In addition, one study used graph theory to investigate the effect of Val66Met on white matter structural networks in 73 adults (aged in their 30s and 40s) [31]. They found no differences with respect to global efficiency, local efficiency and modularity between Val and Met allele groups. Furthermore, responses to random failures were equal in both genotype groups. However, when targeted attacks to central nodes were modelled, Met carriers were significantly more vulnerable, resulting in reduced connectivity of the white matter structural network [31].

4. BDNF Val66Met and Cognitive Function

A diverse and not always consistent range of findings regarding the impact of BDNF Val66Met on cognitive domains has been reported. A number of studies have reported that healthy, young carriers and homozygotes of BDNF Met demonstrate poorer declarative memory, specifically episodic memory [10,18,32,33]. However, statistically significant findings in this domain are not consistently reported [34,35], and it appears that more robust relationships between the polymorphism and cognitive function have been found in samples containing cognitively vulnerable groups, such as older individuals (e.g., [36]) or individuals with schizophrenia (e.g., [10,32]). A recent meta-analysis of almost 6000 participants, comprising a mixed cohort of healthy, psychiatric, younger and older participants, confirmed that Met carriers performed more poorly on declarative memory tasks relative to Val homozygotes [25]. However, when including only the healthy cohorts, this effect became a non-significant trend.
The BDNF Met allele has also been implicated in cognitive deficits outside of memory, including reduced visuospatial function performance in individuals with schizophrenia [32,37] and executive function performance in older adults [38]. However, results regarding the relationship between BDNF Val66Met and these cognitive functions are also not consistent. For example, a large meta-analysis of 7000 heterogeneous participants reported no link between the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and cognitive functions such as executive functioning, cognitive fluency and general intellectual functioning [39]. Conversely, a study using an auditory distraction paradigm—different, methodologically, from standard cognitive testing—has shown that BDNF Val homozygosity may be disadvantageous when inhibitory mechanisms are required, indicating that increased activation associated with the Val allele may not be advantageous for inhibitory effects processed in frontal pathways [40]. A recent systematic review on the topic has confirmed that BDNF Val66Met likely has some impact on cognitive function, although the specific direction is difficult to ascertain [41]. Additionally, Toh and colleagues [41] suggest that Met may be advantageous for executive functioning, although broader results are not consistent.
The potential influence of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on cognition may be age-specific. For example, the determinantal influence of BDNF Met on episodic memory, as well as perceptual speed and executive functioning, is typically larger at older ages [42]. In a study of 116 healthy adults aged from 20 to 93 years, the negative association of age and test performance in item and prospective memory was stronger in BDNF Met carriers than in Val homozygotes [36]. Other areas of cognitive function may also be exacerbated in older Met carriers. A longitudinal study involving older individuals on perceptual speed tasks over a span of 13 years [43] identified that carriage of the BDNF Met allele was associated with a steeper age-related decline in this cognitive ability. Relatively decreased perceptual speed has also been reported in older Met-carrying women [44]. The Met allele has also been implicated in poorer processing speed, delayed recall and general intelligence among elderly individuals [45].
Physiological evidence indicates that older individuals with a BDNF Met allele experience decreased hippocampal activation when completing memory tasks, particularly during the encoding process, relative to that of Val homozygotes [19,46]. Changes in brain white matter, specifically the posterior of the corpus callosum, also feature a significant interaction effect of age and the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism, with older Met carriers possessing a less dense structure [47], although this is not consistently reported [19]. It has been suggested that BDNF expression decreases with age, which may partly account for older-age cognitive and neurological phenotypes [48]. This ageing-related decline in BDNF availability may be further exacerbated by the Met allele, which is associated with less BDNF activity-dependent secretion [10]. However, null and inverse findings regarding the cognitive implications of BDNF Val66Met variants have also been reported. For example, an examination of BDNF Val66Met and performance across multiple cognitive domains in a sample of older Chinese men found no difference between Val/Val, Val/Met or Met/Met groups [49]. This study may, however, have been underpowered to detect the small effects that have been reported elsewhere (n = 161). In a longitudinal study of older adults, Met carriers performed more poorly on a task of executive function at study entry but did not exhibit a decline in performance, which was present in Val homozygotes, over the 10-year follow-up period [50]. Enhanced executive function in Met carriers, particularly in inhibitory tasks, has also been reported in other studies [51,52]. One study found this positive Met effect for executive functioning in a cohort with an average age of 79 but not in their second cohort with an average age 64 [53]. Consistent with what is found in young adults, BDNF Val or Met alleles’ positive or negative effects on cognitive performance in older subjects may depend on the specific task requirements and goals.
Taken together, the current state of evidence indicates that in older adults without clinically significant cognitive impairment the Met allele may be somewhat detrimental to certain cognitive functions and not associated with performance in others.

5. Exploring the Link Between the BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism and Dementia Due to Alzheimer’s Disease

The rapidly increasing number of people with dementia throughout the world presents an immense health, social and economic challenge, for which we currently have no evidence-based capacity to prevent, halt or cure. Many of the neurodegenerative diseases that cause dementia, such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD), are characterised by the development of specific pathological features and the progressive loss of subsets of neurons and their connections [54]. Given the significant role that BDNF exhibits in the support and survival of specific neuronal populations, including those affected in AD, BDNF may too play an important role in the pathophysiology of AD.
BDNF and its receptor TrKB are both reduced in expression in the AD brain [55], with BDNF levels being most significantly reduced in the hippocampus and parietal cortex [56]. Levels of circulating BDNF protein can be established from blood samples and have been reported to be reduced in psychiatric conditions [57] as well as in individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI; [58]), the latter proposed to be a potential precursor state for AD. However, varied reports of increased [59], decreased [60] and unchanged [61] BDNF levels in neurodegenerative conditions such as AD have been found. Increased BDNF in serum has been linked with specific memory deficits in AD [62], and decreased BDNF has been associated with the presence of APOE ε4 and apathy in subsets of AD [63]. In a prospective study of 2131 older individuals without dementia, higher serum BDNF level, but not the BDNF Val66Met genotype, was associated with a reduced risk of subsequently developing dementia [64]. Interestingly, subsequent subgroup analysis in this investigation showed that the association of high BDNF levels and lower risk of dementia was found solely in women with a college degree. However, others have found the BDNF genotype does play a role in the cognitive performance of individuals at higher risk of developing dementia. High amyloid-beta load in the brains of cognitively normal older BDNF Met carriers, relative to Val homozygotes, has been linked with worse episodic memory and executive function [65] and with a steeper trajectory of cognitive decline over time [66]. In a similar study of older people without dementia, carriage of the Met allele was associated with greater entorhinal cortex atrophy and lower Mini-Mental State Examination scores specifically in those individuals with amyloid positivity only [67].
Relatedly, greater hippocampal atrophy over a three-year period has been reported in Met carriers who had higher levels of AD-related pathology [65], and, in autosomal-dominant AD, Met carriers have significantly worse memory performance, lower hippocampal metabolism and increased levels of pathological tau [68]. Such findings may explain why inheritance of a Met allele is associated with a higher risk of conversion from MCI to AD [69]. In cohorts including subjective memory decline, MCI and dementia, the detrimental effects of amyloid positivity (for example, from fluid biomarkers or PET imaging) on brain connectivity (hippocampus to medial/frontal connectivity) were higher in BDNF Met carriers [70]. Finally, in older adults possessing gene mutations linked to dominantly inherited AD but without dementia, BDNF Met carriage was associated with more substantial memory decline and hippocampal volume loss, as well as increased cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) tau levels, relative to Val homozygotes [71].
It is, however, important to acknowledge that contradictory findings have also been reported with regard to the associations of BDNF Val66Met and risk for dementia. For instance, in a Japanese sample of 487 participants with AD and 471 matched controls, BDNF Val homozygosity was observed more commonly among the participants with AD [72]. Similar results with regard to a heightened AD risk incurred from BDNF Val have been reported from within an Italian sample of Caucasians [73]. Specifically, a BDNF Val allele was more frequently observed within AD patients than among healthy volunteers. Such results are further consistent with those of a Hungarian sample, in which BDNF Val homozygosity was present in 59% of AD cases but only 32% of healthy controls [74]. Some degree of the inconsistencies in reported relationships between BDNF Val66Met and AD are likely due to differences in the diagnostic criteria used for AD and differences in population distributions of BDNF Val66Met alleles.
Thus, although the relationship between serum BDNF and AD symptomology may be unclear, current research suggests that inheritance of BDNF Met leads to more severe or more rapidly progressing cognitive deficits in the presence of AD pathology, in most cases. However, the presence of a BDNF Met allele does not appear to directly and independently affect an individual’s risk for dementia. These studies raise the prospect that many effects of the BDNF polymorphism in ‘healthy’ older cohorts, as described above, may have been influenced by the presence of sub-clinical AD and other neurodegenerative pathology.

6. Interactions between the APOE Gene and the BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism

Age and family history are the major risk factors for dementia, with risk of AD rising exponentially after the age of 65 years. The most significant non-mutation genetic risk factor for AD is a common variant in the APOE gene. Specifically, the presence of one or more APOE ε4 alleles increases the risk of AD in older adults in a dose-related fashion [75,76]. With regard to brain structure and function, a growing body of evidence reports interactions between APOE and BDNF Val66Met variants.
In one study of healthy older adults, an interaction effect of BDNF Val66Met and APOE was identified, in which the presence of both of the compromising alleles (Met and ε4) was linked to significantly reduced hippocampal activation during memory tasks [77]. Other imaging studies have found that carriers of both BDNF Met and APOE ε4 possess a lack of neural compensatory capacity when challenged by AD [78]. Specifically, during ageing, the thickness of the precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex decreases more markedly in BDNF Met carriers, and the BDNF Met/APOE ε4 combination is associated with steeper entorhinal cortex atrophy in MCI/AD cases, as well as steeper performance decline on memory tests [78]. The combination of amyloid-beta deposits in the brain with both APOE ε4 and BDNF Met has been related to the steepest declines in memory and language cognitive domains in older individuals without dementia [79]. This latter study estimated BDNF Met carriers with the APOE ε4 allele would have clinically significant cognitive impairment in 3 years, as compared to 10 years if the individual was an APOE ε4 carrier and BDNF Val homozygote. Furthermore, a protective effect of BDNF Val on episodic memory performance in older adults without dementia has been reported [80]. In this study of 433 healthy older adults, no difference in performance was observed between APOE allele types in BDNF Val homozygotes, but the protective effects of APOE ε2 and the detrimental effects of APOE ε4 were observed in BDNF Met carriers. Consistent with other findings [19,79,80], a recent study on amnestic MCI has likewise indicated that inheritance of the APOE ε4/BDNF Met combination was associated with the worst memory performance when compared to other polymorphic combinations [81].
In combination, these studies have demonstrated that an interaction between APOE and BDNF Val66Met variants likely explains more variance in brain and cognitive outcomes than each gene variant explains individually. At the very least, an additive detrimental effect of both of these at-risk allele types is exerted on memory performance, its associated brain regions and patterns of brain connectivity [82].

7. Physical Activity, BDNF, and Risk of Cognitive Decline

It has been suggested that approximately a third [83,84] to half [85] of dementia cases may be effectively preventable by addressing major modifiable factors, such as depression, mid-life hypertension, smoking, mid-life obesity, diabetes, physical inactivity and low educational attainment. Low physical activity in particular has been identified as a major population-attributable risk factor for dementia [83,84,85], as well as increasing the risk of other lifestyle-related diseases associated with increased susceptibility to dementia, including obesity, type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular diseases [86]. Consequently, there is substantial interest in whether increasing physical activity and exercise may constitute an intervention to reduce risk or progression of dementia. Meta-analyses of observational data support a role for exercise in improving cognitive performance, although the results of relevant prospective trials are mixed [87].
In animal studies, brain BDNF is increased in the hippocampus and cerebral cortex following exercise [88], as well as following environmental enrichment which also promotes physical activity (e.g., [89,90,91]). Blocking BDNF action inhibits the exercise-mediated increase in synaptic proteins [92]. Furthermore, exercise can counteract the detrimental effects of other variables that may cause a reduction in BDNF levels, such as stress [93]. In humans, peripheral BDNF is increased with a single session of exercise, with intensified session-based effects and increased resting BDNF with additional subsequent sessions [94]. However, this meta-analysis also reported that these benefits may be modulated by sex, with women experiencing a decreased effect [94]. The beneficial effects of exercise on BDNF secretion also appear to attenuate with age in rodents, with this effect reduced in older mice but still present [95], although this is yet to be clarified in human research.
Still, multiple trials have demonstrated improvements in cognitive functioning and increased levels of serum BDNF following exercise interventions. For instance, in participants with amnestic MCI, acute aerobic exercise and acute resistance training led to improved cognitive functioning compared to control in two separate trials [96,97]. In these studies, increased serum BDNF levels were additionally observed among the participants who completed the acute aerobic exercise program. In a 16-week multimodal physical exercise program involving older cognitively healthy individuals and individuals with MCI, physical training reduced blood-based pro-inflammatory markers, increased BDNF plasma levels and improved cognitive functioning among those with MCI [98]. While upregulation of BDNF expression is presumed to account for some of the exercise-related cognitive benefits, it is interesting to note that the cognitive improvements observed after aerobic exercise in the study by Tsai and colleagues [96] were not significantly correlated with the observed increases in serum BDNF.
With respect to the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism, a longitudinal investigation involving over 700 community-based older adults demonstrated that lower baseline physical activity was linked to increased incidence of cognitive difficulties and dementia, a risk that increased incrementally with the number of Met alleles present [99]. Additionally, a longitudinal analysis following older adults over a 12-year period identified a possible sex interaction, in which physical activity was positively associated with cognitive function in male BDNF Val homozygotes but not in male Met carriers [100]. In contrast, physical activity was not associated with cognition in women of either genotype. A similar finding, that physical activity was associated with improved executive functioning and a slower decline in this cognitive domain over a 9-year period solely in older Val homozygotes, has also been reported [101]. In this study, however, sex was not a moderating factor on the relationships between BDNF Val66Met, exercise and cognitive performance. In a separate cross-sectional study, older Val homozygotes who exercised more regularly had, on average, greater volume in hippocampal and temporal regions when compared to Met carriers who exercised a similar amount [102].
Exercise has been used as an intervention to improve cognitive function in older adults, but the potential interaction with BDNF Val66Met status remains unclear. For example, one study determined that improvements in episodic memory performance following exercise were dependent on Val homozygosity [103], but Nascimento and colleagues [104] reported that BDNF Val66Met was unrelated to any cognitive outcome in their study. In a younger-to-middle-aged (30–55 years old) sample, exercise appeared to compensate for lower performance on cognitive measures of working memory in BDNF Met carriers relative to Val homozygotes [105]. In those even younger (21–35 years), no differential effect of exercise on performance in locomotor learning was linked to variation in BDNF Val66Met [106].
Although difficulties in comparing available studies on this topic are evident due to methodological differences in both exercise intervention and assessment of cognitive function, these findings indicate that age may also play a role in the interaction between BDNF Val66Met and exercise. This is in line with the resource modulation hypothesis, in that gene-related effects on cognitive function are more evident in older participants [42,100,107]. While there may be no overt effect of exercise in younger adults, middle-aged BDNF Met carriers who exercise and generally engage in more physical activity may be able to avoid a negative Met-specific cognitive phenotype, while older adults, particularly those with MCI or cognitive difficulties, may experience greater cognitive benefits from exercise-induced secretion of BDNF if they are Val homozygotes. Additional cross-sectional and longitudinal research covering a wide range of age groups measuring cognition in a consistent manner would be of further benefit to examine this hypothesis.

8. BDNF and Cognitive Reserve

Similar to physical activity, cognitive activity, often reflected by education level, has been identified as one of the major modifiable risk factors for dementia. Although a higher level of education may exert some of these protective effects through its association with socioeconomic status and greater access to healthcare, engaging in further education may also directly benefit the development, structure and function of the brain [108,109].
The cognitive reserve hypothesis posits that greater engagement in cognitively stimulating activities across the lifespan results in increased protective neural compensation and heightened neural efficiency, which results in a later onset of dementia despite having little effect on disease pathology onset or rate of accumulation [110,111]. Stern [110] suggests that individuals with higher cognitive reserve have, on average, higher levels of neuropathology prior to experiencing behavioural changes, displaying resilience to the effects of the pathology. This has subsequently been supported by evidence from a range of studies, in which greater brain connectivity and maintenance of cognitive function have been directly associated with cognitive reserve variables in the presence of MCI and AD [112,113]. The effects of cognitive reserve are not restricted to pathological functioning, and the level of cognitive reserve correlates positively with performance in a range of cognitive domains in healthy older adults [114]. This cross-sectional investigation also reported that a positive association of cognitive reserve and executive functioning was present in BDNF Val homozygotes but not in Met carriers. In a subsequent 36-month longitudinal investigation of this same cohort, cognitive reserve was associated with the rate of change in executive functioning only when the interaction with BDNF Val66Met was considered, with the low-cognitive-reserve/BDNF Met carrier participants the sole group to show the beginnings of cognitive decline [115].
Similar to physical activity, emerging evidence also points to a role of cognitive activity in stimulating the release of BDNF protein [116]. In this small-scale intervention study of older women with MCI (N = 44), the authors observed a parallel increase in both serum BDNF level and cognitive performance following 24 sessions of computerised mental training. The role of the BDNF polymorphism was analysed in a similar study assessing the cognitive outcomes of a computerised brain-training program in 27 older adults who had experienced heart failure [117]. In this investigation, although serum BDNF levels increased following the intervention, no differences in BDNF levels or cognitive performance were observed due to differing BDNF Val66Met variants. Nonetheless, with a small effect size expected with regard to the modulatory effects of BDNF Val66Met on cognitive and serum outcomes following cognitive stimulation, these investigations are likely underpowered to find support, or otherwise, for such a hypothesis.
Relatedly, this BDNF genotype may also impact interventions designed to improve or stabilise cognitive function and cognitive reserve, potentially acting as a bulwark against neurodegenerative conditions such as dementia. The Tasmanian Health Brain Project is a long-term pragmatic interventional study that is investigating the potential cognitive benefits for older adults (50–79 years of age) re-engaging in formal education. This study has found that across all older adults, the cognitive domain of language processing benefits most from engagement in university education [118]. Furthermore, the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism may influence who may benefit from such interventions, with Val homozygotes deriving less benefit than Met carriers [119]. Specifically, Met carriers who were not engaged in education showed a decline in language processing function over time, whereas Met carriers in the intervention group gained a benefit in this cognitive domain in a dose-related fashion relative to the amount of study that they had undertaken.

9. Summary and Limitations

The BDNF protein plays an important role in mediating the interaction between the external environment and internal neurological function and capacity. Met allele carriers for the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism may represent a ‘vulnerable’ group in the face of a brain challenge such as a major neurological disease like AD. From animal studies and investigations of human brain plasticity, the Met allele version appears to be associated with a somewhat deficient plasticity response to experience. However, some of these effects may be direct or indirect and potentially too subtle to be meaningful for future translational studies. In our studies, the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism may be influencing how an individual is accessing the benefits of a lifetime of exposure to cognitively stimulating experiences to build cognitive reserve and other areas of cognitive capacity that may help reduce susceptibility to dementia [114,115,119]. In this regard, it may be vital that people with the vulnerable genotype are engaged in mentally stimulating activities serving as a bulwark against dementia. Specific genetic combinations, such as that of APOE and BDNF Val66Met, may further identify cognitively vulnerable people who may be particularly advised to consider how they can reduce their risk of dementia outside of the influence of ageing and genetic inheritance. Although not reported here, the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism has also been linked to a deleterious response to stress and trauma [120], which may play a role in cognition and subsequently dementia, given the relation between neurodegenerative changes and an abnormal stress response in AD [121].
Although much of the research has focused on BDNF Val66Met allele variation, Chen and colleagues [12] note that it is possible that some of these effects could be related to another component of the BDNF gene or that BDNF Val66Met is mediating a downstream effect or the effect of another gene. Many polymorphisms in the BDNF gene have been identified, including rs11030104, rs16917204, rs7103411, rs6265 and rs2030324, which may have an interaction effect or better explain some of the variations in the levels of BDNF in certain contexts [122]. A further limitation is that most human-based studies on BDNF Val66Met often, but not always, exclusively consider either Caucasian or Asian samples, with participants excluded from analyses if not part of the majority. This is because there is potential for different patterns associated with Asian and European populations, which may be related to a unique location of the SNP in each ethnic group [123].

10. Conclusions

On balance, research to date indicates that the BDNF Met allele is potentially associated with a detrimental influence on the level of cognitive functioning in older adults and may also impart increased risk of progression to dementia through multiple pathways.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.T.B., J.C.V., K.E.S., D.D.W.; methodology, D.T.B., J.C.V., K.E.S., D.D.W., writing—original draft preparation, D.B.; writing—review and editing, D.T.B., J.C.V., K.E.S., K.C., D.D.W., supervision, J.C.V., D.D.W.; project administration, J.C.V. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The authors would like to acknowledge the funding of related research by the JO and JR Wicking Trust (Equity Trustees) as well as the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council. K.C. was supported by the Ministry of Health of the Czech Republic, grant no. 19-04-00560.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bath, K.G.; Lee, F.S. Variant BDNF (Val66Met) impact on brain structure and function. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2006, 6, 79–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Murer, M.; Yan, Q.; Raisman-Vozari, R. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the control human brain, and in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease. Prog. Neurobiol. 2001, 63, 71–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Park, H.; Poo, M.-M. Neurotrophin regulation of neural circuit development and function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2013, 14, 7–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Hartmann, M.; Heumann, R.; Lessmann, V. Synaptic secretion of BDNF after high-frequency stimulation of glutamatergic synapses. EMBO J. 2001, 20, 5887–5897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Zagrebelsky, M.; Korte, M. Form follows function: BDNF and its involvement in sculpting the function and structure of synapses. Neuropharmacology 2014, 76, 628–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Leal, G.; Afonso, P.M.; Salazar, I.L.; Duarte, C.B. Regulation of hippocampal synaptic plasticity by BDNF. Brain Res. 2015, 1621, 82–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Yang, J.; Harte-Hargrove, L.C.; Siao, C.-J.; Marinic, T.; Clarke, R.; Ma, Q.; Jing, D.; LaFrancois, J.J.; Bath, K.G.; Mark, W. proBDNF negatively regulates neuronal remodeling, synaptic transmission, and synaptic plasticity in hippocampus. Cell Rep. 2014, 7, 796–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Angelucci, F.; Čechová, K.; Průša, R.; Hort, J. Amyloid beta soluble forms and plasminogen activation system in Alzheimer’s disease: Consequences on extracellular maturation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and therapeutic implications. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 2019, 25, 303–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Murer, M.G.; Raisman-Vozari, R.; Yan, Q.; Ruberg, M.; Agid, Y.; Michel, P.P. Survival factors promote BDNF protein expression in mesencephalic dopaminergic neurons. Neuroreport 1999, 10, 801–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Egan, M.F.; Kojima, M.; Callicott, J.H.; Goldberg, T.E.; Kolachana, B.S.; Bertolino, A.; Zaitsev, E.; Gold, B.; Goldman, D.; Dean, M. The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell 2003, 112, 257–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Chen, Z.-Y.; Patel, P.D.; Sant, G.; Meng, C.-X.; Teng, K.K.; Hempstead, B.L.; Lee, F.S. Variant brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)(Met66) alters the intracellular trafficking and activity-dependent secretion of wild-type BDNF in neurosecretory cells and cortical neurons. J. Neurosci. 2004, 24, 4401–4411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Chen, Z.; Bath, K.; McEwen, B.; Hempstead, B.; Lee, F. Impact of genetic variant BDNF (Val66Met) on brain structure and function. In Novartis Foundation Symposium; Novartis Foundation: Basel, Switzerland, 2008; p. 180. [Google Scholar]
  13. Chen, Z.-Y.; Jing, D.; Bath, K.G.; Ieraci, A.; Khan, T.; Siao, C.-J.; Herrera, D.G.; Toth, M.; Yang, C.; McEwen, B.S. Genetic variant BDNF (Val66Met) polymorphism alters anxiety-related behavior. Science 2006, 314, 140–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Pattwell, S.S.; Bath, K.G.; Perez-Castro, R.; Lee, F.S.; Chao, M.V.; Ninan, I. The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism impairs synaptic transmission and plasticity in the infralimbic medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2012, 32, 2410–2421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Ninan, I.; Bath, K.G.; Dagar, K.; Perez-Castro, R.; Plummer, M.R.; Lee, F.S.; Chao, M.V. The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism impairs NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 8866–8870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Lin, C.-H.; Huang, Y.-J.; Lin, C.-J.; Lane, H.-Y.; Tsai, G.E. NMDA neurotransmission dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Pharm. Des. 2014, 20, 5169–5179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Lin, C.-H.; Lane, H.-Y. The Role of N-Methyl-D-Aspartate Receptor Neurotransmission and Precision Medicine in Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia. Front. Pharmacol. 2019, 10, 540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Hariri, A.R.; Goldberg, T.E.; Mattay, V.S.; Kolachana, B.S.; Callicott, J.H.; Egan, M.F.; Weinberger, D.R. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism affects human memory-related hippocampal activity and predicts memory performance. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 6690–6694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Kauppi, K.; Nilsson, L.-G.; Adolfsson, R.; Lundquist, A.; Eriksson, E.; Nyberg, L. Decreased medial temporal lobe activation in BDNF 66Met allele carriers during memory encoding. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51, 2462–2468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Soliman, F.; Glatt, C.E.; Bath, K.G.; Levita, L.; Jones, R.M.; Pattwell, S.S.; Jing, D.; Tottenham, N.; Amso, D.; Somerville, L.H. A genetic variant BDNF polymorphism alters extinction learning in both mouse and human. Science 2010, 327, 863–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Montag, C.; Reuter, M.; Newport, B.; Elger, C.; Weber, B. The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism affects amygdala activity in response to emotional stimuli: Evidence from a genetic imaging study. Neuroimage 2008, 42, 1554–1559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Montag, C.; Weber, B.; Fliessbach, K.; Elger, C.; Reuter, M. The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism impacts parahippocampal and amygdala volume in healthy humans: Incremental support for a genetic risk factor for depression. Psychol. Med. 2009, 39, 1831–1839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Pezawas, L.; Verchinski, B.A.; Mattay, V.S.; Callicott, J.H.; Kolachana, B.S.; Straub, R.E.; Egan, M.F.; Meyer-Lindenberg, A.; Weinberger, D.R. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism and variation in human cortical morphology. J. Neurosci. 2004, 24, 10099–10102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Hajek, T.; Kopecek, M.; Höschl, C. Reduced hippocampal volumes in healthy carriers of brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism: Meta-analysis. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 2012, 13, 178–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Kambeitz, J.P.; Bhattacharyya, S.; Kambeitz-Ilankovic, L.M.; Valli, I.; Collier, D.A.; McGuire, P. Effect of BDNF val66met polymorphism on declarative memory and its neural substrate: A meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012, 36, 2165–2177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Harrisberger, F.; Spalek, K.; Smieskova, R.; Schmidt, A.; Coynel, D.; Milnik, A.; Fastenrath, M.; Freytag, V.; Gschwind, L.; Walter, A. The association of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and the hippocampal volumes in healthy humans: A joint meta-analysis of published and new data. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014, 42, 267–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Hashimoto, T.; Fukui, K.; Takeuchi, H.; Yokota, S.; Kikuchi, Y.; Tomita, H.; Taki, Y.; Kawashima, R. Effects of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on gray matter volume in typically developing children and adolescents. Cereb. Cortex 2016, 26, 1795–1803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Chiang, M.-C.; Barysheva, M.; Toga, A.W.; Medland, S.E.; Hansell, N.K.; James, M.R.; McMahon, K.L.; De Zubicaray, G.I.; Martin, N.G.; Wright, M.J. BDNF gene effects on brain circuitry replicated in 455 twins. Neuroimage 2011, 55, 448–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Tost, H.; Alam, T.; Geramita, M.; Rebsch, C.; Kolachana, B.; Dickinson, D.; Verchinski, B.A.; Lemaitre, H.; Barnett, A.S.; Trampush, J.W. Effects of the BDNF val 66 met polymorphism on white matter microstructure in healthy adults. Neuropsychopharmacology 2013, 38, 525–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Montag, C.; Schoene-Bake, J.C.; Faber, J.; Reuter, M.; Weber, B. Genetic variation on the BDNF gene is not associated with differences in white matter tracts in healthy humans measured by tract-based spatial statistics. Genes Brain Behav. 2010, 9, 886–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Park, C.-H.; Kim, J.; Namgung, E.; Lee, D.-W.; Kim, G.H.; Kim, M.; Kim, N.; Kim, T.D.; Kim, S.; Lyoo, I.K. The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism affects the vulnerability of the brain structural network. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Ho, B.-C.; Milev, P.; O’Leary, D.S.; Librant, A.; Andreasen, N.C.; Wassink, T.H. Cognitive and magnetic resonance imaging brain morphometric correlates of brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met gene polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia and healthy volunteers. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2006, 63, 731–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Schofield, P.R.; Williams, L.M.; Paul, R.H.; Gatt, J.M.; Brown, K.; Luty, A.; Cooper, N.; Grieve, S.; Dobson-Stone, C.; Morris, C. Disturbances in selective information processing associated with the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism: Evidence from cognition, the P300 and fronto-hippocampal systems. Biol. Psychol. 2009, 80, 176–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Karnik, M.S.; Wang, L.; Barch, D.M.; Morris, J.C.; Csernansky, J.G. BDNF polymorphism rs6265 and hippocampal structure and memory performance in healthy control subjects. Psychiatry Res. 2010, 178, 425–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Strauss, J.; Barr, C.; George, C.; Ryan, C.; King, N.; Shaikh, S.; Kovacs, M.; Kennedy, J. BDNF and COMT polymorphisms. Neuromol. Med. 2004, 5, 181–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Kennedy, K.M.; Reese, E.D.; Horn, M.M.; Sizemore, A.N.; Unni, A.K.; Meerbrey, M.E.; Kalich, A.G., Jr.; Rodrigue, K.M. BDNF val66met polymorphism affects aging of multiple types of memory. Brain Res. 2015, 1612, 104–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Zhang, X.Y.; Xiu, M.H.; Haile, C.N.; Luo, X.; Xu, K.; Zhang, H.P.; Zuo, L.; Zhang, Z.; Zhang, X.; Kosten, T.A. Cognitive and serum BDNF correlates of BDNF Val66Met gene polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia and normal controls. Hum. Genet. 2012, 131, 1187–1195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Nagel, I.E.; Chicherio, C.; Li, S.-C.; Von Oertzen, T.; Sander, T.; Villringer, A.; Heekeren, H.R.; Bäckman, L.; Lindenberger, U. Human aging magnifies genetic effects on executive functioning and working memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2008, 2, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Mandelman, S.D.; Grigorenko, E.L. BDNF Val66Met and cognition: All, none, or some? A meta-analysis of the genetic association. Genes Brain Behav. 2012, 11, 127–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Getzmann, S.; Gajewski, P.D.; Hengstler, J.G.; Falkenstein, M.; Beste, C. BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and goal-directed behavior in healthy elderly—Evidence from auditory distraction. Neuroimage 2013, 64, 290–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Toh, Y.L.; Ng, T.; Tan, M.; Tan, A.; Chan, A. Impact of brain-derived neurotrophic factor genetic polymorphism on cognition: A systematic review. Brain Behav. 2018, 8, e01009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Papenberg, G.; Salami, A.; Persson, J.; Lindenberger, U.; Bäckman, L. Genetics and functional imaging: Effects of APOE, BDNF, COMT, and KIBRA in aging. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2015, 25, 47–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Ghisletta, P.; Bäckman, L.; Bertram, L.; Brandmaier, A.M.; Gerstorf, D.; Liu, T.; Lindenberger, U. The Val/Met polymorphism of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene predicts decline in perceptual speed in older adults. Psychol. Aging 2014, 29, 384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Laing, K.R.; Mitchell, D.; Wersching, H.; Czira, M.E.; Berger, K.; Baune, B.T. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene: A gender-specific role in cognitive function during normal cognitive aging of the MEMO-Study? Age 2012, 34, 1011–1022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Miyajima, F.; Ollier, W.; Mayes, A.; Jackson, A.; Thacker, N.; Rabbitt, P.; Pendleton, N.; Horan, M.; Payton, A. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor polymorphism Val66Met influences cognitive abilities in the elderly. Genes Brain Behav. 2008, 7, 411–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Sambataro, F.; Murty, V.; Lemaitre, H.; Reed, J.; Das, S.; Goldberg, T.; Callicott, J.; Weinberger, D.; Mattay, V. BNDF modulates normal human hippocampal ageing. Mol. Psychiatry 2010, 15, 116–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Kennedy, K.M.; Rodrigue, K.M.; Land, S.J.; Raz, N. BDNF Val66Met polymorphism influences age differences in microstructure of the Corpus Callosum. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2009, 3, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Mattson, M.P.; Maudsley, S.; Martin, B. A neural signaling triumvirate that influences ageing and age-related disease: Insulin/IGF-1, BDNF and serotonin. Ageing Res. Rev. 2004, 3, 445–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Tsai, S.-J.; Gau, Y.-T.A.; Liu, M.-E.; Hsieh, C.-H.; Liou, Y.-J.; Hong, C.-J. Association study of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and apolipoprotein E polymorphisms and cognitive function in aged males without dementia. Neurosci. Lett. 2008, 433, 158–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Erickson, K.I.; Kim, J.S.; Suever, B.L.; Voss, M.W.; Francis, B.M.; Kramer, A.F. Genetic contributions to age-related decline in executive function: A 10-year longitudinal study of COMT and BDNF polymorphisms. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2008, 2, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Gajewski, P.D.; Hengstler, J.G.; Golka, K.; Falkenstein, M.; Beste, C. The Met-genotype of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism is associated with reduced Stroop interference in elderly. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 3554–3563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Gajewski, P.D.; Hengstler, J.G.; Golka, K.; Falkenstein, M.; Beste, C. The Met-allele of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism enhances task switching in elderly. Neurobiol. Aging 2011, 32, 2327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Harris, S.; Fox, H.; Wright, A.; Hayward, C.; Starr, J.; Whalley, L.J.; Deary, I. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism is associated with age-related change in reasoning skills. Mol. Psychiatry 2006, 11, 505–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Vickers, J.C.; Mitew, S.; Woodhouse, A.; Fernandez-Martos, C.M.; Kirkcaldie, M.T.; Canty, A.J.; McCormack, G.H.; King, A.E. Defining the earliest pathological changes of Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2016, 13, 281–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Allen, S.J.; Watson, J.J.; Dawbarn, D. The neurotrophins and their role in Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 2011, 9, 559–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Hock, C.; Heese, K.; Hulette, C.; Rosenberg, C.; Otten, U. Region-specific neurotrophin imbalances in Alzheimer disease: Decreased levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and increased levels of nerve growth factor in hippocampus and cortical areas. Arch. Neurol. 2000, 57, 846–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Buckley, P.F.; Pillai, A.; Howell, K.R. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor: Findings in schizophrenia. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2011, 24, 122–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Li, G.; Peskind, E.R.; Millard, S.P.; Chi, P.; Sokal, I.; Yu, C.-E.; Bekris, L.M.; Raskind, M.A.; Galasko, D.R.; Montine, T.J. Cerebrospinal fluid concentration of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and cognitive function in non-demented subjects. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e5424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Faria, M.C.; Gonçalves, G.S.; Rocha, N.P.; Moraes, E.N.; Bicalho, M.A.; Cintra, M.T.G.; de Paula, J.J.; de Miranda, L.F.J.R.; de Souza Ferreira, A.C.; Teixeira, A.L. Increased plasma levels of BDNF and inflammatory markers in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2014, 53, 166–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Pláteník, J.; Fišar, Z.; Buchal, R.; Jirák, R.; Kitzlerová, E.; Zvěřová, M.; Raboch, J. GSK3β, CREB, and BDNF in peripheral blood of patients with Alzheimer’s disease and depression. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2014, 50, 83–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Woolley, J.D.; Strobl, E.V.; Shelly, W.B.; Karydas, A.M.; Robin Ketelle, R.N.; Wolkowitz, O.M.; Miller, B.L.; Rankin, K.P. BDNF serum concentrations show no relationship with diagnostic group or medication status in neurodegenerative disease. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2012, 9, 815–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. O’Bryant, S.E.; Hobson, V.L.; Hall, J.R.; Barber, R.C.; Zhang, S.; Johnson, L.; Diaz-Arrastia, R. Serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels are specifically associated with memory performance among Alzheimer’s disease cases. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2011, 31, 31–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  63. Álvarez, A.; Aleixandre, M.; Linares, C.; Masliah, E.; Moessler, H. Apathy and APOE4 are associated with reduced BDNF levels in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2014, 42, 1347–1355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  64. Weinstein, G.; Beiser, A.S.; Choi, S.H.; Preis, S.R.; Chen, T.C.; Vorgas, D.; Au, R.; Pikula, A.; Wolf, P.A.; DeStefano, A.L. Serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor and the risk for dementia: The Framingham Heart Study. JAMA Neurol. 2014, 71, 55–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Lim, Y.Y.; Villemagne, V.L.; Laws, S.M.; Ames, D.; Pietrzak, R.H.; Ellis, K.A.; Harrington, K.D.; Bourgeat, P.; Salvado, O.; Darby, D. BDNF Val66Met, Aβ amyloid, and cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 2013, 34, 2457–2464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Boots, E.A.; Schultz, S.A.; Clark, L.R.; Racine, A.M.; Darst, B.F.; Koscik, R.L.; Carlsson, C.M.; Gallagher, C.L.; Hogan, K.J.; Bendlin, B.B. BDNF Val66Met predicts cognitive decline in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer’s Prevention. Neurology 2017, 88, 2098–2106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Xia, H.; Wang, M.; Li, J.-Q.; Tan, C.-C.; Cao, X.-P.; Tan, L.; Yu, J.-T.; Initiative, A.s.D.N. The Influence of BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism on Cognition, Cerebrospinal Fluid, and Neuroimaging Markers in Non-Demented Elderly. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2019, 68, 405–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Lim, Y.Y.; Hassenstab, J.; Cruchaga, C.; Goate, A.; Fagan, A.M.; Benzinger, T.L.; Maruff, P.; Snyder, P.J.; Masters, C.L.; Allegri, R. BDNF Val66Met moderates memory impairment, hippocampal function and tau in preclinical autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2016, 139, 2766–2777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Forlenza, O.V.; Diniz, B.S.; Teixeira, A.L.; Ojopi, E.B.; Talib, L.L.; Mendonça, V.A.; Izzo, G.; Gattaz, W.F. Effect of brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism and serum levels on the progression of mild cognitive impairment. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 2010, 11, 774–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Franzmeier, N.; Ren, J.; Damm, A.; Monté-Rubio, G.; Boada, M.; Ruiz, A.; Ramirez, A.; Jessen, F.; Düzel, E.; Gómez, O.R. The BDNF Val66Met SNP modulates the association between beta-amyloid and hippocampal disconnection in Alzheimer’s disease. Mol. Psychiatry 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Lim, Y.Y.; Hassenstab, J.; Goate, A.; Fagan, A.M.; Benzinger, T.L.; Cruchaga, C.; McDade, E.; Chhatwal, J.; Levin, J.; Farlow, M.R. Effect of BDNFVal66Met on disease markers in dominantly inherited Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 2018, 84, 424–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Matsushita, S.; Arai, H.; Matsui, T.; Yuzuriha, T.; Urakami, K.; Masaki, T.; Higuchi, S. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene polymorphisms and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neural Transm. 2005, 112, 703–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Ventriglia, M.; Chiavetto, L.B.; Benussi, L.; Binetti, G.; Zanetti, O.; Riva, M.; Gennarelli, M. Association between the BDNF 196 A/G polymorphism and sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Mol. Psychiatry 2002, 7, 136–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Fehér, Á.; Juhász, A.; Rimanóczy, Á.; Kálmán, J.; Janka, Z. Association between BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and Alzheimer disease, dementia with Lewy bodies, and Pick disease. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2009, 23, 224–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Corder, E.H.; Saunders, A.M.; Strittmatter, W.J.; Schmechel, D.E.; Gaskell, P.C.; Small, G.; Roses, A.; Haines, J.; Pericak-Vance, M.A. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science 1993, 261, 921–923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Neu, S.C.; Pa, J.; Kukull, W.; Beekly, D.; Kuzma, A.; Gangadharan, P.; Wang, L.-S.; Romero, K.; Arneric, S.P.; Redolfi, A. Apolipoprotein E genotype and sex risk factors for Alzheimer disease: A meta-analysis. JAMA Neurol. 2017, 74, 1178–1189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Kauppi, K.; Nilsson, L.-G.; Persson, J.; Nyberg, L. Additive genetic effect of APOE and BDNF on hippocampus activity. Neuroimage 2014, 89, 306–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Gomar, J.J.; Conejero-Goldberg, C.; Huey, E.D.; Davies, P.; Goldberg, T.E.; Initiative, A.s.D.N. Lack of neural compensatory mechanisms of BDNF val66met met carriers and APOE E4 carriers in healthy aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 2016, 39, 165–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Lim, Y.Y.; Villemagne, V.L.; Laws, S.M.; Pietrzak, R.; Snyder, P.; Ames, D.; Ellis, K.A.; Harrington, K.; Rembach, A.; Martins, R.N. APOE and BDNF polymorphisms moderate amyloid β-related cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Mol. Psychiatry 2015, 20, 1322–1328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Ward, D.D.; Summers, M.J.; Saunders, N.L.; Janssen, P.; Stuart, K.E.; Vickers, J.C. APOE and BDNF Val66Met polymorphisms combine to influence episodic memory function in older adults. Behav. Brain Res. 2014, 271, 309–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Cechova, K.; Andel, R.; Angelucci, F.; Chmatalova, Z.; Markova, H.; Laczó, J.; Vyhnalek, M.; Matoska, V.; Kaplan, V.; Nedelska, Z. Impact of APOE and BDNF Val66Met Gene Polymorphisms on Cognitive Functions in Patients with Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2020, 73, 247–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Pietzuch, M.; King, A.E.; Ward, D.D.; Vickers, J.C. The influence of genetic factors and cognitive reserve on structural and functional resting-state brain networks in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2019, 11, 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  83. Livingston, G.; Sommerlad, A.; Orgeta, V.; Costafreda, S.G.; Huntley, J.; Ames, D.; Ballard, C.; Banerjee, S.; Burns, A.; Cohen-Mansfield, J. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet 2017, 390, 2673–2734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Norton, S.; Matthews, F.E.; Barnes, D.E.; Yaffe, K.; Brayne, C. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: An analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurol. 2014, 13, 788–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Barnes, D.E.; Yaffe, K. The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol. 2011, 10, 819–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Pedersen, B.K.; Saltin, B. Evidence for prescribing exercise as therapy in chronic disease. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2006, 16, 3–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Alty, J.; Farrow, M.; Lawler, K. Exercise and dementia prevention. Pract. Neurol. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Neeper, S.A.; Gómez-Pinilla, F.; Choi, J.; Cotman, C.W. Physical activity increases mRNA for brain-derived neurotrophic factor and nerve growth factor in rat brain. Brain Res. 1996, 726, 49–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Ickes, B.R.; Pham, T.M.; Sanders, L.A.; Albeck, D.S.; Mohammed, A.H.; Granholm, A.-C. Long-term environmental enrichment leads to regional increases in neurotrophin levels in rat brain. Exp. Neurol. 2000, 164, 45–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Novkovic, T.; Mittmann, T.; Manahan-Vaughan, D. BDNF contributes to the facilitation of hippocampal synaptic plasticity and learning enabled by environmental enrichment. Hippocampus 2015, 25, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Stuart, K.E.; King, A.E.; Fernandez-Martos, C.M.; Dittmann, J.; Summers, M.J.; Vickers, J.C. Mid-life environmental enrichment increases synaptic density in CA1 in a mouse model of Aβ-associated pathology and positively influences synaptic and cognitive health in healthy ageing. J. Comp. Neurol. 2017, 525, 1797–1810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Vaynman, S.S.; Ying, Z.; Yin, D.; Gomez-Pinilla, F. Exercise differentially regulates synaptic proteins associated to the function of BDNF. Brain Res. 2006, 1070, 124–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  93. Russo-Neustadt, A.; Ha, T.; Ramirez, R.; Kesslak, J.P. Physical activity–antidepressant treatment combination: Impact on brain-derived neurotrophic factor and behavior in an animal model. Behav. Brain Res. 2001, 120, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Szuhany, K.L.; Bugatti, M.; Otto, M.W. A meta-analytic review of the effects of exercise on brain-derived neurotrophic factor. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2015, 60, 56–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  95. Adlard, P.A.; Perreau, V.M.; Cotman, C.W. The exercise-induced expression of BDNF within the hippocampus varies across life-span. Neurobiol. Aging 2005, 26, 511–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Tsai, C.-L.; Ukropec, J.; Ukropcová, B.; Pai, M.-C. An acute bout of aerobic or strength exercise specifically modifies circulating exerkine levels and neurocognitive functions in elderly individuals with mild cognitive impairment. NeuroImage Clin. 2018, 17, 272–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Tsai, C.-L.; Pai, M.-C.; Ukropec, J.; Ukropcová, B. Distinctive effects of aerobic and resistance exercise modes on neurocognitive and biochemical changes in individuals with mild cognitive impairment. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2019, 16, 316–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Manuela Crispim Nascimento, C.; Rodrigues Pereira, J.; Pires de Andrade, L.; Garuffi, M.; Leme Talib, L.; Vicente Forlenza, O.; Maria Cancela, J.; Regina Cominetti, M.; Stella, F. Physical exercise in MCI elderly promotes reduction of pro-inflammatory cytokines and improvements on cognition and BDNF peripheral levels. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2014, 11, 799–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Kim, J.-M.; Stewart, R.; Bae, K.-Y.; Kim, S.-W.; Yang, S.-J.; Park, K.-H.; Shin, I.-S.; Yoon, J.-S. Role of BDNF val66met polymorphism on the association between physical activity and incident dementia. Neurobiol. Aging 2011, 32, 551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Watts, A.; Andrews, S.J.; Anstey, K.J. Sex differences in the impact of BDNF genotype on the longitudinal relationship between physical activity and cognitive performance. Gerontology 2018, 64, 361–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Thibeau, S.; McFall, G.P.; Wiebe, S.A.; Anstey, K.J.; Dixon, R.A. Genetic factors moderate everyday physical activity effects on executive functions in aging: Evidence from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Neuropsychology 2016, 30, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Brown, B.M.; Bourgeat, P.; Peiffer, J.J.; Burnham, S.; Laws, S.M.; Rainey-Smith, S.R.; Bartrés-Faz, D.; Villemagne, V.L.; Taddei, K.; Rembach, A. Influence of BDNF Val66Met on the relationship between physical activity and brain volume. Neurology 2014, 83, 1345–1352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Canivet, A.; Albinet, C.T.; André, N.; Pylouster, J.; Rodríguez-Ballesteros, M.; Kitzis, A.; Audiffren, M. Effects of BDNF polymorphism and physical activity on episodic memory in the elderly: A cross sectional study. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. 2015, 12, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  104. Nascimento, C.M.C.; Pereira, J.R.; Pires de Andrade, L.; Garuffi, M.; Ayan, C.; Kerr, D.S.; Talib, L.L.; Cominetti, M.R.; Stella, F. Physical exercise improves peripheral BDNF levels and cognitive functions in mild cognitive impairment elderly with different bdnf Val66Met genotypes. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2015, 43, 81–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Erickson, K.I.; Banducci, S.E.; Weinstein, A.M.; MacDonald III, A.W.; Ferrell, R.E.; Halder, I.; Flory, J.D.; Manuck, S.B. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism moderates an effect of physical activity on working memory performance. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 1770–1779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  106. Helm, E.E.; Matt, K.S.; Kirschner, K.F.; Pohlig, R.T.; Kohl, D.; Reisman, D.S. The influence of high intensity exercise and the Val66Met polymorphism on circulating BDNF and locomotor learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2017, 144, 77–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Lindenberger, U.; Nagel, I.E.; Chicherio, C.; Li, S.-C.; Heekeren, H.R.; Bäckman, L. Age-related decline in brain resources modulates genetic effects on cognitive functioning. Front. Neurosci. 2008, 2, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  108. Bozzali, M.; Dowling, C.; Serra, L.; Spanò, B.; Torso, M.; Marra, C.; Castelli, D.; Dowell, N.G.; Koch, G.; Caltagirone, C. The impact of cognitive reserve on brain functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2015, 44, 243–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Liu, Y.; Julkunen, V.; Paajanen, T.; Westman, E.; Wahlund, L.-O.; Aitken, A.; Sobow, T.; Mecocci, P.; Tsolaki, M.; Vellas, B. Education increases reserve against Alzheimer’s disease—Evidence from structural MRI analysis. Neuroradiology 2012, 54, 929–938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  110. Stern, Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2002, 8, 448–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 2015–2028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Franzmeier, N.; Duering, M.; Weiner, M.; Dichgans, M.; Ewers, M.; Initiative, A.s.D.N. Left frontal cortex connectivity underlies cognitive reserve in prodromal Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2017, 88, 1054–1061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  113. Solé-Padullés, C.; Bartrés-Faz, D.; Junqué, C.; Vendrell, P.; Rami, L.; Clemente, I.C.; Bosch, B.; Villar, A.; Bargalló, N.; Jurado, M.A. Brain structure and function related to cognitive reserve variables in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 2009, 30, 1114–1124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Ward, D.D.; Summers, M.J.; Saunders, N.; Ritchie, K.; Summers, J.; Vickers, J. The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism moderates the relationship between cognitive reserve and executive function. Transl. Psychiatry 2015, 5, e590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  115. Ward, D.D.; Andel, R.; Saunders, N.L.; Thow, M.E.; Klekociuk, S.Z.; Bindoff, A.D.; Vickers, J.C. The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism moderates the effect of cognitive reserve on 36-month cognitive change in healthy older adults. Alzheimer’s Dement. Transl. Res. Clin. Interv. 2017, 3, 323–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Damirchi, A.; Hosseini, F.; Babaei, P. Mental training enhances cognitive function and BDNF more than either physical or combined training in elderly women with MCI: A small-scale study. Am. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. Other Dement. 2018, 33, 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Pressler, S.J.; Titler, M.; Koelling, T.M.; Riley, P.L.; Jung, M.; Hoyland-Domenico, L.; Ronis, D.L.; Smith, D.G.; Bleske, B.E.; Dorsey, S.G. Nurse-enhanced computerized cognitive training increases serum brain-derived neurotropic factor levels and improves working memory in heart failure. J. Card. Fail. 2015, 21, 630–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Thow, M.E.; Summers, M.J.; Saunders, N.L.; Summers, J.J.; Ritchie, K.; Vickers, J.C. Further education improves cognitive reserve and triggers improvement in selective cognitive functions in older adults: The Tasmanian Healthy Brain Project. Alzheimer’s Dement. Diagn. Assess. Dis. Monit. 2018, 10, 22–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Ward, D.D.; Summers, M.J.; Valenzuela, M.; Srikanth, V.; Summers, J.; King, A.; Ritchie, K.; Robinson, A.; Vickers, J. Associations of Later-Life Education, the BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism and Cognitive Change in Older Adults. J. Prev. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2020, 7, 37–42. [Google Scholar]
  120. Notaras, M.; van den Buuse, M. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF): Novel insights into regulation and genetic variation. Neuroscientist 2019, 25, 434–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Stuart, K.E.; King, A.E.; Fernandez-Martos, C.M.; Summers, M.J.; Vickers, J.C. Environmental novelty exacerbates stress hormones and Aβ pathology in an Alzheimer’s model. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 2764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Lin, Y.; Cheng, S.; Xie, Z.; Zhang, D. Association of rs6265 and rs2030324 polymorphisms in brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene with Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e94961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  123. Petryshen, T.L.; Sabeti, P.C.; Aldinger, K.A.; Fry, B.; Fan, J.B.; Schaffner, S.; Waggoner, S.G.; Tahl, A.R.; Sklar, P. Population genetic study of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene. Mol. Psychiatry 2010, 15, 810–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Brown, D.T.; Vickers, J.C.; Stuart, K.E.; Cechova, K.; Ward, D.D. The BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism Modulates Resilience of Neurological Functioning to Brain Ageing and Dementia: A Narrative Review. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 195. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10040195

AMA Style

Brown DT, Vickers JC, Stuart KE, Cechova K, Ward DD. The BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism Modulates Resilience of Neurological Functioning to Brain Ageing and Dementia: A Narrative Review. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(4):195. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10040195

Chicago/Turabian Style

Brown, Donnamay T., James C. Vickers, Kimberley E. Stuart, Katerina Cechova, and David D. Ward. 2020. "The BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism Modulates Resilience of Neurological Functioning to Brain Ageing and Dementia: A Narrative Review" Brain Sciences 10, no. 4: 195. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10040195

APA Style

Brown, D. T., Vickers, J. C., Stuart, K. E., Cechova, K., & Ward, D. D. (2020). The BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism Modulates Resilience of Neurological Functioning to Brain Ageing and Dementia: A Narrative Review. Brain Sciences, 10(4), 195. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10040195

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop