The Relationship between Emotionally Laden Landmarks, Spatial Abilities, and Personality Traits: An Exploratory Study
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Method and Materials
2.1. Participants
2.2. Setting and Materials
2.2.1. Virtual Routes
2.2.2. Images
2.2.3. Procedure
Questionnaires
Cognitive Tasks
Landmarks Tasks
2.3. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Recognition Task
3.2. Route Continuation Task
3.2.1. Accuracy
3.2.2. Response Time
3.2.3. Correlations
3.3. Distance Comparison Task
3.3.1. Accuracy
3.3.2. Response Time
3.3.3. Correlations
- (i)
- The participants were better at judging distances among landmarks in positive as compared to the negative and neutral conditions. No effect of emotions was found in the route continuation task;
- (ii)
- As regards the route continuation task, the role of basic spatial abilities and personality traits specifically emerged in the positive, and to a less extent, in the neutral condition: the faster and more accurate participants were at the B-RL test, the more accurate they were in judging if at specific landmarks they did right or left. Similarly, the more they declared to be friendly/compassionate, the more accurate they were in the route continuation task. Furthermore, specific positive associations appeared between performance at the Corsi test and performance in the positive condition: the higher the performance at the Corsi test (both normal and reversed), the more accurate the performance at route continuation task;
- (iii)
- As regards the distance comparison task, the role of spatial abilities emerged for all the three conditions, but again to a greater extent in the positive and neutral condition as compared to the negative one. Overall, the faster and more accurate participants were in the mental rotation task, the faster they were in judging correctly the distances among landmarks. Instead, the correlations with a tendency towards significance appeared between emotional stability, openness, and performance in the negative condition. Specifically, the higher the emotional stability and openness to experience, the greater the time needed to evaluate distances between negative stimuli.
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Mean | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Landmark | Code | Valence | Arousal | |
Negative | AimedGun | 6230 | 2.37 | 7.35 |
Attack | 6550 | 2.73 | 7.09 | |
Attack | 6510 | 2.46 | 6.96 | |
KKKrally | 9810 | 2.09 | 6.62 | |
Gang | 6821 | 2.38 | 6.29 | |
Fire | 9623 | 3.04 | 6.05 | |
Skinhead | 9800 | 2.04 | 6.05 | |
Snake | 1022 | 4.26 | 6.02 | |
Terrorist | 6213 | 2.91 | 5.86 | |
Dog | 1301 | 3.7 | 5.77 | |
Bomb | 2692 | 3.36 | 5.35 | |
Toddler | 2095 | 1.79 | 5.25 | |
Positive | Roller | 8492 | 7.21 | 7.31 |
Lightning | 5950 | 5.99 | 6.79 | |
Sailing | 8080 | 7.73 | 6.65 | |
Water Skier | 8200 | 7.54 | 6.35 | |
EroticCouple | 4608 | 7.07 | 6.47 | |
Money | 8501 | 7.91 | 6.44 | |
Romantic | 4597 | 6.95 | 5.91 | |
Wedding | 4626 | 7.6 | 5.78 | |
Fireworks | 5480 | 7.53 | 5.48 | |
Baby | 2045 | 7.87 | 5.47 | |
Pregnant | 2155 | 6.78 | 5.43 | |
Puppies | 1710 | 8.34 | 5.41 | |
Neutral | Lamp | 7175 | 4.87 | 1.72 |
Chess | 2580 | 5.71 | 2.79 | |
Couple | 2396 | 4.91 | 3.34 | |
Boy | 2280 | 4.22 | 3.77 | |
Hammer | 7110 | 4.55 | 2.27 | |
Tissue | 7950 | 4.94 | 2.28 | |
Window | 7490 | 5.52 | 2.42 | |
Baskets | 7041 | 4.99 | 2.6 | |
Basket | 7010 | 4.94 | 1.76 | |
Fan | 7020 | 4.97 | 2.17 | |
Fork | 7080 | 5.27 | 2.32 | |
RollingPin | 7000 | 5.00 | 2.42 | |
NeutGirl | 2440 | 4.49 | 2.63 | |
Girl | 2411 | 5.07 | 2.86 | |
Flowers | 5731 | 5.39 | 2.74 | |
Bridge | 7547 | 5.21 | 3.18 | |
Spoon | 7004 | 5.04 | 2.00 | |
Shoes | 7031 | 4.52 | 2.03 | |
Bowl | 7006 | 4.88 | 2.33 | |
Building | 7491 | 4.82 | 2.39 | |
Man | 2190 | 4.83 | 2.41 | |
Golf | 8312 | 5.37 | 3.32 | |
NeuWoman | 2038 | 5.09 | 2.94 | |
Farmer | 2191 | 5.3 | 3.61 |
Skewness (S) | Std. Err. | S/Std. Err. | Kurtosis (K) | Std. Err. | K/Std. Err. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B-RL_Acc *** | −2.045 | 0.209 | 9.806 | 3.650 | 0.414 | 8.814 |
B-RL_RT ** | 1.034 | 0.209 | 4.959 | 0.198 | 0.414 | 0.479 |
CBT * | 0.386 | 0.206 | 1.869 | −0.200 | 0.410 | 0.488 |
CBT_REV * | 0.073 | 0.206 | 0.355 | −0.323 | 0.410 | 0.788 |
MR_Acc * | −0.134 | 0.208 | 0.646 | −0.833 | 0.413 | 2.019 |
MR_RT ** | 0.767 | 0.208 | 3.694 | 0.475 | 0.413 | 1.150 |
TIPI_AG * | −0.240 | 0.206 | 1.165 | 0.459 | 0.410 | 1.121 |
TIPI_CO * | −0.471 | 0.206 | 2.285 | −0.349 | 0.410 | 0.852 |
TIPI_ES ** | −0.732 | 0.206 | 3.547 | −0.096 | 0.410 | 0.235 |
TIPI_EX * | 0.043 | 0.206 | 0.208 | −0.981 | 0.410 | 2.395 |
TIPI_OP ** | −0.682 | 0.206 | 3.305 | −0.348 | 0.410 | 0.849 |
Neg_Rec_Acc *** | −1.067 | 0.207 | 5.152 | 0.837 | 0.411 | 2.036 |
Neu_Rec_Acc ** | −0.975 | 0.208 | 4.693 | 0.321 | 0.413 | 0.777 |
Pos_Rec_Acc *** | −1.108 | 0.210 | 5.274 | 0.755 | 0.417 | 1.811 |
Neg_Comp_Acc * | 0.108 | 0.208 | 0.518 | −0.204 | 0.413 | 0.494 |
Neg_Comp_RT ** | 1.025 | 0.208 | 4.935 | 1.112 | 0.413 | 2.694 |
Neg_Cont_Acc * | −0.372 | 0.208 | 1.789 | −0.877 | 0.413 | 2.126 |
Neg_Cont_RT *** | 1.232 | 0.208 | 5.930 | 1.440 | 0.413 | 3.490 |
Neu_Comp_Acc * | 0.069 | 0.207 | 0.335 | −0.302 | 0.411 | 0.733 |
Neu_Comp_RT *** | 1.293 | 0.207 | 6.245 | 1.416 | 0.411 | 3.444 |
Neu_Cont_Acc * | −0.306 | 0.209 | 1.463 | −0.967 | 0.416 | 2.327 |
Neu_Cont_RT *** | 1.659 | 0.209 | 7.928 | 3.045 | 0.416 | 7.326 |
Pos_Comp_Acc * | 0.095 | 0.206 | 0.461 | −0.702 | 0.410 | 1.713 |
Pos_Comp_RT ** | 0.803 | 0.206 | 3.893 | 0.051 | 0.410 | 0.125 |
Pos_Cont_Acc * | −0.551 | 0.208 | 2.652 | −0.261 | 0.413 | 0.633 |
Pos_Cont_RT *** | 1.770 | 0.208 | 8.520 | 3.273 | 0.413 | 7.932 |
References
- Nuhn, E.; Timpf, S. Personal dimensions of landmarks. In Annual International Conference on Geographic Information Science; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2017; pp. 129–143. [Google Scholar]
- Balaban, C.Z.; Karimpur, H.; Röser, F.; Hamburger, K. Turn left where you felt unhappy: How affect influences landmark-based wayfnding. Cogn. Process. 2017, 18, 135–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piccardi, L.; Guariglia, P.; Nori, R.; Palmiero, M. The Role of Emotional Landmarks in Embodied and Not-Embodied Tasks. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Palmiero, M.; Piccardi, L. The role of emotional landmarks on topographical memory. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ruotolo, F.; Claessen, M.H.G.; van der Ham, I.J.M. Putting emotions in routes: The influence of emotionally laden landmarks on spatial memory. Psychol. Res. 2019, 83, 1083–1095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shepard, R.N.; Metzler, J. Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 1971, 171, 701–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cooper, L.A.; Shepard, R.N. Chronometric studies of the rotation of mental images. In Visual information Processing; Chase, W.G., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1973; pp. 75–176. [Google Scholar]
- Iachini, T.; Ruggiero, G.; Bartolo, A.; Rapuano, M.; Ruotolo, F. The effect of body-related stimuli on mental rotation in children, young and elderly adults. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kozhevnikov, M.; Motes, M.A.; Rasch, B.; Blajenkova, O. Perspective-taking vs. mental rotation transformations and how they predict spatial navigation performance. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 2006, 20, 397–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pazzaglia, F.; De Beni, R. Are people with high and low mental rotation abilities differently susceptible to the alignment effect? Perception 2006, 35, 369–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Moffat, S.D.; Hampson, E.; Hatzipantelis, M. Navigation in a “virtual” maze: Sex differences and correlation with psychometric measures of spatial ability in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 1998, 19, 73–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blajenkova, O.; Motes, M.A.; Kozhevnikov, M. Individual differences in the representations of novel environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 2005, 25, 97–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaltner, S.; Jansen, P. Emotion and affect in mental imagery: Do fear and anxiety manipulate mental rotation performance? Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Piccardi, L.; Risetti, M.; Nori, R.; Tanzilli, A.; Bernardi, L.; Guariglia, C. Perspective changing in primary and secondary learning: A gender difference study. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2011, 21, 114–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchini, F.; Incoccia, C.; Palermo, L.; Piccardi, L.; Zompanti, L.; Sabatini, U.; Peran, P.; Guariglia, C. Developmental topographical disorientation in a healthy subject. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 1563–1573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piccardi, L.; Berthoz, A.; Baulac, M.; Denos, M.; Dupont, S.; Samson, S.; Guariglia, C. Different spatial memory systems are involved in small-and large-scale environments: Evidence from patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Exp. Brain. Res. 2010, 206, 171–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piccardi, L.; Iaria, G.; Bianchini, F.; Zompanti, L.; Guariglia, C. Dissociated deficits of visuo-spatial memory in near space and navigational space: Evidence from brain-damaged patients and healthy older participants. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2011, 18, 362–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nemmi, F.; Boccia, M.; Piccardi, L.; Galati, G.; Guariglia, C. Segregation of neural circuits involved in spatial learning in reaching and navigational space. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51, 1561–1570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palmiero, M.; Nori, R.; Rogolino, C.; D’Amico, S.; Piccardi, L. Situated navigational working memory: The role of positive mood. Cogn. Process. 2015, 16, 327–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piccardi, L.; Iaria, G.; Ricci, M.; Bianchini, F.; Zompanti, L.; Guariglia, C. Walking in the Corsi test: Which type of memory do you need? Neurosci. Lett. 2008, 432, 127–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piccardi, L.; Bianchini, F.; Argento, O.; De Nigris, A.; Maialetti, A.; Palermo, L.; Guariglia, C. The Walking Corsi Test (WalCT): Standardization of the topographical memory test in an Italian population. Neurol. Sci. 2013, 34, 971–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hjelmervik, H.; Westerhausen, R.; Hirnstein, M.; Specht, K.; Hausmann, M. The neural correlates of sex differences in left–right confusion. NeuroImage 2015, 113, 196–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bryant, K.J. Personality correlates of sense of direction and geographic orientation. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1982, 43, 1318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pazzaglia, F.; Meneghetti, C.; Ronconi, L. Tracing a route and finding a shortcut: The working memory, motivational, and personality factors involved. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wyllie, D.S.; Smith, G.C. Effects of extroversion on the routine spatial behavior of middle adolescents. Prof. Geogr. 1996, 48, 166–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walkowiak, S.; Foulsham, T.; Eardley, A.F. Individual differences and personality correlates of navigational performance in the virtual route learning task. Comput. Human Behav. 2015, 45, 402–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Komulainen, E.; Meskanen, K.; Lipsanen, J.; Lahti, J.M.; Jylhä, P.; Melartin, T.; Wichers, M.; Isometsa, E.; Ekelund, J. The effect of personality on daily life emotional processes. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e110907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lischetzke, T.; Eid, M. Why extraverts are happier than introverts: The role of mood regulation. J. Personal. 2006, 74, 1127–1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hutcherson, C.A.; Goldin, P.R.; Ramel, W.; McRae, K.; Gross, J.J. Attention and emotion influence the relationship between extraversion and neural response. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2008, 3, 71–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gosling, S.D.; Rentfrow, P.J.; Swann, W.B., Jr. A very brief measure of the big five personality domains. J. Res. Personal. 2003, 37, 504–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lang, P.J.; Bradley, M.M.; Cuthbert, B.N. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual; Technical Report A-8; University of Florida: Gainesville, FL, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Ofte, S.H.; Hugdahl, K. Rightleft discrimination in male and female, young and old subjects. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2002, 24, 82–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S. Using Multivariate Statistics; Pearson: Boston, MA, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- West, S.G.; Finch, J.F.; Curran, P.J. Structural equation models with non normal variables: Problems and remedies. In Structural Equation Modeling: Concepts, Issues and Applications; Hoyle, R.H., Ed.; Sage: Newbery Park, CA, USA, 1995; pp. 56–75. [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini, Y.; Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B. Stat. Methodol. 1995, 57, 289–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nori, R.; Piccardi, L.; Pelosi, A.; De Luca, D.; Frasca, F.; Giusberti, F. Perspective changing in WalCT and VR-WalCT: A gender difference study [WalCT–VR-WalCT: Gender differences]. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 53, 316–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perrochon, A.; Kemoun, G.; Dugué, B.; Berthoz, A. Cognitive impairment assessment through visuospatial memory can be performed with a modified walking Corsi test using the ‘magic carpet’. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Dis. Extra. 2014, 4, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rusconi, M.L.; Suardi, A.; Zanetti, M.; Rozzini, L. Spatial navigation in elderly healthy subjects, amnestic and non amnestic MCI patients. J. Neurol. Sci. 2015, 359, 430–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malhotra, P.; Jäger, H.R.; Parton, A.; Greenwood, R.; Playford, E.D.; Brown, M.M.; Driver, J.; Husain, M. Spatial working memory capacity in unilateral neglect. Brain 2005, 128, 424–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jordan, K.; Wüstenberg, T.; Jaspers-Feyer, F.; Fellbrich, A.; Peters, M. Sex differences in left/right confusion. Cortex. 2006, 42, 69–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Isen, A.M. On the relationship between affect and creative problem solving. In Affect, Creative Experience, and Psychological Adjustment; Russ, S., Ed.; Taylor & Francis: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1999; pp. 3–17. [Google Scholar]
- Storbeck, J.; Maswood, R. Happiness increases verbal and spatial working memory capacity where sadness does not: Emotion, working memory and executive control. Cogn. Emot. 2016, 30, 925–938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moran, T.P. Anxiety and working memory capacity: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychol. Bull. 2016, 142, 831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ribeiro, F.S.; Santos, F.H.D.; Albuquerque, P.B. How Does Allocation of Emotional Stimuli Impact Working Memory Tasks? An Overview. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2019, 15, 81–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Isen, A.M. Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1987, 20, 203–253. [Google Scholar]
- George, J.M.; Brief, A.P. Negative affectivity and coping with job loss. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1996, 21, 7–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Staw, B.M.; Barsade, S.G. Affect and managerial performance: A test of the sadder-but-wiser vs. happier-and-smarter hypotheses. Adm. Sci. Q. 1993, 38, 304–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Staw, B.M.; Sutton, R.I.; Pelled, L.H. Employee positive emotion and favorable outcomes at the workplace. Organ. Sci. 1994, 5, 51–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aspinwall, L.G.; Taylor, S.E. A stitch in time: Self-regulation and proactive coping. Psychol. Bull. 1997, 121, 417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Taylor, S.E.; Aspinwall, L.G. Mediating and moderating processes in psychosocial stress: Appraisal, coping, resistance, and vulnerability. In Psychosocial Stress: Perspectives on Structure, Theory, Life-Course, and Methods; Kaplan, H.B., Ed.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1996; pp. 71–110. [Google Scholar]
- Bless, H.; Hamilton, D.L.; Mackie, D.M. Mood effects on the organization of person information. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 22, 497–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fiedler, K.; Nickel, S.; Asbeck, J.; Pagel, U. Mood and the generation effect. Cogn. Emot. 2003, 17, 585–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ashby, F.G.; Isen, A.M.; Turken, A.U. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychol. Rev. 1999, 106, 529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa Jr, P.T.; McCrae, R.R. Four ways five factors are basic. Personal. Individ. Dif. 1992, 13, 653–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa Jr, P.T.; McCrae, R.R.; Dye, D.A. Facet scales for agreeableness and conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO Personality Inventory. Personal. Individ. Dif. 1991, 12, 887–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chapman, B.; Duberstein, P.; Tindle, H.A.; Sink, K.M.; Robbins, J.; Tancredi, D.J.; Franks, P.; The Gingko Evaluation of Memory (GEM) Study Investigators. Personality predicts cognitive function over 7 years in older persons. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2012, 20, 612–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baker, T.J.; Bichsel, J. Personality predictors of intelligence: Differences between young and cognitively healthy older adults. Personal. Individ. Dif. 2006, 41, 861–871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Denburg, N.L.; Weller, J.A.; Yamada, T.H.; Shivapour, D.M.; Kaup, A.R.; LaLoggia, A.; Cole, C.A.; Tranel, D.; Bechara, A. Poor decision making among older adults is related to elevated levels of neuroticism. Ann. Behav. Med. 2009, 37, 164–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ghiabi, B.; Besharat, M.A. An investigation of the relationship between Personality dimensions and emotional intelligence. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2011, 30, 416–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Robinson, M.D.; Ode, S.; Moeller, S.K.; Goetz, P.W. Neuroticism and affective priming: Evidence for a neuroticism-linked negative schema. Personal. Individ. Dif. 2007, 42, 1221–1231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Chan, E.; Baumann, O.; Bellgrove, M.A.; Mattingley, J.B. Negative emotional experiences during navigation enhance parahippocampal activity during recall of place information. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014, 26, 154–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lawton, C.A. Gender differences in way-finding strategies: Relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles 1994, 30, 765–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lawton, C.A.; Charleston, S.I.; Zieles, A.S. Individual-and gender-related differences in indoor wayfinding. Environ. Behav. 1996, 28, 204–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montello, D.R.; Lovelace, K.L.; Golledge, R.G.; Self, C.M. Sex-related differences and similarities in geographic and environmental spatial abilities. Ann. Am. Assoc. Geogr. 1999, 89, 515–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Prestopnik, J.L.; Roskos–Ewoldsen, B. The relations among wayfinding strategy use, sense of direction, sex, familiarity, and wayfinding ability. J. Environ. Psychol. 2000, 20, 177–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmitz, S. Gender differences in acquisition of environmental knowledge related to wayfinding behavior, spatial anxiety and self-estimated environmental competencies. Sex Roles 1999, 41, 71–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iachini, T.; Ruotolo, F.; Ruggiero, G. The effects of familiarity and gender on spatial representation. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 227–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moffat, S.D. Aging and spatial navigation: What do we know and where do we go? Neuropsychol. Rev. 2009, 19, 478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ruggiero, G.; D’Errico, O.; Iachini, T. Development of egocentric and allocentric spatial representations from childhood to elderly age. Psychol. Res. 2016, 80, 259–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Uttal, D.H. Seeing the big picture: Map use and the development of spatial cognition. Dev. Sci. 2000, 3, 247–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woollett, K.; Maguire, E.A. The effect of navigational expertise on wayfinding in new environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 565–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Woollett, K.; Maguire, E.A. Acquiring “the Knowledge” of London’s layout drives structural brain changes. Curr. Biol. 2011, 21, 2109–2114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lopez, A.; Caffò, A.O.; Bosco, A. Topographical disorientation in aging. Familiarity with the environment does matter. Neurol. Sci. 2018, 39, 1519–1528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lopez, A.; Caffò, A.O.; Spano, G.; Bosco, A. The effect of aging on memory for recent and remote egocentric and allocentric information. Exp. Aging Res. 2019, 45, 57–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Route Continuation Task- Accuracy | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Negative | Neutral | Positive | ||||
r·P/ R·S | p-level | r·P/ R·S | p-Level | r·P/ R·S | p-Level | |
B-RL_ACC | 0.06 | 0.504 | 0.13 | 0.13 | 0.14 | 0.12 |
B-RL_RT | −0.15 | 0.078 | −0.26 * | 0 | −0.40 * | 0 |
CBT | 0.16 | 0.057 | 0.09 | 0.28 | 0.38* | 0 |
CBT_REV | 0.15 | 0.079 | 0.18 | 0.04 | 0.33* | 0 |
MR_ACC | 0.12 | 0.174 | 0.06 | 0.50 | −0.04 | 0.64 |
MR_RT | −0.17 | 0.055 | −0.13 | 0.13 | −0.05 | 0.60 |
TIPI_AG | 0.20 | 0.022 | 0.24 * | 0 | 0.38* | 0 |
TIPI_ES | −0.04 | 0.643 | −0.04 | 0.63 | −0.01 | 0.89 |
TIPI_EX | −0.04 | 0.680 | −0.11 | 0.22 | 0.01 | 0.89 |
TIPI_OP | −0.02 | 0.790 | −0.04 | 0.68 | −0.08 | 0.39 |
TIPI_CO | 0 | 0.963 | −0.02 | 0.79 | 0 | 0.96 |
Route Continuation Task- Response Time | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Negative | Neutral | Positive | ||||
R·S | p-Level | R·S | p-Level | R·S | p-Level | |
B-RL_ACC | 0.058 | 0.510 | 0.093 | 0.293 | −0.086 | 0.327 |
B-RL_RT | 0.145 | 0.097 | 0.086 | 0.332 | 0.088 | 0.311 |
CBT | −0.119 | 0.168 | 0.022 | 0.799 | 0.062 | 0.476 |
CBT_REV | −0.083 | 0.337 | −0.011 | 0.896 | −0.026 | 0.763 |
MR_ACC | 0.020 | 0.816 | 0.153 | 0.078 | −0.105 | 0.228 |
MR_RT | 0.156 | 0.071 | −0.006 | 0.947 | 0.120 | 0.168 |
TIPI_AG | −0.045 | 0.606 | −0.024 | 0.784 | −0.044 | 0.615 |
TIPI_CO | 0.092 | 0.287 | 0.148 | 0.088 | 0.046 | 0.595 |
TIPI_ES | 0.047 | 0.584 | 0.047 | 0.586 | −0.030 | 0.728 |
TIPI_EX | 0.160 | 0.063 | −0.112 | 0.198 | −0.051 | 0.552 |
TIPI_OP | 0.021 | 0.805 | 0.048 | 0.585 | −0.036 | 0.674 |
Distance Comparison Task- Accuracy | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Negative | Neutral | Positive | ||||
r·P/R·S | p-Level | r·P/R·S | p-Level | r·P/R·S | p-Level | |
B-RL_ACC | 0.15 | 0.09 | 0.20 | 0.02 | −0.02 | 0.79 |
B-RL_RT | −0.21 | 0.01 | −0.01 | 0.94 | −0.01 | 0.91 |
CBT | 0.13 | 0.12 | 0.11 | 0.20 | −0.10 | 0.23 |
CBT_REV | 0.18 | 0.04 | 0.16 | 0.07 | −0.06 | 0.51 |
MR_ACC | 0.12 | 0.17 | 0.27 * | 0 | 0.09 | 0.30 |
MR_RT | −0.18 | 0.03 | −0.08 | 0.36 | −0.35 * | 0 |
TIPI_AG | 0.12 | 0.15 | 0.09 | 0.32 | 0.06 | 0.48 |
TIPI_ES | 0.11 | 0.22 | 0.13 | 0.12 | 0 | 0.96 |
TIPI_EX | −0.11 | 0.21 | −0.10 | 0.23 | −0.06 | 0.48 |
TIPI_OP | 0.13 | 0.14 | 0.15 | 0.08 | −0.06 | 0.51 |
TIPI_CO | −0.06 | 0.51 | −0.12 | 0.16 | 0.02 | 0.85 |
Distance Comparison Task- Response Time | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Negative | Neutral | Positive | ||||
R·S | p-Level | R·S | p-Level | R·S | p-Level | |
B-RL_ACC | 0 | 1.00 | −0.02 | 0.80 | 0.01 | 0.93 |
B-RL_RT | −0.01 | 0.90 | 0.01 | 0.87 | −0.06 | 0.46 |
CBT | −0.07 | 0.43 | −0.01 | 0.86 | 0.06 | 0.49 |
CBT_REV | −0.02 | 0.85 | 0.02 | 0.82 | 0.01 | 0.95 |
MR_ACC | −0.12 | 0.17 | −0.03 | 0.76 | −0.13 | 0.14 |
MR_RT | 0.34 * | 0 | 0.35 * | 0 | 0.39 * | 0 |
TIPI_AG | 0.01 | 0.95 | −0.14 | 0.10 | −0.03 | 0.76 |
TIPI_CO | −0.06 | 0.49 | 0.07 | 0.43 | −0.04 | 0.62 |
TIPI_ES | 0.20 | 0.02 | 0.13 | 0.14 | 0.06 | 0.50 |
TIPI_EX | 0.08 | 0.38 | −0.09 | 0.28 | 0.05 | 0.53 |
TIPI_OP | 0.19 | 0.03 | 0.11 | 0.20 | 0.05 | 0.60 |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Ruotolo, F.; Sbordone, F.L.; van der Ham, I.J.M. The Relationship between Emotionally Laden Landmarks, Spatial Abilities, and Personality Traits: An Exploratory Study. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 326. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10060326
Ruotolo F, Sbordone FL, van der Ham IJM. The Relationship between Emotionally Laden Landmarks, Spatial Abilities, and Personality Traits: An Exploratory Study. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(6):326. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10060326
Chicago/Turabian StyleRuotolo, Francesco, Filomena L. Sbordone, and Ineke J.M. van der Ham. 2020. "The Relationship between Emotionally Laden Landmarks, Spatial Abilities, and Personality Traits: An Exploratory Study" Brain Sciences 10, no. 6: 326. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10060326
APA StyleRuotolo, F., Sbordone, F. L., & van der Ham, I. J. M. (2020). The Relationship between Emotionally Laden Landmarks, Spatial Abilities, and Personality Traits: An Exploratory Study. Brain Sciences, 10(6), 326. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10060326