Differences and Similarities in Predictors of Expressive Vocabulary Development between Children with Down Syndrome and Young Typically Developing Children
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Expressive Vocabulary, a Specific Area of Weakness in Children with Down Syndrome?
Stages of Early Expressive Vocabulary Development | TD | DS |
---|---|---|
Developmental process | Rapid growth during early childhood [25,26,27] | Slow growth during childhood [17,28] |
Utterance of first words | At the age of approximately 1 year [29,30] | At the age of approximately 1–9 years [31], but large individual variation [32]. Some say their first words at the same time as TD children [33] |
Achievement of 50 words | At the age of approximately 18 months [32,34] | For 25%, at approximately 3 years of age; for 50%, at approximately 4 years; and for 75%, at approximately 5 years [32]. Large individual variation [35] |
Onset of sentence production, i.e., combination of two words or more | At the age of approximately 18 months [36] or after reaching 50 words [34,36] | Approximately 3.5–5 years of age [37,38] or after reaching 100 words [39] |
2.1. Predictors of Expressive Vocabulary Development
2.2. The Current Study
- What variables at 3 years of age predict expressive vocabulary in typically developing children at ages 3, 4 and 5 years?
- What variables at 6 years of age predict expressive vocabulary in children with Down syndrome at ages 6, 7 and 8 years (first three years of primary school)?
- What differences and similarities exist between the estimated effects of the predictors of expressive vocabulary between children with Down syndrome and typically developing children?
3. Materials and Methods
3.1. Participants
3.2. Data Collection Procedure
3.3. Measures
3.4. Expressive Vocabulary
3.5. Receptive Vocabulary
3.6. Auditory Memory
3.7. Phonological Awareness
3.8. Oral Motor Skills
3.9. Nonverbal Mental Ability
3.10. Parental Questionnaires
3.11. Socioemotional Functioning
3.12. Background Questionnaire, Socioeconomic Status (SES)
3.13. Background Questionnaire, Home Literacy Environment
3.14. Background Questionnaire, Reading Skills
3.15. Background Questionnaire, Language Problems in the Family
3.16. Data Analyses
4. Results
4.1. Descriptive Statistics
4.2. Predictors of Expressive Language
4.3. Change over Time
5. Discussion
5.1. Development of Expressive Vocabulary
5.2. Predictors of Expressive Vocabulary in Typically Developing Children
5.3. Predictors of Expressive Vocabulary in Children with Down Syndrome
5.4. Differences and Similarities in Predictors and Predictive Values
5.5. Limitations
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Down Syndrome | Typically Developing Children with a Similar Nonverbal Mental Age | Sign. Test of Differences in Beta between Groups | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 43) | (n = 57) | ||||||||
b | 95% CI | p-Value | b | 95% CI | p-Value | b | 95% CI | p-Value | |
Intercept | 0.23 | −0.7 to 0.53 | 0.14 | 1.07 | 0.81 to 1.32 | <0.001 | |||
Gender ¹ | 0.19 | −0.18 to 0.55 | 0.30 | −0.08 | −0.39 to 0.23 | 0.62 | −0.27 | −0.75 to 0.21 | 0.27 |
Nonverbal mental age T1 2 | 0.02 | −0.18 to 0.21 | 0.86 | 0.09 | −0.09 to 0.27 | 0.33 | 0.07 | −0.20 to 0.34 | 0.60 |
Mean parental education T1 | −0.02 | −0.20 to 0.15 | 0.78 | −0.05 | −0.20 to 0.10 | 0.50 | −0.03 | −0.26 to 0.20 | 0.83 |
Home literacy environment T1 2 | 0.08 | −0.12 to 0.27 | 0.43 | 0.08 | −0.07 to 0.23 | 0.30 | 0.00 | −0.24 to 0.25 | 0.99 |
Language problems in family T1 ¹ | −0.04 | −0.45 to 0.38 | 0.86 | −0.09 | −0.41 to 0.23 | 0.59 | −0.05 | −0.58 to 0.47 | 0.84 |
Socioemotional functioning T1 2 | 0.00 | −0.19 to 0.20 | 0.96 | 0.04 | −0.18 to 0.27 | 0.71 | 0.04 | −0.26 to 0.34 | 0.80 |
Auditory memory T1 2 | 0.58 | 0.21 to 0.94 | 0.003 | 0.13 | −0.05 to 0.31 | 0.15 | −0.45 | −0.85 to −0.04 | 0.03 |
Oral motor skills T1 2 | 0.20 | −0.04 to 0.44 | 0.10 | 0.03 | −0.11 to 0.18 | 0.66 | −0.17 | −0.45 to 0.11 | 0.24 |
Phonological awareness T1 2 | 0.02 | −0.14 to 0.18 | 0.82 | −0.03 | −0.36 to 0.30 | 0.87 | −0.05 | −0.41 to 0.32 | 0.80 |
Receptive vocabulary T1 2 | 0.32 | 0.07 to 0.57 | 0.01 | 0.16 | −0.05 to 0.37 | 0.14 | −0.16 | −0.49 to 0.17 | 0.34 |
Early reading skills T1 2 | 0.06 | −0.08 to 0.20 | 0.41 | 0.16 | −0.12 to 0.44 | 0.25 | 0.10 | −0.21 to 0.42 | 0.52 |
Time ¹ | |||||||||
T1–T2 | 0.51 | 0.34 to 0.67 | <0.001 | 0.75 | 0.60 to 0.89 | <0.001 | 0.24 | 0.02 to 0.46 | 0.03 |
T2–T3 | 0.30 | 0.13 to 0.46 | <0.001 | 0.64 | 0.50 to 0.78 | <0.001 | 0.34 | 0.12 to 0.56 | 0.002 |
T1–T3 | 0.80 | 0.64 to 0.97 | <0.001 | 1.38 | 1.24 to 1.53 | <0.001 | 0.58 | 0.36 to 0.80 | <0.001 |
References
- Brysbaert, M.; Stevens, M.; Mandera, P.; Keuleers, E. How many words do we know? Practical estimates of vocabulary size dependent on word definition, the degree of language input and the participant’s age. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bleses, D.; Makransky, G.; Dale, P.S.; HØJen, A.; Ari, B.A. Early productive vocabulary predicts academic achievement 10 years later. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2016, 37, 1461–1476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coplan, R.J.; Armer, M. Talking yourself out of being shy: Shyness, expressive vocabulary, and socioemotional adjustment in preschool. Merrill Palmer Q. 2005, 51, 20–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dionne, G.; Tremblay, R.; Boivin, M.; Laplante, D.; Pérusse, D. Physical aggression and expressive vocabulary in 19-month-old twins. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 39, 261–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Irwin, J.R.; Carter, A.S.; Briggs-Gowan, M.J. The social-emotional development of “late-talking” toddlers. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2002, 41, 1324–1332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paul, R.; Looney, S.S.; Dahm, P.S. Communication and socialization skills at ages 2 and 3 in “late-talking” young children. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1991, 34, 858–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Whitehouse, A.J.O.; Robinson, M.; Zubrick, S.R. Late talking and the risk for psychosocial problems during childhood and adolescence. Pediatrics 2011, 128, e324–e332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Webb, S. Receptive and productive vocabulary learning: The effects of reading and writing on word knowledge. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 2005, 27, 33–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clegg, J.; Law, J.; Rush, R.; Peters, T.J.; Roulstone, S. The contribution of early language development to children’s emotional and behavioural functioning at 6 years: An analysis of data from the Children in Focus sample from the ALSPAC birth cohort. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2015, 56, 67–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Næss, K.A.B.; Lervåg, A.; Lyster, S.A.H.; Hulme, C. Longitudinal relationships between language and verbal short-term memory skills in children with Down syndrome. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2015, 135, 43–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neil, N.; Jones, E.A. Communication intervention for individuals with Down syndrome: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev. Neurorehabil. 2018, 21, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, E.; Hokstad, S.; Næss, K.A.B. Children with Down syndrome can benefit from language interventions; Results from a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Commun. Disord. 2020, 85, 105992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parker, S.E.; Mai, C.T.; Canfield, M.A.; Rickard, R.; Wang, Y.; Meyer, R.E.; Anderson, P.; Mason, C.A.; Collins, J.S.; Kirby, R.S.; et al. Updated national birth prevalence estimates for selected birth defects in the United States, 2004–2006. Birth Defects Res. A Clin. Mol. Teratol. 2010, 88, 1008–1016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Gameren-Oosterom, H.B.M.; Fekkes, M.; Buitendijk, S.E.; Mohangoo, A.D.; Bruil, J.; Van Wouwe, J.P. Development, problem behavior, and quality of life in a population based sample of eight-year-old children with Down syndrome. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e21879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Abbeduto, L.; Warren, S.F.; Conners, F.A. Language development in Down syndrome: From the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2007, 13, 247–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, G.E.; Losh, M.; Estigarribia, B.; Sideris, J.; Roberts, J. Longitudinal profiles of expressive vocabulary, syntax and pragmatic language in boys with fragile X syndrome or Down syndrome. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2013, 48, 432–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Næss, K.A.B. Language and Reading Skills in Children with Down Syndrome; Reprosentralen: Oslo, Norway, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, J.; Price, J.; Barnes, E.; Nelson, L.; Burchinal, M.; Hennon, E.A.; Moskowitz, L.; Edwards, A.; Malkin, C.; Anderson, K.; et al. Receptive vocabulary, expressive vocabulary, and speech production of boys with fragile X syndrome in comparison to boys with Down syndrome. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 2007, 112, 177–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ypsilanti, A.; Grouios, G.; Alevriadou, A.; Tsapkini, K. Expressive and receptive vocabulary in children with Williams and Down syndromes. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2005, 49, 353–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Caselli, M.C.; Vicari, S.; Longobardi, E.; Lami, L.; Pizzoli, C.; Stella, G. Gestures and words in early development of children with Down syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1998, 41, 1125–1135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Checa, E.; Galeote, M.; Soto, P. The composition of early vocabulary in spanish children with Down syndrome and their peers with typical development. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2016, 25, 605–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galeote, M.; Soto, P.; Checa, E.; Gómez, A.; Lamela, E. The acquisition of productive vocabulary in Spanish children with Down syndrome. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2009, 33, 292–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galeote, M.; Sebastian, E.; Checa, E.; Rey, R.; Soto, P. The development of vocabulary in Spanish children with Down syndrome: Comprehension, production, and gestures. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 36, 184–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chapman, R.S.; Seung, H.K.; Schwartz, S.E.; Kay-Raining Bird, E. Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1998, 41, 861–873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller, J.F. Profiles of language development in children with Down syndrome. In Improving the Communication of People with Down Syndrome; Miller, J.F., Leddy, K., Leavitt, L.A., Eds.; Paul H. Brooks Publishing Co.: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1999; pp. 11–40. [Google Scholar]
- Bassano, D.; Eme, P.E.; Champaud, C. A naturalistic study of early lexical development: General processes and inter-individual variations in French children. First Lang. 2005, 25, 67–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hart, B.; Risley, T.R. Promoting productive language through incidental teaching. Educ. Urban Soc. 1978, 10, 407–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Böhning, M. Down-Syndrom. In Leitfaden. Sprache—Sprechen—Stimme—Schlucken; Siegmüller, J., Bartels, H., Eds.; Elsevier: Munich, Germany, 2010; pp. 191–193. [Google Scholar]
- Stoel-Gammon, C. Speech Acquisition and Approaches to Intervention. In Speech Language and Intervention in Down Syndrome; Rondal, J.A., Buckley, S., Eds.; Whurr Publishers Ltd.: London, UK, 2003; pp. 49–62. [Google Scholar]
- Kristoffersen, K.E.; Simonsen, H.G. Tidlig Språkutvikling hos Norske Barn. Macarthur-Bates Foreldrerapport for Kommunikativ Utvikling; Novus: Oslo, Norway, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Stoel-Gammon, C. Down syndrome phonology: Developmental patterns and intervention strategies. Down Syndr. Res. Pract. 2001, 7, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Berglund, E.; Eriksson, M.r.; Johansson, I.n. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2001, 44, 179–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chapman, R.S. Language development in children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 1998, 3, 307–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Von Tetzchner, S. Utviklingspsykologi; Gyldendal Akademisk Forlag: Oslo, Norway, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Hart, B. The initial growth of expressive vocabulary among children with Down syndrome. J. Early Interv. 1996, 20, 211–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Skipp, A.; Windfuhr, K.L.; Conti-Ramsden, G. Children’s grammatical categories of verb and noun: A comparative look at children with specific language impairment (SLI) and normal language (NL). Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2002, 37, 253–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schaner-Wolles, C. Sprachentwicklung Bei Geistiger Behindering; Williams-Beuren-Syndrom und Down-Syndrom. In Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Spracherwerb; Grimm, H., Ed.; Hogrefe Verlag für Psychologie: Göttingen, Germany, 2000; pp. 663–685. [Google Scholar]
- Buckley, S.J.; Bird, G. Speech and Language Development for Infants with Down Syndrome (0–5 Years); Down Syndrome Educational Trust: Southsea, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Lewis, V. Development and Disability; Blackwell Publishing: Oxford, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Berk, S.; Lillo-Martin, D. The two-word stage: Motivated by linguistic or cognitive constraints? Cogn. Psychol. 2012, 65, 118–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rowe, M.L.; Raudenbush, S.W.; Goldin-Meadow, S. The pace of vocabulary growth helps predict later vocabulary skill. Child Dev. 2012, 83, 508–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polišenská, K.; Kapalková, S. Language profiles in children with Down syndrome and children with language impairment: Implications for early intervention. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2014, 35, 373–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rice, M.L.; Warren, S.F.; Betz, S.K. Language symptoms of developmental language disorders: An overview of autism, Down syndrome, fragile X, specific language impairment, and Williams syndrome. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2005, 26, 7–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gathercole, S.E. Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2006, 27, 513–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gathercole, S.E.; Baddeley, A.D. The Children’s Test of Nonword Repetition; Psychological Corporation: London, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Beitchman, J.H.; Jiang, H.; Koyama, E.; Johnson, C.J.; Escobar, M.; Atkinson, L.; Brownlie, E.B.; Vida, R. Models and determinants of vocabulary growth from kindergarten to adulthood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2008, 49, 626–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff-Ginsberg, E. Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Dev. 1991, 62, 782–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, E. The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev. 2003, 74, 1368–1378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kalb, G.; van Ours, J.C. Reading to young children: A head-start in life? Econ. Educ. Rev. 2014, 40, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Logan, J.A.R.; Justice, L.M.; Yumuş, M.; Chaparro-Moreno, L.J. When children are not read to at home. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2019, 40, 383–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Massaro, D.W. Reading aloud to children: Benefits and implications for acquiring literacy before schooling begins. Am. J. Psychol. 2017, 130, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bishop, D.V.M.; Price, T.S.; Dale, P.S.; Plomin, R. Outcomes of early language delay. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2003, 46, 561–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Reilly, S.; Wake, M.; Bavin, E.L.; Prior, M.; Williams, J.; Bretherton, L.; Eadie, P.; Barrett, Y.; Ukoumunne, O.C. Predicting language at 2 years of age: A prospective community study. Pediatrics 2007, 120, e1441–e1449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reilly, S.; Wake, M.; Ukoumunne, O.C.; Bavin, E.; Prior, M.; Cini, E.; Conway, L.; Eadie, P.; Bretherton, L. Predicting language outcomes at 4 years of age: Findings from early language in victoria study. Pediatrics 2010, 126, e1530–e1537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van der Schuit, M.; Segers, E.; van Balkom, H.; Verhoeven, L. How cognitive factors affect language development in children with intellectual disabilities. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 1884–1894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vandereet, J.; Maes, B.; Lembrechts, D.; Zink, I. Expressive vocabulary acquisition in children with intellectual disability: Speech or manual signs? J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 36, 91–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eriksson, M.; Marschik, P.B.; Tulviste, T.; Almgren, M.; Pérez Pereira, M.; Wehberg, S.; Marjanovič-Umek, L.; Gayraud, F.; Kovacevic, M.; Gallego, C. Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 30, 326–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patterson, J.L. Expressive vocabulary development and word combinations of Spanish-English bilingual toddlers. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 1998, 7, 46–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sim, F.; O’Dowd, J.; Thompson, L.; Law, J.; Macmillan, S.; Affleck, M.; Gillberg, C.; Wilson, P. Language and social/emotional problems identified at a universal developmental assessment at 30 months. BMC Pediatr. 2013, 13, 206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bornstein, M.H.; Hendricks, C. Basic language comprehension and production in >100,000 young children from sixteen developing nations. J. Child. Lang. 2012, 39, 899–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gathercole, S.E.; Baddeley, A.D. Phonological working memory: A critical building block for reading development and vocabulary acquisition? Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 1993, 8, 259–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carroll, J.M.; Snowling, M.J.; Stevenson, J.; Hulme, C. The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 39, 913–923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Walley, A.C.; Metsala, J.L.; Garlock, V.M. Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Read. Writ. 2003, 16, 5–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alcock, K. The development of oral motor control and language. Down Syndr. Res. Pract. 2006, 11, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cain, K.; Oakhill, J. Matthew effects in young readers. J. Learn. Disabil. 2011, 44, 431–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hagtvet, B.E.; Lyster, H.S.A.; Melbye-Lervåg, M.; Næss, B.K.A.; Hjetland, H.N.; Engevik, L.I.; Hølland, S.; Karlsen, J.; Klem, M.; Kruse, J. Ordforråd i førskolealder og senere leseferdigheter–En metaanalytisk tilnærming. Spesialpedagogikk 2011, 1, 34–49. [Google Scholar]
- Deckers, S.R.J.M.; Van Zaalen, Y.; Van Balkom, H.; Verhoeven, L. Predictors of receptive and expressive vocabulary development in children with Down syndrome. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2019, 21, 10–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- te Kaat-van den Os, D.; Volman, C.; Jongmans, M.; Lauteslager, P. Expressive vocabulary development in children with Down syndrome: A longitudinal study. J. Policy Pract. Intellect. Disabil. 2017, 14, 311–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wechsler, D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Dunn, L.M.; Dunn, L.M.; Whetton, C.; Burley, J. British Picture Vocabulary Scale; NFER Nelson: London, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Lyster, S.-A.H.; Horn, E.; Rygvold., A.-L. Vocabulary and vocabulary development in Norwegian children and adolescents [in Norwegian: Ordforråd ogordforrådsutvikling hos norske barn og unge]. Spesialpedagogikk 2010, 74, 35–43. [Google Scholar]
- Child Language & Learning’s Tests. Tests Adapted or Developed to a Longitudinal Study; University of Oslo: Oslo, Norway, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Revised; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Furnes, B.; Samuelson, S. Preschool cognitive and language skills predicting kindergarten and Grade 1 reading and spelling: A cross-linguistic comparison. J. Res. Read. 2009, 3, 275–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bakke, M.; Bergendal, B.; McAllister, A.; Sjögreen, L.; Asten, P. Development and evaluation of a comprehensive screening for orofacial dysfunction. Swed. Dent. J. 2007, 31, 75–84. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Goodman, R. The Strenghts and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1997, 38, 581–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- VanderWeele, T.J. Principles of confounder selection. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2019, 34, 211–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hurt, H.; Betancourt, L. Effect of socioeconomic status disparity on child language and neural outcome: How early is early? Pediatr. Res. 2016, 79, 148–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schwab, J.F.; Lew-Williams, C. Language learning, socioeconomic status, and child-directed speech. Wiley inter-disciplinary reviews. Cogn. Sci. 2016, 7, 264–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marjanovič-Umek, L.; Urška, F.-P. Gender Differences in Children’s Language: A Meta-Analysis of Slovenian Studies. CEPS J. 2017, 7, 97–111. [Google Scholar]
- Adani, S.; Cepanec, M. Sex differences in early communication development: Behavioral and neurobiological indicators of more vulnerable communication system development in boys. Croat. Med. J. 2019, 60, 141–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Norbury, C.F.; Gooch, D.; Wray, C.; Baird, G.; Charman, T.; Simonoff, E.; Vamvakas, G.; Pickles, A. The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: Evidence from a population study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr. 2016, 57, 1247–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Little, T.D.; Jorgensen, T.D.; Lang, K.M.; Moore, E.W.G. On the joys of missing data. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2014, 39, 151–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cardoso-Martins, C.; Mervis, C.B.; Mervis, C.A. Early vocabulary acquisition by children with Down syndrome. Am. J. Ment. Defic. 1985, 90, 177–184. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Coggins, T.E.; Stoel-Gammon, C. Clarification strategies used by four Down’s syndrome children for maintaining normal conversational interaction. Educ. Train. Ment. Retard. 1982, 17, 65–67. [Google Scholar]
- Næss, K.A.B.; Lyster, S.A.H.; Hulme, C.; Melby-Lervåg, M. Language and verbal short-term memory skills in children with Down syndrome: A meta-analytic review. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 2225–2234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tomblin, J.B.; Mainela-Arnold, E.; Zhang, X. Procedural learning in adolescents with and without specific language impairment. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2007, 3, 269–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wise, J.C.; Sevcik, R.A.; Morris, R.D.; Lovett, M.W.; Wolf, M. The relationship among receptive and expressive vocabulary, listening comprehension, pre-reading skills, word identification skills, and reading comprehension by children with reading disabilities. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2007, 50, 1093–1109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kent, R.D.; Vorperian, H.K. Speech impairment in Down syndrome: A review. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2013, 56, 178–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kumin, L. Communication Skills in Children with Down Syndrome. A Guide for Parents; Woodbine House: Rockville, MD, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Buckley, S. Speech and Language Development for Individuals with Down Syndrome—An Overview; Down Syndrome Educational Trust: Southsea, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Næss, K.A.B.; Nygaard, E.; Ostad, J.; Dolva, A.S.; Lyster, S.A.H. The profile of social functioning in children with Down syndrome. Disabil. Rehabil. 2016, 39, 1320–1331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Chapman, R.S. Language Development in Children and Adolescents with Down Syndrome. In Improving the Communication of People with Down Syndrome; Miller, J.F., Leddy, K., Leavitt, L.A., Eds.; Paul H. Brooks Publishing Co.: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1999; pp. 41–60. [Google Scholar]
- Rondal, J.A. Language in Down Syndrome: Current Perspectives. In Down Syndrome: A Review of Current Knowledge; Rondal, J., Perera, J., Nadel, L., Eds.; Whurr Publishers Ltd.: London, UK, 1999; pp. 143–149. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, E.; Næss, K.A.B.; Jarrold, C. Assessing pragmatic communication in children with Down syndrome. J. Commun. Disord. 2017, 68, 10–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Barnes, E.; Roberts, J.; Long, S.H.; Martin, G.E.; Berni, M.C.; Mandulak, K.C.; Sideris, J. Phonological accuracy and intelligibility in connected speech of boys with fragile X syndrome or Down syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2009, 52, 1048–1061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cleland, J.; Wood, S.; Hardcastle, W.; Wishart, J.; Timmins, C. Relationship between speech, oromotor, language and cognitive abilities in children with Down’s syndrome. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2010, 45, 83–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, G.E.; Klusek, J.; Estigarribia, B.; Roberts, J.E. Language characteristics of individuals with Down syndrome. Top. Lang. Disord. 2009, 29, 112–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kumin, L. Intelligibility of speech in children with Down syndrome in natural settings: Parents’ perspective. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1994, 78, 307–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rupela, V.; Velleman, S.L.; Andrianopoulos, M.V. Motor speech skills in children with Down syndrome: A descriptive study. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2016, 18, 483–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wilson, E.M.A.L.; Camarata, S.M.; Shriberg, L.D. Estimates of the prevalence of speech and motor speech disorders in adolescents with Down syndrome. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 2019, 33, 772–789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rosin, P.; Swift, E. Communication Interventions: Improving the Speech Intelligibility of Children with Down Syndrome. In Improving the Communication of People with Down Syndrome; Miller, J.F., Leddy, K., Leavitt, L.A., Eds.; Paul H. Brooks Publishing Co.: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1999; pp. 133–160. [Google Scholar]
- Houwen, S.; Visser, L.; van der Putten, A.; Vlaskamp, C. The interrelationships between motor, cognitive, and language development in children with and without intellectual and developmental disabilities. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2016, 53-54, 19–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kiphard, E.J. Zur Effizienz Psychomotorischer Förderprogramme bei Sprachgestörten Kindern–Theorethische Begründungen und Praxisbezogene Hinweise. In Theorie und Praxis in der Sprachpädagogischen Arbeit; Frühwirt, I., Meixner, F., Eds.; Jugend und Volk: Vienna, Austria, 1992; pp. 33–36. [Google Scholar]
- Richter, E.; Brügge, W.; Mohs, K. So Lernen Kinder Sprechen. Die Normale und die Gestörte Sprachentwicklung; Reinhardt Verlag: Munich, Germany, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Ingram, R.E.; Luxton, D.D. Vulnerability-Stress Models. In Development of Psychopathology: A Vulnerability-Stress Perspective; Hankin, B.L., Abela, J.R.Z., Eds.; Sage Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2005; pp. 32–46. [Google Scholar]
- Bergelson, E.; Swingley, D. At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 3253–3258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gershkoff-Stowe, L.; Hahn, E.R. Word comprehension and production asymmetries in children and adults. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2013, 114, 489–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A.; Gathercole, S.; Papagno, C. The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychol. Rev. 1998, 105, 158–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gathercole, S.E.; Baddeley, A.D. Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. J. Mem. Lang. 1989, 28, 200–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, G.D.A.; Hulme, C. Nonword Repetition, STM, and Word Age-of-Acquisition: A Computational Model. In Models of Short-Term Memory; Gathercole, S.E., Ed.; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 1996; pp. 129–148. [Google Scholar]
- Hulme, C.; Maughan, S.; Brown, G.D.A. Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. J. Mem. Lang. 1991, 30, 685–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gathercole, S.E.; Hitch, G.J.; Service, E.; Martin, A.J. Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. Dev. Psychol. 1997, 33, 966–979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kormos, J.; Sáfár, A. Phonological short-term memory, working memory and foreign language performance in intensive language learning. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2008, 11, 261–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Senechal, M.; LeFevre, J.A. Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Dev. 2002, 73, 445–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brinchmann, E.I.; Braeken, J.; Lyster, S.A.H. Is there a direct relation between the development of vocabulary and grammar? Dev. Sci. 2019, 22, e12709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jusczyk, P.W.; Friederici, A.D.; Wessels, J.M.I.; Svenkerud, V.Y.; Jusczyk, A.M. Infants’ sensitivity to the sound patterns of native language words. J. Mem. Lang. 1993, 32, 402–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowey, J.A. Nonword repetition and young children’s receptive vocabulary: A longitudinal study. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2001, 22, 441–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alloway, T.P.; Gathercole, S.E.; Willis, C.; Adams, A.M. A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2004, 87, 85–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hulme, C.; Roodenrys, S. Practitioner review: Verbal working memory development and its disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1995, 36, 373–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adams, A.M. How language is embodied in bilinguals and children with specific language impairment. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Leddy, M. The Biological Bases of Speech in People with Down Syndrome. In Improving the Communication of People with Down Syndrome; Miller, J.F., Leddy, M., Leavitt, L.A., Eds.; Paul H. Brookes: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1999; pp. 61–80. [Google Scholar]
- Kumin, L. Differential diagnosis and treatment of speech sound production problems in individuals with Down syndrome. Down Syndr. Q. 2006, 8, 7–18. [Google Scholar]
- Hart, B.; Risley, T.R. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children; Brookes: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Ostad, J. Zweisprachigkeit bei Kindern mit Down-Syndrom; Verlag Dr. Kovač: Hamburg, Germany, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Ghassabian, A.; Rescorla, L.; Henrichs, J.; Jaddoe, V.W.; Verhulst, F.C.; Tiemeier, H. Early lexical development and risk of verbal and nonverbal cognitive delay at school age. Acta Paediatr. 2014, 103, 70–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henrichs, J.; Rescorla, L.; Schenk, J.J.; Schmidt, H.G.; Jaddoe, V.W.V.; Hofman, A.; Raat, H.; Verhulst, F.C.; Tiemeier, H. Examining continuity of early expressive vocabulary development: The generation R study. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2011, 54, 854–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Austin, P.C.; Steyerberg, E.W. The number of subjects per variable required in linear regression analyses. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2015, 68, 627–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schmidt, F.L. The relative efficiency of regression and simple unit predictor weights in applied differential psychology. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1971, 31, 699–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Althouse, A.D. Adjust for Multiple Comparisons? It’s Not That Simple. Ann. Thorac. Surg. 2016, 101, 1644–1645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hochberg, Y.; Benjamini, Y. More powerful procedures for multiple significance testing. Stat. Med. 1990, 9, 811–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Down Syndrome | Typically Developing | Group Difference | Group Difference Controlled for Gender, Parental Education Level and Nonverbal Mental Age ¹ | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 43) | (n = 57) | |||||||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | b | p-Value | b | p-Value | |
Age T1 (months) | 75.8 | 3.5 | 36.7 | 4.2 | −1.97 | <0.001 | −1.98 | <0.001 |
Mean parental education T1 1 | 2.5 | 1.1 | 2.6 | 1.1 | 0.07 | 0.73 | 0.08 | 0.71 |
Nonverbal mental age T1 4 | 12.2 | 5.4 | 12.6 | 4.6 | 0.08 | 0.71 | 0.11 | 0.61 |
Socioemotional functioning T1 2,4 | 10.7 | 5.1 | 6.7 | 3.6 | −0.84 | <0.001 | −0.84 | <0.001 |
Auditory memory T1 4 | −0.5 | 0.5 | 0.3 | 0.8 | 0.99 | <0.001 | 0.95 | <0.001 |
Oral motor skills T1 3,4 | 0.2 | 0.1 | 0.1 | 0.2 | −0.62 | 0.003 | −0.57 | 0.005 |
Phonological awareness T1 4 | −0.2 | 1.0 | 0.2 | 0.4 | 0.53 | 0.008 | 0.48 | 0.01 |
Receptive vocabulary T1 | 23.2 | 11.4 | 26.2 | 11.0 | 0.26 | 0.20 | 0.18 | 0.30 |
Expressive vocabulary T1 | 8.6 | 5.8 | 12.4 | 4.2 | 0.72 | <0.001 | 0.68 | <0.001 |
Expressive vocabulary T2 | 12.0 | 6.6 | 17.4 | 4.3 | 0.90 | <0.001 | 0.85 | <0.001 |
Expressive vocabulary T3 ¹ | 14.0 | 6.3 | 21.8 | 4.3 | 1.20 | <0.001 | 1.15 | <0.001 |
Down Syndrome | Typically Developing Children with a Similar Nonverbal Mental Age | Sign. Test of Differences in Beta between Groups | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 43) | (n = 57) | ||||||||
b | 95% CI | p-Value | b | 95% CI | p-Value | b | 95% CI | p-Value | |
Gender ¹ | 0.10 | −0.33 to 0.54 | 0.64 | 0.06 | −0.32 to 0.43 | 0.77 | −0.05 | −0.62 to 0.53 | 0.87 |
Nonverbal mental age T1 ² | 0.41 | 0.23 to 0.59 | <0.001 | 0.21 | 0.03 to 0.39 | 0.02 | −0.20 | −0.45 to 0.06 | 0.13 |
Mean parental education T1 | 0.15 | −0.07 to 0.37 | 0.18 | 0.03 | −0.15 to 0.22 | 0.73 | −0.12 | −0.41 to 0.17 | 0.42 |
Home literacy environment T1 ² | 0.28 | 0.04 to 0.52 | 0.02 | 0.17 | 0.01 to 0.34 | 0.04 | −0.10 | −0.39 to 0.19 | 0.48 |
Language problems in family T1 ¹ | −0.43 | −0.94 to 0.07 | 0.09 | −0.01 | −0.43 to 0.41 | 0.97 | 0.43 | −0.23 to 1.09 | 0.20 |
Socioemotional functioning T1 ² | 0.20 | −0.00 to 0.40 | 0.05 | 0.03 | −0.21 to 0.28 | 0.80 | -0.17 | −0.49 to 0.15 | 0.29 |
Auditory memory T1 ² | 0.96 | 0.67 to 1.25 | <0.001 | 0.28 | 0.14 to 0.43 | <0.001 | −0.68 | −1.00 to −0.36 | <0.001 |
Oral motor skills T1 ² | 0.61 | 0.37 to 0.84 | <0.001 | 0.16 | 0.00 to 0.32 | 0.04 | −0.44 | −0.73 to −0.16 | 0.002 |
Phonological awareness T1 ² | 0.36 | 0.21 to 0.51 | <0.001 | 0.31 | 0.03 to 0.60 | 0.03 | -0.05 | −0.37 to 0.27 | 0.77 |
Receptive vocabulary T1 ² | 0.61 | 0.45 to 0.77 | <0.001 | 0.34 | 0.19 to 0.48 | <0.001 | −0.27 | −0.49 to −0.05 | 0.02 |
Early reading skills T1 ² | 0.17 | −0.00 to 0.34 | 0.06 | 0.28 | −0.05 to 0.62 | 0.10 | 0.12 | −0.26 to 0.49 | 0.54 |
Time (change in expressive vocabulary) ¹ | |||||||||
T1–T2 | 0.51 | 0.34 to 0.67 | <0.001 | 0.75 | 0.60 to 0.89 | <0.001 | 0.24 | 0.02 to 0.46 | 0.03 |
T2–T3 | 0.30 | 0.13 to 0.46 | <0.001 | 0.64 | 0.50 to 0.78 | <0.001 | 0.34 | 0.12 to 0.56 | 0.002 |
T1–T3 | 0.80 | 0.64 to 0.97 | <0.001 | 1.38 | 1.24 to 1.53 | <0.001 | 0.58 | 0.36 to 0.80 | <0.001 |
Down Syndrome | Typically Developing Children with a Similar Nonverbal Mental Age | Sign. Test of Differences in Beta between Groups | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 43) | (n = 57) | ||||||||
b | 95% CI | p-Value | b | 95% CI | p-Value | b | 95% CI | p-Value | |
Gender ¹ | −0.03 | −0.42 to 0.37 | 0.90 | 0.06 | −0.28 to 0.40 | 0.71 | 0.09 | −0.43 to 0.61 | 0.73 |
Nonverbal mental age T1 ² | 0.41 | 0.22 to 0.59 | <0.001 | 0.22 | 0.04 to 0.40 | 0.02 | −0.18 | −0.44 to 0.07 | 0.16 |
Mean parental education T1 ² | 0.14 | −0.06 to 0.34 | 0.17 | 0.06 | −0.11 to 0.23 | 0.47 | −0.08 | −0.34 to 0.18 | 0.55 |
Home literacy environment T1 ² | 0.25 | 0.03 to 0.47 | 0.03 | 0.15 | −0.00 to 0.31 | 0.05 | −0.09 | −0.36 to 0.17 | 0.48 |
Language problems in family T1 ¹ | 0.35 | −0.12 to 0.81 | 0.15 | −0.03 | −0.41 to 0.35 | 0.87 | −0.31 | −0.92 to 0.29 | 0.30 |
Socioemotional functioning T1 ² | 0.08 | −0.11 to 0.28 | 0.39 | 0.10 | −0.13 to 0.33 | 0.38 | 0.02 | −0.29 to 0.33 | 0.90 |
Auditory memory T1 ² | 0.83 | 0.54 to 1.12 | <0.001 | 0.23 | 0.09 to 0.37 | 0.002 | −0.60 | −0.92 to −0.28 | <0.001 |
Oral motor skills T1 ² | 0.48 | 0.25 to 0.71 | 0.001 | 0.12 | −0.03 to 0.27 | 0.11 | −0.36 | −0.63 to −0.09 | 0.01 |
Phonological awareness T1 ² | 0.30 | 0.15 to 0.44 | <0.001 | 0.18 | −0.12 to 0.48 | 0.24 | −0.12 | −0.44 to 0.20 | 0.47 |
Receptive vocabulary T1 ² | 0.55 | 0.37 to 0.73 | <0.001 | 0.30 | 0.14 to 0.45 | <0.001 | −0.25 | −0.47 to −0.04 | 0.02 |
Early reading skills T1 ² | 0.13 | −0.04 to 0.29 | 0.13 | 0.17 | −0.14 to 0.48 | 0.29 | 0.04 | −0.30 to 0.39 | 0.81 |
Time ¹ | |||||||||
T1–T2 | 0.51 | 0.34 to 0.67 | <0.001 | 0.75 | 0.60 to 0.89 | <0.001 | 0.24 | 0.02 to 0.46 | 0.03 |
T2–T3 | 0.30 | 0.13 to 0.46 | <0.001 | 0.64 | 0.50 to 0.78 | <0.001 | 0.34 | 0.12 to 0.56 | 0.002 |
T1–T3 | 0.80 | 0.64 to 0.97 | <0.001 | 1.38 | 1.24 to 1.53 | <0.001 | 0.58 | 0.36 to 0.80 | <0.001 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Næss, K.-A.B.; Ostad, J.; Nygaard, E. Differences and Similarities in Predictors of Expressive Vocabulary Development between Children with Down Syndrome and Young Typically Developing Children. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 312. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030312
Næss K-AB, Ostad J, Nygaard E. Differences and Similarities in Predictors of Expressive Vocabulary Development between Children with Down Syndrome and Young Typically Developing Children. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(3):312. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030312
Chicago/Turabian StyleNæss, Kari-Anne B., Johanne Ostad, and Egil Nygaard. 2021. "Differences and Similarities in Predictors of Expressive Vocabulary Development between Children with Down Syndrome and Young Typically Developing Children" Brain Sciences 11, no. 3: 312. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030312
APA StyleNæss, K. -A. B., Ostad, J., & Nygaard, E. (2021). Differences and Similarities in Predictors of Expressive Vocabulary Development between Children with Down Syndrome and Young Typically Developing Children. Brain Sciences, 11(3), 312. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030312