Evaluating the Involving Relationships between Temperament and Motor Coordination in Early Childhood: A Prognostic Measurement
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Developing Perspectives of Temperament: The Three General Temperamental Dimensions
1.2. Characteristics of the Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD)
2. The Present Study
3. Subjects and Methods
3.1. Instruments
3.1.1. Children’s Behavior Questionnaire—Very Short Form (CBQ—VSF)
3.1.2. Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire (DCDQ)
4. Procedure
5. Results
5.1. Evaluation of the Psychometric Properties of the Greek Version of Children’s Behavior Questionnaire—Very Short Form (CBQ—VSF)
5.2. Evaluation of the Psychometric Properties of the Greek Version of Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire (DCDQ)
5.3. Relations between the Temperament Components and Indicators of Developmental Coordination Disorder
5.4. Investigating Gender Differences in the Three Components of Temperament
6. Discussion
6.1. Evaluation of the Psychometric Properties of the Greek Version of Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ)
6.2. Evaluation of the Psychometric Properties of the Greek Version of Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire (DCDQ)
6.3. Relations between the Temperament Components and Indicators of Developmental Coordination Disorder
6.4. Gender Differences
7. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Calkins, S.D.; Swingler, M.M. Psychobiological Measures of Temperament in Childhood. Handbook of Temperament; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Calkins, S.D.; Dedmon, S.E.; Gill, K.L.; Lomax, L.E.; Johnson, L.M. Frustration in infancy: Implications for emotion regulation, physiological processes, and temperament. Infancy 2002, 3, 175–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Shiner, R.; Caspi, A. Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: Measurement, development, and consequences. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2003, 44, 2–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rettew, D.C.; McKee, L. Temperament and its role in developmental psychopathology. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 2005, 13, 14–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rothbart, M.K. Temperament, development, and personality. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 16, 207–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chang, H.; Olson, S.L.; Sameroff, A.J.; Sexton, H.R. Child effortful control as a mediator of parenting practices on externalizing behavior: Evidence for a sex-differentiated pathway across the transition from preschool to school. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2011, 39, 71–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Zentner, M.; Bates, J.E. Child temperament: An integrative review of concepts, research programs, and measures. Int. J. Dev. Sci. 2008, 2, 7–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomas, A.; Chess, S. The New York longitudinal study: From infancy to early adult life. In The Study of Temperament: Changes, Continuities, and Challenges; Lomin, R.P., Dunn, J., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London, UK, 1986; pp. 39–52. [Google Scholar]
- Kagan, J.; Fox, N.A. Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases. In Handbook of Child Psychology: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development; Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., Lerner, R.M., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 167–225. [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart, M.K. Temperament. A developmental framework. In Explorations in Temperament; Stralau, J., Angleitner, A., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1991; pp. 61–74. [Google Scholar]
- Putnam, S.P.; Stifter, C.A. Reactivity and regulation: The impact of Mary Rothbart on the study of temperament. Infant Child Dev. 2008, 17, 311–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gartstein, M.A.; Putnam, S.P.; Rothbart, M.K. Etiology of preschool behavior problems: Contributions of temperament attributes in early childhood. Infant Ment. Health J. 2012, 33, 197–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Putnam, S.P.; Ellis, L.K.; Rothbart, M.K. The structure of temperament from infancy through adolescence. In Advances/Proceedings in Research on Temperament; Eliasz, A., Angleitner, A., Eds.; Pabst Scientist Publisher: Lengerich, Germany, 2001; pp. 165–182. [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart, M.K. Becoming Who We Are: Temperament, Personality, and Development; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart, M.K.; Derryberry, D.; Hershey, K. Stability of temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to parent report at seven years. In Temperament and Personality Development across the Life Span; Molfese, V.J., Molfese, D.L., Eds.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, MI, USA, 2000; pp. 85–119. [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart, M.K.; Bates, J.E. Temperament. In Handbook of Child Psychology: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development; Lerner, D.W., Eisenberg, R., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 99–166. [Google Scholar]
- Posner, M.I.; Rothbart, M.K.; Sheese, B.E.; Voelker, P. Control networks and neuromodulators of early development. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 48, 827–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rothbart, M.K. Temperament and the pursuit of an integrated Developmental Psychology. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2004, 50, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilmut, K.; Brown, J.H.; Wann, J.P. Attention disengagement in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. J. Disabil. Rehabil. 2007, 29, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barnhart, R.C.; Davenport, M.J.; Epps, S.B.; Nordquist, V.M. Developmental coordination disorder. Phys. Ther. 2003, 83, 722–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wills, T.A.; Stoolmiller, M. The role of self-control in early escalation of substance use: A time-varying analysis. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2002, 70, 986–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cermak, S.A.; Larkin, D. Developmental Coordination Disorder; Delmar Thomson: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Dunford, C.; Missiuna, C.; Street, E.; Sibert, J. Children’s perceptions of the impact of developmental coordination disorder on activities of daily living. Br. J. Occup. Ther. 2005, 68, 207–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cocks, N.; Barton, B.; Donell, M. Self-concept of boys with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 2009, 29, 6–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heath, N.L.; Toste, J.R.; Missiuna, C. An exploration of the relationship between motor impairment and emotional/behavioral difficulties amongst children suspected of having DCD. Isr. J. Occup. Ther. 2005, 14, 153–171. [Google Scholar]
- Nakagawa, A.; Sukigara, M.; Miyachi, T.; Nakai, A. Relations between temperament, sensory processing, and motor coordination in 3-year-old children. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 623–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Johnson, M.H.; Gliga, T.; Jones, E.; Charman, T. Annual research review: Infant development, autism, and ADHD: Early pathways to emerging disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2014, 56, 228–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- DeSantis, A.; Harkins, D.; Tronick, E.; Kaplan, E.; Beeghly, M. Exploring an integrative model of infant behavior: What is the relationship among temperament, sensory processing, and neurobehavioral measures. Infant Behav. Dev. 2011, 34, 280–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nakagawa, A.; Sukigara, M. Variable coordination of eye and head movements during the early development of attention: A longitudinal study of infants aged 12–36 months. Infant Behav. Dev. 2013, 36, 517–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nakagawa, A.; Miyachi, T.; Sukigara, M.; Seo, T. What is the nature of the relationship linking posture and movement patterns to temperament in the first year of life? Presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 19–21 March 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Cosentino-Rocha, L.; Linhares, M.B.M. Child temperament and gender differences. Paidéia 2013, 23, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Else-Quest, N.M.; Hyde, J.S.; Goldsmith, H.H.; Van Hulle, C.A. Gender differences in temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2006, 132, 33–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Walker, S.; Berthelsen, D.C.; Irving, K.A. Temperament and peer acceptance in early childhood: Sex and social status differences. Child Study J. 2001, 31, 177–192. [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart, M.K.; Ahadi, S.A.; Hershey, K.L.; Fisher, P. Investigations of temperament at 3–7 years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 1394–1408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Putnam, S.P.; Rothbart, M.K. Development of short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. J. Personality Assess. 2006, 87, 102–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Τsiara, E. The Temperamental Traits of Young Children and Their Relationships with Their Social Skills and Perceptions. Master’s Thesis, University of Thessaly, Thessaly, Greece, 2016. Unpublished. [Google Scholar]
- Kalia, E. Parental learning Support, parental Characteristics and learning Skills of Children. Master’s Thesis, University of Thessaly, Thessaly, Greece, 2013. Unpublished. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, Β.N.; Crawford, S.G. The Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire (DCDQ). In Administration Manual for the DCDQ’07 with Psychometric Properties; Wilson: Calgary, AB, Canada, 2007; pp. 1–245. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, B.N.; Crawford, S.G.; Green, D.; Roberts, G.; Aylott, A.; Kaplan, B.J. Psychometric properties of the revised developmental coordination disorder questionnaire. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 2009, 29, 182–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wilson, B.N.; Kaplan, B.J.; Crawford, S.G.; Campbell, A.; Dewey, D. Reliability and validity of a parent questionnaire on childhood motor skills. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2000, 54, 484–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Geisinger, K.F. Cross-cultural normative assessment: Translation and adaptation issues influencing the normative interpretation of assessment instruments. Psychol. Assess. 1994, 6, 304–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bentler, P.M. EQS.6 Structural Equations Program Manual; Multivariate Software: Encino, CA, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, T.A. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Hu, L.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kline, R. Principal and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Papantoniou, G.; Moraitou, D.; Katsadima, E.; Dinou, M. Action control and dispositional hope: An examination of their effect on self-regulated learning. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 8, 5–32. [Google Scholar]
- Platsidou, M. Confirmatory factor analysis in psychological research: Basic principles, limitations, and application examples. Sci. Ann. Dep. Psychol. 2001, 4, 367–394. [Google Scholar]
- Gouze, K.R.; Lavigne, J.V.; Hopkins, J.; Bryant, F.B.; Lebailly, S.A. The relationship between temperamental negative affect, effortful control, and sensory regulation: A new look. Infant Ment. Health J. 2012, 33, 620–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rothbart, M.K.; Ahadi, S.A.; Evans, D.E. Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 78, 122–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Posner, M.I.; Rothbart, M.K. Devel oping mechanisms of self-regulation. Dev. Psychopathol. 2002, 12, 427–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rivilis, I.; Liu, J.; Cairney, J.; Hay, J.; Klentrou, P.; Faught, B.E. A prospective cohort study comparing workload in children with and without developmental coordination disorder. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 33, 442–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bradley, M.; Codispoti, M.; Sabatinelli, D.; Lang, P. Emotion and motivation II: Sex differences in picture processing. Emotion 2001, 1, 300–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maccoby, E.E. Gender and relationships: A developmental account. Am. Psychol. 1990, 45, 513–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Years of Education | N | Frequency (%) | Professional Status | N | Frequency (%) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Basic Education | 10 | 4.3% | Unemployed | 24 | 10.4% |
Secondary Education | 69 | 29.9% | Professional Scientists | 15 | 6.5% |
University Degree | 117 | 50.6% | Scientists Employees | 69 | 29.9% |
Masters’ Degree | 25 | 10.8% | State/Private Employees | 78 | 33.8% |
PhD | 10 | 4.3% | Businessmen | 19 | 8.2% |
Farmers | 2 | 0.9% | |||
Engaged in Household | 24 | 10.4% |
Statements | Extraversion | Negative Affectivity | Effortful Control | Error | R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Likes going down high slides or other adventurous activities (Statement 4) | 0.653 | 0.757 | 0.427 | ||
Often rushes into new situations (Statement 7) | 0.553 | 0.833 | 0.305 | ||
Prefers quiet activities to active games (Statement 13) | 0.360 | 0.933 | 0.130 | ||
Is full of energy, even in the evening (Statement 25) | 0.451 | 0.893 | 0.203 | ||
Likes rough and rowdy games (Statement 28) | 0.403 | 0.915 | 0.162 | ||
Gets quite frustrated when prevented from doing something s/he wants to do (Statement 2) | 0.539 | 0.842 | 0.291 | ||
Is quite upset by a little cut or bruise (Statement 5) | 0.443 | 0.897 | 0.196 | ||
Is afraid of burglars or the “boogie man” (Statement 11) | 0.325 | 0.946 | 0.106 | ||
When angry about something, s/he tends to stay upset for ten minutes or longer (Statement 14) | 0.604 | 0.797 | 0.365 | ||
Seems to feel depressed when unable to accomplish some task (Statement 17) | 0.580 | 0.815 | 0.336 | ||
Is very difficult to soothe when s/he has become upset (Statement 23) | 0.667 | 0.745 | 0.445 | ||
Is not very upset at minor cuts or bruises (Statement 29) | 0.202 | 0.979 | 0.041 | ||
Is slow and unhurried in deciding what to do next (Statement 32) | 0.453 | 0.892 | 0.205 | ||
When drawing or coloring in a book, shows strong concentration (Statement 3) | 0.664 | 0.747 | 0.441 | ||
Prepares for trips and outings by planning things s/he will need (Statement 6) | 0.385 | 0.923 | 0.148 | ||
Likes being sung to (Statement 9) | 0.394 | 0.919 | 0.155 | ||
Notices it when parents are wearing new clothing (Statement 12) | 0.579 | 0.815 | 0.335 | ||
When building or putting something together, becomes very involved in what s/he is doing, and works for long periods (Statement 15) | 0.459 | 0.888 | 0.211 | ||
Is good at following instructions (Statement 18) | 0.566 | 0.824 | 0.321 | ||
Likes the sound of words, such as nursery rhymes (Statement 21) | 0.572 | 0.820 | 0.327 | ||
Comments when a parent has changed his/her appearance (Statement 36) | 0.439 | 0.898 | 0.193 | ||
Correlations between factors (latent variables) | Correlations between the items’ (observed variables’) measurement errors | ||||
F1 − F2 = 0.276 | E3 − E2 = 0.210 | ||||
F3 − F1 = 0.262 | E12 − E2 = 0.363 | ||||
F3 − F2 = 0.442 | E36 − E2 = 0.240 | ||||
E17 − E3 = 0.215 | |||||
E28 − E3 = −0.340 | |||||
E6 − E4 = 0.365 | |||||
E29 − E5 = 0.408 | |||||
E7 − E6 = 0.371 | |||||
E13 − E6 = −0.217 | |||||
E21 − E9 = 0.322 | |||||
E28 − E9 = −0.198 | |||||
E17 − E12 = 0.328 | |||||
E36 − E12 = 0.462 | |||||
E17 − E15 = 0.287 | |||||
E13 − E15 = −0.188 | |||||
E36 − E17 = 0.215 | |||||
E23 − E18 = −0.195 | |||||
E13 − E18 = −0.185 |
Extraversion | Negative Affectivity | Effortful Control | Control during Movement | Fine Motor/Handwriting | General Coordination | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Extraversion | ||||||
Negative Affectivity | 0.237 ** | |||||
Effortful Control | 0.249 ** | |||||
Control during Movement | 0.408 ** | |||||
Fine Movement | 0.316 ** | 0.505 ** | ||||
General Coordination | 0.266 * | 0.486 ** | 0.422 ** |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sofologi, M.; Koulouri, S.; Moraitou, D.; Papantoniou, G. Evaluating the Involving Relationships between Temperament and Motor Coordination in Early Childhood: A Prognostic Measurement. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 333. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030333
Sofologi M, Koulouri S, Moraitou D, Papantoniou G. Evaluating the Involving Relationships between Temperament and Motor Coordination in Early Childhood: A Prognostic Measurement. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(3):333. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030333
Chicago/Turabian StyleSofologi, Maria, Sophia Koulouri, Despina Moraitou, and Georgia Papantoniou. 2021. "Evaluating the Involving Relationships between Temperament and Motor Coordination in Early Childhood: A Prognostic Measurement" Brain Sciences 11, no. 3: 333. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030333
APA StyleSofologi, M., Koulouri, S., Moraitou, D., & Papantoniou, G. (2021). Evaluating the Involving Relationships between Temperament and Motor Coordination in Early Childhood: A Prognostic Measurement. Brain Sciences, 11(3), 333. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030333