Agency, Ownership and the Potential Space
Abstract
:1. Introduction
“I have tried to draw attention to the importance … of a third area, that of play, which expands into creative living and into the whole cultural life of man. … I have located this important area of experience in the potential space between the individual and the environment … [a space] which both joins and separates”.(Donald Winnicott, [1], pp. 102–103)
2. The Potential Space
3. Agency and Ownership of the Experience
4. Mental Models and Cognitive Maps
5. Agency and Ownership on Mental Maps
6. Agency and Ownership of Mental Maps and the Potential Space
6.1. Playing
- A two and a half-year-old child after having been frightened by having his head go underwater while being given a bath, became highly resistant to taking a bath. Some months later, after gentle but persistent coaxing by his mother, he very reluctantly allowed himself to be placed in four inches of bath water. The child’s entire body was tense; his hands were tightly clamped on to his mother’s. He was not crying, but his eyes were pleadingly glued to those of his mother. One knee was locked in extension while the other was flexed in order to hold as much of himself out of the water as he could. His mother began almost immediately to try to interest him in some bath toys. He was not the least bit interested until she told him she would like some tea. At that point the tension that had been apparent in his arms, legs, abdomen, and particularly his face, abruptly gave way to a new physical and psycho–logical state. His knees were now bent a little; his eyes surveyed the toy cups and saucers and spotted an empty shampoo bottle which he chose to use as milk for the tea; the tension in his voice shifted from the tense insistent plea, “My not like bath, my not like bath”, to a narrative of his play: “Tea not too hot, it’s okay now. My blow on it for you. Tea yummy”. The mother had some “tea” and asked for more. After a few minutes, the mother began to reach for the washcloth. This resulted in the child’s ending of the playas abruptly as he had started it with a return of all of the initial signs of anxiety that had preceded the play. After the mother reassured the child that she would hold him so he would not slip, she asked him if he had any more tea. He does, and playing is resumed.
6.2. Transitional Object and Phenomena
- Y has developed in quite a straightforward way throughout. He now has three healthy children of his own. He was fed at the breast for four months and then weaned without difficulty. Y sucked his thumb in the early weeks and this again “made weaning easier for him than for his older brother”. Soon after weaning at five to six months he adopted the end of the blanket where the stitching finished. He was pleased if a little bit of the wool stuck out at the corner and with this he would tickle his nose. This very early became his “Baa”; he invented this word for it himself as soon as he could use organized sounds. From the time when he was about a year old he was able to substitute for the end of the blanket a soft green jersey with a red tie. This was not a “comforter” as in the case of the depressive older brother, but a “soother”. It was a sedative which always worked. This is a typical example of what I am calling a Transitional Object. When Y was a little boy it was always certain that if anyone gave him his “Baa” he would immediately suck it and lose anxiety, and in fact he would go to sleep within a few minutes if the time for sleep were at all near. The thumb-sucking continued at the same time, lasting until he was three or four years old, and he remembers thumb-sucking and a hard place on one thumb which resulted from it. He is now interested (as a father) in the thumb-sucking of his children and their use of “Baas”.
6.3. The Analytic Space
6.4. From Illusory Experience to Cultural Experience, Art and Religion
6.5. The Area of Creativity
7. Neuroanatomical Considerations
8. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix
Glossary | |
Agency | the sense that “I am the one who is generating an experience represented on a cognitive map”. The agency process may insert different priors or beliefs one has with respect to the represented environment. |
Cognitive map | a schematic-like mental representation of the relationships between entities in the world including places, events, people, or even concepts. |
Mental model | a small-scale representation of the external world and the relationships between its various parts as well as the individual who represents them. A mental model enables an individual to use past experiences to predict future ones. |
Ownership | the sense that “I am the one who is undergoing an experience, represented on a cognitive map”. This implies an “ownership-schemator” that processes newly acquired world- and self-related information in an individual manner. |
Potential space | an intermediate area of experiencing that lies between imagination and reality. Specific forms of potential space include the play space, the area of the transitional object and phenomena, the analytic space, the area of cultural experience, and the area of creativity. |
Psyche-soma | the unification of the body with an imaginative elaboration of somatic parts, feelings, and functions. |
References
- Winnicott, D. Playing and Reality; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Ogden, T.H. On potential space. Int. J. Psychoanal. 1985, 66, 129–141. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson-Laird, P.N. Mental Models in Cognitive Science. Cogn. Sci. 1980, 115, 71–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson-Laird, P.N. Mental models and human reasoning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2010, 107, 18243–18250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Arzy, S.; Dafni-Merom, A. Imagining and Experiencing the Self on Cognitive Maps. In The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2020; pp. 311–331. [Google Scholar]
- Tversky, B. Cognitive maps, cognitive collages, and spatial mental models. Lect. Notes Comput. Sci. 1993, 716 LNCS, 14–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Behrens, T.E.J.; Muller, T.H.; Whittington, J.C.R.; Mark, S.; Baram, A.; Stacenhfeld, K.L.; Kurth-Nelson, Z. What is a cognitive map? Organising knowledge for flexible behaviour. Neuron 2018, 100, 490–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- O’Keefe, J.; Nadel, L. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map; Clarendon Press: Oxford, UK, 1978; Volume 3. [Google Scholar]
- Craik, K. The Nature of Explanation; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1943. [Google Scholar]
- Tolman, E.C. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol. Rev. 1948, 55, 189–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Moser, E.I.; Roudi, Y.; Witter, M.P.; Kentros, C.; Bonhoeffer, T.; Moser, M.B. Grid cells and cortical representation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014, 15, 466–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Arzy, S. When speaking of the experience, do not leave out the experiencer: On self and magnitude. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Arzy, S.; Collette, S.; Ionta, S.; Fornari, E.; Blanke, O. Subjective mental time: The functional architecture of projecting the self to past and future. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2009, 30, 2009–2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blanke, O. Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012, 13, 556–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gusnard, D.A.; Akbudak, E.; Shulman, G.L.; Raichle, M.E. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 4259–4264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Baars, B.J.; Ramsoy, T.Z.; Laureys, S. Brain, conscious experience and the observing self. Trends Neurosci. 2003, 26, 671–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallagher, S. Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 14–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arzy, S.; Schacter, D.L. Self-agency and self-ownership in cogntivie mapping. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019, 23, 476–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Turner, J.F.; Çukur, T. A brief history of illusion: Milner, Winnicott and Rycroft. Int. J. Psychoanal. 2002, 83, 1063–1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Caldwell, L. Changing the terms of psychoanalysis. From object to space and the nature of the transition. Nat. Hum. 2008, 10, 9–28. [Google Scholar]
- Green, A. The Work of the Negative; Free Association Books: London, UK, 1999; ISBN 1-85343-470-1. [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher, S. How the Body Shapes the Mind; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2005; ISBN 9780199271948. [Google Scholar]
- Kanitscheider, I.; Fiete, I. Training recurrent networks to generate hypotheses about how the brain solves hard navigation problems. In Proceedings of the 31st Conference on Neural Information Processing Systems (NIPS 2017), Long Beach, CA, USA, 4–9 December 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Schwabe, L.; Blanke, O. Cognitive neuroscience of ownership and agency. Conscious. Cogn. 2007, 16, 661–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Synofzik, M.; Vosgerau, G.; Newen, A. Beyond the comparator model: A multifactorial two-step account of agency. Conscious. Cogn. 2008, 17, 219–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moser, E.I.; Kropff, E.; Moser, M.-B. Place cells, grid cells, and the brain’s spatial representation system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2008, 31, 69–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schmajuk, N.A.; Buhusi, C.V. Spatial and temporal cognitive mapping: A neural network approach. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1997, 1, 109–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eichenbaum, H. On the Integration of Space, Time, and Memory. Neuron 2017, 95, 1007–1018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsao, A.; Sugar, J.; Lu, L.; Wang, C.; Knierim, J.J.; Moser, M.-B.; Moser, E.I. Integrating time from experience in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Nature 2018, 561, 57–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bartlett, F.C. Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1932. [Google Scholar]
- Tavares, R.M.; Mendelsohn, A.; Grossman, Y.; Williams, C.H.; Shapiro, M.; Trope, Y.; Schiller, D. A map for social navigation in the human brain. Neuron 2015, 87, 231–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Parkinson, C.; Kleinbaum, A.M.; Wheatley, T. Spontaneous neural encoding of social network position. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2017, 1, 72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peer, M.; Salomon, R.; Goldberg, I.; Blanke, O.; Arzy, S. Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 11072–11077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bellmund, J.L.S.; Gärdenfors, P.; Moser, E.I.; Doeller, C.F. Navigating cognition: Spatial codes for human thinking. Science 2018, 362, eaat6766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Constantinescu, A.O.; O’Reilly, J.X.; Behrens, T.E.J. Organizing conceptual knowledge in humans with a gridlike code. Science 2016, 352, 1464–1468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Garvert, M.M.; Dolan, R.J.; Behrens, T.E. A map of abstract relational knowledge in the human hippocampal–entorhinal cortex. Elife 2017, 6, e17086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Srull, T.K.; Wyer, R.S. Person memory and judgment. Psychol. Rev. 1989, 96, 58–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Metzinger, T. Being No One. The Self-Model Theory of Subjectivity; Bradford Books: Cambridge, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Wolpert, D.M.; Miall, R.C. Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control. Neural Netw. 1996, 9, 1265–1279. [Google Scholar]
- Wolpert, D.M.; Ghahramani, Z.; Jordan, M.I. An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 1995, 269, 1880–1882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rumelhart, D. Schemata: The Building Blocks of Cognition. In Theoretical Issues in Reading Comprehension; Spiro, R., Bruce, B., Brewer, W., Eds.; Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1980; pp. 33–58. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, S.E.; Crocker, J. Schematic bases of social information processing. In Social Cognition: The Ontario Symposium; Higgins, E.T., Herman, P., Zanna, P., Eds.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1981; pp. 89–134. [Google Scholar]
- Kant, I. Critique of Pure Reason; Guyer, P., Wood, A.W., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Javadi, A.-H.; Emo, B.; Howard, L.R.; Zisch, F.E.; Yu, Y.; Knight, R.; Pinelo Silva, J.; Spiers, H.J. Hippocampal and prefrontal processing of network topology to simulate the future. Nat. Commun. 2017, 8, 14652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Epstein, R.A.; Patai, E.Z.; Julian, J.B.; Spiers, H.J. The cognitive map in humans: Spatial navigation and beyond. Nat. Neurosci. 2017, 20, 1504–1513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Patai, E.Z.; Javadi, A.-H.; Ozubko, J.D.; Ji, S.; Robin, J.; Moscovitch, M.; Grady, C.; Winocur, G.; Rosenbaum, R.S.; Spiers, H.J. Hippocampal and Retrosplenial Goal Distance Coding After Long-term Consolidation of a Real-World Environment. Cereb. Cortex 2019, 29, 2748–2758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kalman, R.E. A New Approach to Linear Filtering and Prediction Problems. J. Basic Eng. 1960, 82, 35–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kumaran, D.; Banino, A.; Blundell, C.; Hassabis, D.; Dayan, P. Computations underlying social hierarchy learning: Distinct neural mechanisms for updating and representing self-relevant information. Neuron 2016, 92, 1135–1147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lenormand, M. Winnicott’s theory of playing a reconsideration. Int. J. Psychoanal. 2018, 99, 82–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winnicott, D.W. Transitional objects and transitional phenomena. J. Anal. Psychol. 1953, 34, 89–97. [Google Scholar]
- Winnicott, D.W. Mind and its relation to the psychesoma. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 1954, 27, 201–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ogden, T.H. The Analytic Third: Working with Intersubjective Clinical Facts. Int. J. Psycho-Anal. 1994, 75, 3–19. [Google Scholar]
- Buber, M. I and Thou; Scrinbers: New York, NY, USA, 1970. [Google Scholar]
- Buber, M. Between Man and Man; Martino Fine Books: Eastford, CT, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Winnicott, D.W. Hate in the counter-transference. J. Psychother. Pract. Res. 1994, 3, 348–356. [Google Scholar]
- Arzy, S.; Idel, M.; Landis, T.; Blanke, O. Why revelations have occurred on mountains? Linking mystical experiences and cognitive neuroscience. Med. Hypotheses 2005, 65, 841–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Idel, M. Messianic Mystics; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Idel, M. Hasidism: Between Ecstasy and Magic; State University of New York Press: Albany, NY, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Arzy, S.; Idel, M. Kabbalah: A Neurocognitive Approach to Mystical Experiences; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 2015; ISBN 9780300152364. [Google Scholar]
- Arzy, S.; Schurr, R. “God has sent me to you”: Right temporal epilepsy, left prefrontal psychosis. Epilepsy Behav. 2016, 60, 7–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Winnicott, D.W. (Ed.) Ego distortion in terms of true and false self. In The Maturational Process and the Facilitating Environment: Studies in the Theory of Emotional Development; International UP Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1960; pp. 140–152. [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher, S. Self-reference and schizophrenia: A cognitive model of immunity to error through misidentification. In Exploring the Self: Philosophical and Psychopathological Perspectives on Self-Experience; Zahavi, D., Ed.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands; Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2000; pp. 203–239. [Google Scholar]
- Cannon, T.D. How Schizophrenia Develops: Cognitive and Brain Mechanisms Underlying Onset of Psychosis. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015, 19, 744–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Farrer, C.; Frith, C.D. Experiencing oneself vs another person as being the cause of an action: The neural correlates of the experience of agency. Neuroimage 2002, 15, 596–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sperduti, M.; Delaveau, P.; Fossati, P.; Nadel, J. Different brain structures related to self- and external-agency attribution: A brief review and meta-analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 2011, 216, 151–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seghezzi, S.; Zirone, E.; Paulesu, E.; Zapparoli, L. The brain in (Willed) action: A meta-analytical comparison of imaging studies on motor intentionality and sense of agency. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Seghezzi, S.; Giannini, G.; Zapparoli, L. Neurofunctional correlates of body-ownership and sense of agency: A meta-analytical account of self-consciousness. Cortex 2019, 121, 169–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schore, A.N. Attachment affect regulation, and the developing right brain: Linking developmental neuroscience to pediatrics. Pediatr. Rev. 2005, 26, 204–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cirulli, F.; Berry, A.; Alleva, E. Early disruption of the mother–infant relationship: Effects on brain plasticity and implications for psychopathology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2003, 27, 73–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lyons-Ruth, K. Contributions of the Mother-Infant Relationship to Dissociative, Borderline, and Conduct Symptoms in Young Adulthood. Infant Ment. Health J. 2008, 29, 203–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Buckner, R.L.; Andrews-Hanna, J.R.; Schacter, D.L. The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008, 1124, 1–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Raichle, M.E. The brain’s default mode network. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2015, 38, 433–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Braga, R.M.; Buckner, R.L. Parallel Interdigitated Distributed Networks within the Individual Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Neuron 2017, 95, 457–471.e5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Andrews-Hanna, J.R.; Smallwood, J.; Spreng, R.N. The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2014, 1316, 29–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gusnard, D.A.; Raichle, M.E. Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 2, 685–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buckner, R.L.; Sepulcre, J.; Talukdar, T.; Krienen, F.M.; Liu, H.; Hedden, T.; Andrews-Hanna, J.R.; Sperling, R.A.; Johnson, K.A. Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: Mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 2009, 29, 1860–1873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Yomogida, Y.; Sugiura, M.; Sassa, Y.; Wakusawa, K.; Sekiguchi, A.; Fukushima, A.; Takeuchi, H.; Horie, K.; Sato, S.; Kawashima, R. The neural basis of agency: An fMRI study. Neuroimage 2010, 50, 198–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsakiris, M.; Longo, M.R.; Haggard, P. Having a body versus moving your body: Neural signatures of agency and body-ownership. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 2740–2749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Arzy, S.; Thut, G.; Mohr, C.; Michel, C.M.; Blanke, O. Neural basis of embodiment: Distinct contributions of temporoparietal junction and extrastriate body area. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 8074–8081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Renes, R.A.; van Haren, N.E.M.; Aarts, H.; Vink, M. An exploratory fMRI study into inferences of self-agency. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2015, 10, 708–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zito, G.A.; Wiest, R.; Aybek, S. Neural correlates of sense of agency in motor control: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0234321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zapparoli, L.; Seghezzi, S.; Zirone, E.; Guidali, G.; Tettamanti, M.; Banfi, G.; Bolognini, N.; Paulesu, E. How the effects of actions become our own. Sci. Adv. 2020, 6, eaay8301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Golland, Y.; Bentin, S.; Gelbard, H.; Benjamini, Y.; Heller, R.; Nir, Y.; Hasson, U.; Malach, R. Extrinsic and intrinsic systems in the posterior cortex of the human brain revealed during natural sensory stimulation. Cereb. Cortex 2007, 17, 766–777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Arzy, S.; Arzouan, Y.; Adi-Japha, E.; Solomon, S.; Blanke, O. The “intrinsic” system in the human cortex and self-projection: A data driven analysis. Neuroreport 2010, 21, 569–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beaty, R.E.; Seli, P.; Schacter, D.L. Network Neuroscience of Creative Cognition: Mapping Cognitive Mechanisms and Individual Differences in the Creative Brain. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2019, 27, 22–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Molnar-Szakacs, I.; Arzy, S. Searching for an integrated self-representation. Commun. Integr. Biol. 2009, 2, 365–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maillet, D.; Beaty, R.E.; Kucyi, A.; Schacter, D.L. Large-scale network interactions involved in dividing attention between the external environment and internal thoughts to pursue two distinct goals. Neuroimage 2019, 197, 49–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bram, A.D.; Gabbard, G.O. Potential space and reflective functioning: Towards conceptual clarification and preliminary clinical implications. Int. J. Psychoanal. 2001, 82, 685–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chescheir, M.W. Some implications of Winnicott’s concept for clinical practice. Clin. Soc. Work J. 1985, 13, 218–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Arzy, S. Agency, Ownership and the Potential Space. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 460. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11040460
Arzy S. Agency, Ownership and the Potential Space. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(4):460. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11040460
Chicago/Turabian StyleArzy, Shahar. 2021. "Agency, Ownership and the Potential Space" Brain Sciences 11, no. 4: 460. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11040460
APA StyleArzy, S. (2021). Agency, Ownership and the Potential Space. Brain Sciences, 11(4), 460. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11040460