Self-Boundary Dissolution in Meditation: A Phenomenological Investigation
Abstract
:1. Introduction
Live as such: Stretch a hand into the endlessoutwards of the worldand turn the outside inand the world into a chamberand God into a little soulinside the endless body–Yehuda Amichai [1].
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Training
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Interview Method
2.5. Analysis of Interviews
- 1.
- The textual data were organized thematically, based on a preliminary list of thematic categories which were addressed in earlier studies (sense of agency, sense of ownership, self-location, body sensations and first-person perspective, sense of time, self-other distinction [25]). A sample of 20 interviews chosen randomly was coded according to these themes by the first author and two research assistants;
- 2.
- The coded descriptions of each theme were examined separately across all participants in search of commonalities (concrete example follows). Each description (excerpts of one to three sentences) was abstracted and rephrased into short descriptive units capturing the essential meaning of each description. In a process of open data-driven coding, these abstracted descriptive units were clustered into a few specialized subcategories capturing the range of experiences pertaining to each experiential category. This process was done separately for both meditative conditions of SB+/SB- and for each salient category. In this stage, it became evident which categories and subcategories were robust and prevalent in the interviews. Some subcategories were conjoined, while others dropped (see Section 3);
- 3.
- The participants were characterized according to the resulting categories and subcategories. Based on the full interview transcription, a numerical value was assigned to the subcategory best reflecting the type of experience described for each experiential category. This was done for each meditative condition separately (SB+\SB−). To ensure intersubjective reliability, each participant was characterized by two independent raters (out of a total of four raters including the first and last authors). Additionally, raters evaluated on a 1–10 scale the degree of SB dissolution for each participant. The characterization was done based on the recorded and transcribed interviews in full. Each assigned value was accompanied by a chosen illustrative excerpt from the interview. Prior to the characterization, raters familiarized themselves with the concepts related to the experiential categories and with guidelines of phenomenological analysis. Raters followed concrete guidelines for a critical examination of descriptions for authenticity and particular relevance to actual lived experience, as opposed to general utterances of beliefs and preconceptions [42].
2.6. Quantitative Measures
- Index of meditative expertise: Participants filled out a table detailing the amount of previous retreat practice (for each retreat, days in the retreat, and number of hours practiced per day) as well as home practice (for different periods of home practice, estimated dates, and minutes per day practiced). Based on this data, the sum of practiced hours was calculated for each participant as a proxy of meditative expertise.
- Self-ratings: After each meditation epoch, participants reported the degree of quality and stability of the respective state on a 3-point Likert scale (quality: ‘To what extent did your boundaries dissolve\appear clear?’; stability: ‘How stable was the meditation?’, 1—not much, 3—greatly). While these ratings will be used in an epoch-specific manner for the neural analyses, we computed an average for each measure in order to assess individual differences.
- Phenomenological dimensions: Based on the subcategories’ ordering that emerged from the qualitative analysis of the interviews, we derived a numeric measure for each subcategory, signifying an ordinal ordering within each category. Multiply assigned subcategories were averaged (see quantitative results Section 3.3 for details).
2.7. Quantitative Analyses
3. Results
“Now I can let go of the barrier and then the sensation extends... [Gesture: arms open wide and away from the body] there’s no sense of boundary, but my boundary expands and expands. It’s not mine, really, but... there’s dissolution happening in something which is a space. [....] It’s not like you’re non-existent but you’re part of something, a flow, an energy, light, wave. [3 s pause] But there’s no meaning to your form, to light. There’s no form.” (#6)
3.1. Quantitative Validation of Methodological Approach
3.1.1. Self-Rating of Depth and Stability of Meditative States
3.1.2. Interrater Agreement
3.2. Phenomenological Characterization
Experiential Category | Subcategories | Experiential Category | Subcategories |
---|---|---|---|
Sense of Agency |
| Attentional disposition |
|
Self-location |
| Body sensations |
|
First-person perspective |
| Affective valence |
|
With Boundaries (SB+) | Without Boundaries (SB−) | |
---|---|---|
Meditation technique |
|
|
3.2.1. With Boundaries (SB+) Meditative Condition
Subcategory | Quotations | Demonstration |
---|---|---|
Technique A-Sensations scanning (n = 38) | M28: I begin with breathing and I base my focus there, and then I notice different parts of the body—the hands, the head—there was some pressure there. I attend all contact points of the body with the surface and blanket, so… it’s like the boundaries are at the periphery of my body. | Maintaining a clear sense of boundaries |
Agency #1- Active (continuously) (n = 34) | ||
Attention #1- Focused dynamic (n = 33) | M39: It’s bringing the attention to different parts of the body. And then trying to be very clear about where the boundaries are. [...] There’s a felt sense of the attention that is effortful and energetic. | Actively controlling attention |
Sensations #1- Prominent (n = 42) | M38: A sense of lucidity. It’s clearer where I am. I am here. This is me. This is where it begins, and this is where it ends. | Increased sensitivity to sensations |
Location #1- Within body (n = 41) | M20: Like some kind of spirit that I’m travelling with inside the body. [...] I feel only proximate space that is very very close. | Experiencing within their body |
M29: Attention is simply listening to the body, to its sensation, and that naturally forms a certain space that kind of blocks it in some border. | ||
Technique B- Feeling\visualizing form of body image (n = 20) | M6: I can use a broad view, to see myself for a second, as if I opened my eyes and looked at myself. [...] It demands some effort to stay with sensations and with this gaze… discerning, focusing at… a physical sensation that defines a shape. The bladder, there’s a shape there. It’s pressing. | Combining body scan with maintaining a visual or proprioceptive image of the body’s form |
Subcategory | Quotations | Demonstration |
---|---|---|
Agency #2- Responsive/intermediate (n = 13) | M41: Sometimes [attention] was moving and it didn’t happen deliberately. Moments where I find myself in the sense of body boundaries at the scalp. It came and went on its own. When I identified consciousness wandering off from body sensations, I brought it back. | Maintaining awareness of the appearance of mind wandering and sensations |
Attention #2- Wide and dynamic (n = 9) | M10: Feelings of weight and warmth… I passed from one area to the other, wherever there was tension, and then with attention it loosened a bit, and the feeling spread out. | Attention to areas and non-specific feelings |
Attention #3- Wide and static (n = 4) | M12: I simply feel all my body and then it really loosens up like with the volume and the length and width. Like… like being touched all over my body [... becomes] a sense of my presence as a body in space. | Passive presence within boundaries accompanied by a wider scope of attention, inclusive of surrounding space |
Technique C- Dwelling within body boundaries (n = 15) | ||
Affective valence #5-Positive (n = 13) | M41: That clarity [of bodily sensations] was nice. It was nice to focus on it because it was very concrete. It served as an anchor. Very… tangible. | A pleasant feeling of familiarity and control |
Affective valence #2-Slightly Negative (n = 13) | M40: When you feel the boundaries, you are also more present to the body’s discomfort. | Bodily discomfort |
3.2.2. Without Boundaries (SB−) Meditative Condition
“A sense of floating in some sky consciousness” (M13)\“There’s something very delicate, very innocent there, vulnerable. I can’t explain” (M32)\“An experience without an observer interfering, discerning.” (M46)\“Like leaning back inside the head” (M32)\“It’s a puddle and there’s no… it’s not solid anymore. (M11)\“some sort of intergalactic blackness [...] completely still and deeply serene.” (M43)
Meditation Technique
Subcategory | Quotations | Demonstration |
---|---|---|
Technique G—Release, relaxation or passivity (n = 29) | M23: I simply imagine my body dissolving into the mattress [...] It is something I’m directing but then it’s like it gets out of my control… It really happens in itself. | The meditative gestures employed create conditions which allow SB dissolution |
M44: Immediately there’s some relief… [speech slowing down] like now it’s okay to go back and expand [smiles and opens her arms]. It’s this movement of... [audible whisper of breathing out, closes her eyes and widens her arms to the sides] … Like there’s an air-conditioner working, and then it’s turned off. And then there’s some kind of... [lets her head fall back and sighs...] It’s okay to stop doing something. | Enacted gestures of relaxation | |
M28: There’s effort in letting go. It’s… it’s funny to say that. There’s a sense of giving up on the holding on. But then there’s no need for more effort—you just give up. | Letting go of intentional effort | |
M25: It’s like I’m falling down there. [...] Some kind of wide opening with no direction. I think there’s no other movement. No mental movement of wanting or not wanting. Some kind of presence of simply being. | Disengage from mental activity and perceptual content | |
Technique F—Imagination (n = 21) | M11: I imagined myself on one vacation [...] lying on a rhapsody looking at the water and then I just stayed with... this sense of melting of the water and this soft movement. | Imagined situations |
M13: Some kind of sky consciousness, a sort of flight… not looking downwards but sort of drifting. I was sort of floating in the sky… I saw some clouds. Something very loose, light. | Spatial imagination | |
Technique E—Turning attention outwards (n = 15) | M8: There’s more attention on the sound of the room… of breathing. There’s an experience of the body with no specific attention or specific sensations. […] It’s a sort of roaming with whatever comes up. | Open attention to drifting freely with the surrounding |
M10: I was feeling the room and then I had this feeling of awareness being in the rooms outside where you [experimenters] were, the lobby. So I felt aware of all that and the people there, and it started to open and open, I mean, to the grass outside the building and the campus. | A feeling of expansion outwards | |
M21: I turn the inward gaze outwards. [...] I’m trying not to focus on anything and from that place can come something obscure and hazy which is just... nothing. Like being focused on nothing. My effort goes to keeping it there. | Disengage from any perceived object through diverting attention | |
Technique D—Focus on sensations (n = 5) | M39: So when my hand is kind of immersed in the other hand [...] There is a sense of, when you sink, as you sink into that thing, you actually kind of penetrate the boundaries of it in a certain way. | Notice the indistinctness of a local sense of boundary |
Self-Location
Subcategory | Quotations | Demonstration |
---|---|---|
Location #4-Indeterminate self-world structure-radical change (n = 12) | M28: There’s a giving up on the body. There’s no relation to the body. Consciousness is more in peripheral space. [...] Like you’re floating. Consciousness is floating somewhere vague. | An amorphous sense of space and a faint sense of self-location or lack thereof |
M6: There’s a field of sensuality, and it’s not bounded. […] I’m not located anywhere. I am not. [...] It didn’t lose the sense of being-part-of, not entirely. | ||
M11: It’s a puddle and there’s no… it’s not solid anymore. [...] There’s no longer me. It… [7 s of silence] it stops being separate. It’s not me melting on the bed. There’s no me and there’s no bed. It all melts together. | Total immersion within space | |
M25: It’s like falling down there [...] Opening, release. It’s a space that’s… empty. There’s nothing in it. | ||
Location #2-Body & wider surrounding (n = 21) | M34: My attention shifted to being more in a space that… is also the body but also around the body. It’s not the universe, just around the body […] like some pleasant cloud, and its edges… not clear where it ends. | Located centered in the body, SB apparent but less defined |
M36: The sense of body boundaries is beginning to diminish and there’s a sense of intimacy or belonging that’s experienced somehow in relation to the room’s boundaries. | ||
Location #3-Expansion into vast space (n = 14) | M31: It’s as if I’m present in all of space altogether, like all of the space that my consciousness surrounds in this moment. […] It’s like my presence is something much bigger than, say, just where my body is located. Rather, who I am is present in a very large space, large at least like, say, a building, or something like that. | Dynamic expansion of oneself |
Attentional Disposition
Subcategory | Quotations | Demonstration |
---|---|---|
Attention #2-Wide, Dynamic (n = 22) | M8: There’s more attention on the sound of the room… of breathing. There’s an experience of the body with no specific attention or specific sensations. […] It’s a sort of roaming with whatever comes up. | Let attention drift around freely |
M20: I’m taking my attention, this energetic feeling, to wander [...] I can feel the whole room, and sometimes I left for other spaces, sometimes to the sky, and I was kind of travelling. | Discerning distinct features within the experience | |
Attention #3-Wide, static (n = 21) | M34: Attention is much more open and wide. It doesn’t include this sense of effort of being with something. All of a sudden attention rests and things appear in it… but it doesn’t stick. There’s much less preference for anything. | Receptive to whatever arises in experience |
M39: [Attention is] wider than the body, so it kind of permeates the space around the body. [8 silent seconds] It is quite soft, it feels quite light. | Wide attention | |
Attention #4-Formless (n = 9) | M33: Attention flows with everything […] It’s nowhere but it’s also not lost. […] A kind of very very strong sensory experience of flowing and unraveling of all that was condensed. | Vague or formless experience |
M32: Attention isn’t there. It’s gone. There’s no experience at all, so I don’t know what is there. I know that at some point attention gets back to some experience but there’s a stage in which there’s no… no space, nothing. | Lacking content and form completely |
Sense of Agency
Subcategory | Quotations | Demonstration |
---|---|---|
Agency #1-Active (n = 14) | M18: It’s not happening by itself, so I try to put myself in that state of expanding but then concentration is lost, so I need to activate it again. | Clear intention with a continuous and effortful sense of control |
M31: My action is to try and keep blowing up this balloon, through imagination and moving the mental gaze outwards to a wider space. [...] It’s like a struggle between this centering gravitational force, and the attempt to really push the dividing line outwards as much as I can, until it’s gone. | ||
Agency #2-Responsive (n = 19) | M41: I’m still directed towards a certain task [e.g dissolving boundaries]. It’s not this total surrender that I’m a vessel to anything that’s there. There’s an agenda… like an easy effort without ambition, yes, some sort of intention. | Somewhat passive while maintaining a task |
Agency #3-Passive (n = 15) | M6: A deep breath… and then everything loses its grip. [...] Until now I was putting an artificial barrier over sensation, and now it’s possible to let go of this barrier and let it extend… [opens her hands away from the body] | A sense of release and surrender |
M34: There’s an element of complete inaction. I sense within it the movement of breath, but its base is very still. I feel I can characterize it as deep serenity. | Lack of action |
First-Person Perspective
Subcategory | Quotations | Demonstration |
---|---|---|
1PP #3-Non-dual (n = 7) | M32: The process begins from the inside and then there’s nothing inside or outside. [...] There’s something very delicate, very innocent there, fragile… [...] There’s no experience of attention at all. [...] No someone that’s… no attending. | Not relative to a first-personal observer position. Formless experience. |
M6: There’s a field of sensuality, and it’s not bounded. […] I’m not located anywhere. I am not. It contributes to the dissolution. There’s no sense of observer. No witness. [...] It didn’t lose the sense of being-part-of, not entirely. [...] There’s a sense of flow and it is going through something. | ||
1PP #2-Intermediate (n = 12) | M26: In the beginning there was clearly a center referring to the boundarylessness, so there was some relation between two things, but in other moments space was the dominant thing without feeling the ‘you’ relating to space. | Blurry or unstable distinction between subject and object |
M35: [In some area] there was more intimacy… [5 s pause] there is greater cohesion between the sensation and the observer. |
Body Sensations
Subcategory | Quotations | Demonstration |
---|---|---|
Sensations #1-Prominent (n = 8) | M39: There’s an opening in the chest, opening in the shoulders, sometime a kind of opening of the face, and I’m like, two sides of the face can’t just sort of fall [extends the word] to either side, allow gravity in each side to pull its own direction | Concrete reference to local and specific bodily sensations |
Sensations #2-Indistinct (n = 20) | M37: I was less aware of bodily experience. Just something more loose, that the body feels more like one piece. | Sensations clarity diminish |
Sensations #3-Dissociated, imperceptible (n = 18) | M43: It’s this feeling of floating, of something sort of airy, wide open, spacious floating | Non-local feeling with no references to the body |
Affective Valence
Subcategory | Quotations | Demonstration |
---|---|---|
Affective #5-positive (n = 29) | M13: It was released, a flight. It was much much more pleasant. I realized the body is a burden and I became connected with a kind of vast consciousness. | A sense of soft or spacious being involving open relaxation |
M42: I felt more security or calm, like it’s my home. [...] Being without boundaries generated some sort of serenity, of spaciousness. | ||
Affective #2-Slightly negative (n = 3) | M27: I experienced stress in having to quickly change from a state of being entirely in the body to a state in which I’m really not in the body, supposedly. So I felt stress because I felt the time dimension. That I won’t make it. | Stress and other mild emotions |
M40: It was more emotional around this sense of being in space. A trace of anxiety… a strange thought of… preparing for what may come... | ||
Affective #1-Highly negative (n = 4) | M7: I felt my heart pounding as if I’m in a dramatic moment of my life. Really even slightly intimidating in intensity. […] I feel like being in a closed room all day, and then opening the door and realizing I’m above a jungle. | Stressful fear |
Rated Degree of Dissolution
3.3. Quantitative Relationships between Categories
4. Discussion
4.1. Phenomenological Mapping of Meditative SB Alteration
4.2. The Intricate Dynamics of Self-Boundaries and Body-Boundaries
4.3. Attentional Disengagement (Letting Go) Facilitates SB Dissolution
4.4. The Relation of Affect and Sense of Boundaries
4.5. An Enactive Approach to the Self
4.6. Operationalizing Phenomenology within the Neurophenomenological Context
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Amichai, Y. My son was drafted. In Open Closed Open; Schocken Press: Jerusalem, Israel, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Damasio, A. Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain; Pantheon Books: New York, NY, USA, 2012; ISBN 0-307-47495-X. [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher, S.; Zahavi, D. Phenomenological approaches to self-consciousness. In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy; Zalta, E.N., Ed.; Stanford University Press: Redwood City, CA, USA, 2010; Available online: http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2010/entries/self-consciousness-phenomenological/ (accessed on 1 April 2021).
- Metzinger, T. Being No One: The Self-Model Theory of Subjectivity; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2003; ISBN 0-262-63308-6. [Google Scholar]
- Siderits, M.; Thompson, E.; Zahavi, D. (Eds.) Self, No-Self: Perspectives from Analytical, Phenomenological, and Indian Traditions; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2011; ISBN 978-0-19-959380-4. [Google Scholar]
- Damasio, A.R. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness; Harcourt Brace: New York, NY, USA, 1999; ISBN 0-15-601075-5. [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher, S.; Zahavi, D. The Phenomenological Mind: An Introduction to Philosophy of Mind and Cognitive Science; Taylor & Francis: New York, NY, USA, 2008; ISBN 0-415-39121-0. [Google Scholar]
- James, W. The Stream of Consciousness. In Principles of Psychology; Henry Holt and Company: New York, NY, USA, 1890; Volume 1, pp. 224–290. [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher, S. Philosophical Conceptions of the Self: Implications for Cognitive Science. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 14–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blanke, O.; Metzinger, T. Full-Body Illusions and Minimal Phenomenal Selfhood. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009, 13, 7–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seth, A.K.; Tsakiris, M. Being a Beast Machine: The Somatic Basis of Selfhood. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2018, 22, 969–981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ratcliffe, M. Selfhood, schizophrenia, and the interpersonal regulation of experience. In Embodiment, Enaction, and Culture: Investigating the Constitution of the Shared World; Durt, C., Tewes, C., Fuchs, T., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2017; pp. 149–172. ISBN 978-0-262-03555-2. [Google Scholar]
- Kyselo, M. The Minimal Self Needs a Social Update. In Philosophical Psychology; Routledge: London, UK, 2016; Volume 29, pp. 1057–1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zahavi, D. Self and Other: Exploring Subjectivity, Empathy, and Shame; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Legrand, D. The Bodily Self: The Sensori-Motor Roots of Pre-Reflective Self-Consciousness. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2006, 5, 89–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ratcliffe, M. Feelings of Being: Phenomenology, Psychiatry and the Sense of Reality; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2008; ISBN 978-0-199-20646-9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blanke, O.; Arzy, S. The Out-of-Body Experience: Disturbed Self-Processing at the Temporo-Parietal Junction. Neuroscientist 2005, 11, 16–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerrans, P. Depersonalization Disorder, Affective Processing and Predictive Coding. Rev. Phil. Psych. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sass, L.A. Self-Disturbance and Schizophrenia: Structure, Specificity, Pathogenesis. Rech. Psychanal. 2013, 2, 119–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Vignemont, F. Bodily Awareness. In Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy; Zalta, E.N., Ed.; Stanford University Press: Redwood City, CA, USA, 2020; Available online: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2020/entries/bodily-awareness/ (accessed on 1 April 2021).
- Herbert, B.M.; Pollatos, O. The Body in the Mind: On the Relationship Between Interoception and Embodiment. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2012, 4, 692–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ionta, S.; Heydrich, L.; Lenggenhager, B.; Mouthon, M.; Fornari, E.; Chapuis, D.; Gassert, R.; Blanke, O. Multisensory Mechanisms in Temporo-Parietal Cortex Support Self-Location and First-Person Perspective. Neuron 2011, 70, 363–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sperduti, M.; Delaveau, P.; Fossati, P.; Nadel, J. Different Brain Structures Related to Self- and External-Agency Attribution: A Brief Review and Meta-Analysis. Brain Struct Funct. 2011, 216, 151–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tsakiris, M.; Costantini, M.; Haggard, P. The Role of the Right Temporo-Parietal Junction in Maintaining a Coherent Sense of One’s Body. Neuropsychologia 2008, 46, 3014–3018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ataria, Y.; Dor-Ziderman, Y.; Berkovich-Ohana, A. How Does It Feel to Lack a Sense of Boundaries? A Case Study of a Long-Term Mindfulness Meditator. Conscious. Cogn. 2015, 37, 133–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dor-Ziderman, Y.; Berkovich-Ohana, A.; Glicksohn, J.; Goldstein, A. Mindfulness-Induced Selflessness: A MEG Neurophenomenological Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dor-Ziderman, Y.; Ataria, Y.; Fulder, S.; Goldstein, A.; Berkovich-Ohana, A. Self-Specific Processing in the Meditating Brain: A MEG Neurophenomenology Study. Neurosci. Conscious. 2016, 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Millière, R.; Carhart-Harris, R.L.; Roseman, L.; Trautwein, F.-M.; Berkovich-Ohana, A. Psychedelics, Meditation, and Self-Consciousness. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lutz, A. Toward a Neurophenomenology as an Account of Generative Passages: A First Empirical Case Study. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2002, 1, 133–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, E.; Lutz, A.; Cosmelli, D. Neurophenomenology: An Introduction for Neurophilosophers. In Cognition and the Brain: The Philosophy and Neuroscience Movement; Brook, A., Akins, K., Eds.; Cambridge University: Cambridge, UK, 2005; pp. 40–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berkovich-Ohana, A.; Dor-Ziderman, Y.; Trautwein, F.-M.; Schweitzer, Y.; Nave, O.; Fulder, S.; Ataria, Y. The Hitchhiker’s Guide to Neurophenomenology—The Case of Studying Self Boundaries With Meditators. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abram, D. The Spell of the Sensuous: Perception and Language in a More-than-Human World; Vintage: New York, NY, USA, 1997; ISBN 978-0-679-77639-0. [Google Scholar]
- Husserl, E. Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy: Second Book Studies in the Phenomenology of Constitution; Springer Science & Business Media: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1989; ISBN 978-0-7923-0011-3. [Google Scholar]
- Ataria, Y. Traumatic and Mystical Experiences: The Dark Nights of the Soul. J. Humanist Psychol. 2014, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ataria, Y. Body Disownership in Complex Posttraumatic Stress Disorder; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2018; ISBN 978-1-349-95366-0. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindahl, J.R.; Fisher, N.E.; Cooper, D.J.; Rosen, R.K.; Britton, W.B. The Varieties of Contemplative Experience: A Mixed-Methods Study of Meditation-Related Challenges in Western Buddhists. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0176239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salgues, S.; Plancher, G.; Jacquot, L.; Naveteur, J.; Fanuel, L.; Gálvez-García, G.; Michael, G.A. To the Self and beyond: Arousal and Functional Connectivity of the Temporo-Parietal Junction Contributes to Spontaneous Sensations Perception. Behav. Brain Res. 2021, 396, 112880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martin, A.K.; Kessler, K.; Cooke, S.; Huang, J.; Meinzer, M. The Right Temporoparietal Junction Is Causally Associated with Embodied Perspective-Taking. J. Neurosci. 2020, 40, 3089–3095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arzy, S.; Thut, G.; Mohr, C.; Michel, C.M.; Blanke, O. Neural Basis of Embodiment: Distinct Contributions of Temporoparietal Junction and Extrastriate Body Area. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 8074–8081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guterstam, A.; Björnsdotter, M.; Gentile, G.; Ehrsson, H.H. Posterior Cingulate Cortex Integrates the Senses of Self-Location and Body Ownership. Curr. Biol. 2015, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Park, H.-D.; Blanke, O. Coupling Inner and Outer Body for Self-Consciousness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019, 23, 377–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petitmengin, C.; Remillieux, A.; Valenzuela-Moguillansky, C. Discovering the Structures of Lived Experience: Towards a Micro-Phenomenological Analysis Method. Phenom. Cogn. Sci. 2019, 18, 691–730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindahl, J.R.; Britton, W.B. “I Have This Feeling of Not Really Being Here”: Buddhist Meditation and Changes in Sense of Self. J. Conscious. Stud. 2019, 26, 157–183. [Google Scholar]
- Gentsch, A.; Schütz-Bosbach, S. I Did It: Unconscious Expectation of Sensory Consequences Modulates the Experience of Self-Agency and Its Functional Signature. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011, 23, 3817–3828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Timm, J.; Schönwiesner, M.; Schröger, E.; SanMiguel, I. Sensory Suppression of Brain Responses to Self-Generated Sounds Is Observed with and without the Perception of Agency. Cortex 2016, 80, 5–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bernasconi, F.; Noel, J.-P.; Park, H.; Faivre, N.; Seeck, M.; Spinelli, L.; Schaller, K.; Blanke, O.; Serino, A. Audio-Tactile and Peripersonal Space Processing around the Trunk in Human Parietal and Temporal Cortex: An Intracranial EEG Study. bioRxiv 2018, 28, 3385–3397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dittrich, A. The Standardized Psychometric Assessment of Altered States of Consciousness (ASCs) in Humans. Pharmacopsychiatry 1998, 31, 80–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spielberger, C.D. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. In The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Carlson, B.; Putnam, A. The Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES-II). Psychoanal. Inq. 2000, 20, 361–366. [Google Scholar]
- Petitmengin, C. Describing One’s Subjective Experience in the Second Person: An Interview Method for the Science of Consciousness. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2006, 5, 229–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Froese, T.; Gould, C.; Barrett, A. Re-viewing from within: A Commentary on First- and Second-Person Methods in the Science of Consciousness. Constr. Found. 2011, 6, 254–269. [Google Scholar]
- Bitbol, M.; Petitmengin, C. On the Possibility and Reality of Introspection. Mind Matter 2016, 14, 51–75. [Google Scholar]
- Petitmengin, C.; Remillieux, A.; Cahour, B.; Carter-Thomas, S. A Gap in Nisbett and Wilson’s Findings? A First-Person Access to Our Cognitive Processes. Conscious. Cogn. 2013, 22, 654–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lutz, A.; Jha, A.P.; Dunne, J.D.; Saron, C.D. Investigating the Phenomenological Matrix of Mindfulness-Related Practices from a Neurocognitive Perspective. Am. Psychol. 2015, 70, 632–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flick, U. The Concepts of Qualitative Data: Challenges in Neoliberal Times for Qualitative Inquiry. Qual. Inq. 2019, 25, 713–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valenzuela-Moguillansky, C.; Vásquez-Rosati, A. An Analysis Procedure for the Micro-Phenomenological Interview. Construct. Found 2019, 14, 44. [Google Scholar]
- Kirilenko, A.P.; Stepchenkova, S. Inter-Coder Agreement in One-to-Many Classification: Fuzzy Kappa. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0149787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing URL; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Epskamp, S.; Cramer, A.O.J.; Waldorp, L.J.; Schmittmann, V.D.; Borsboom, D. Qgraph: Network Visualizations of Relationships in Psychometric Data. J. Stat. Soft. 2012, 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landis, J.R.; Koch, G.G. The Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical Data. Biometrics 1977, 33, 159–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christoff, K.; Cosmelli, D.; Legrand, D.; Thompson, E. Specifying the Self for Cognitive Neuroscience. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 104–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Metzinger, T. The Myth of Cognitive Agency: Subpersonal Thinking as a Cyclically Recurring Loss of Mental Autonomy. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Metzinger, T.K. The Problem of Mental ActionThe Problem of Mental Action: Predictive Control without Sensory Sheets. In Philosophy and Predictive Processing; MIND Group: Kragujevac, Serbia, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petitmengin, C. Towards the Source of Thoughts: The gestural and transmodal dimension of lived experience. J. Conscious. Stud. 2007, 14, 54–82. [Google Scholar]
- Epskamp, S.; Fried, E.I. A Tutorial on Regularized Partial Correlation Networks. Psychol. Methods 2018, 23, 617–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shear, J.; Varela, F.J. The View from within: First-Person Approaches to the Study of Consciousness; Imprint Academic: Exeter, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Lutz, A.; Slagter, H.A.; Dunne, J.D.; Davidson, R.J. Attention Regulation and Monitoring in Meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008, 12, 163–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Przyrembel, M.; Singer, T. Experiencing Meditation—Evidence for Differential Effects of Three Contemplative Mental Practices in Micro-Phenomenological Interviews. Conscious. Cogn. 2018, 62, 82–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abdoun, O.; Zorn, J.; Poletti, S.; Fucci, E.; Lutz, A. Training Novice Practitioners to Reliably Report Their Meditation Experience Using Shared Phenomenological Dimensions. Conscious. Cogn. 2019, 68, 57–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kordes, U.; Oblak, A.; Smrdu, M.; Demsar, E. Ethnography of Meditation: An Account of Pursuing Meditative Practice as a Tool for Researching Consciousness. J. Conscious. Stud. 2019, 26, 184–237. [Google Scholar]
- Dahl, C.J.; Lutz, A.; Davidson, R.J. Reconstructing and Deconstructing the Self: Cognitive Mechanisms in Meditation Practice. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015, 19, 515–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vago, D.R.P.D.; David, S.A.M.D. Self-Awareness, Self-Regulation, and Self-Transcendence (S-ART): A Framework for Understanding the Neurobiological Mechanisms of Mindfulness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Petitmengin, C.; van Beek, M.; Bitbol, M.; Nissou, J.-M.; Roepstorff, A. Studying the Experience of Meditation through Micro-Phenomenology. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ataria, Y. Where Do We End and Where Does the World Begin? The Case of Insight Meditation. Philos. Psychol. 2014, 1–19. [Google Scholar]
- Lindahl, J.R.; Kaplan, C.T.; Winget, E.M.; Britton, W.B. A Phenomenology of Meditation-Induced Light Experiences: Traditional Buddhist and Neurobiological Perspectives. Front. Psychol. 2014, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gethin, R. The Foundations of Buddhism; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Josipovic, Z. Neural Correlates of Nondual Awareness in Meditation: Neural Correlates and Nondual Awareness. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2014, 1307, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Metzinger, T. Minimal Phenomenal Experience: Meditation, Tonic Alertness, and the Phenomenology of “Pure” Consciousness. PhiMiSci 2020, 1, 1–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Josipovic, Z.; Miskovic, V. Nondual Awareness and Minimal Phenomenal Experience. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 2087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albahari, M. Nirvana and Ownerless Consciousness. In Self, No Self?: Perspectives From Analytical, Phenomenological, and Indian Traditions; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2011; pp. 79–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watzl, S. Structuring Mind: The Nature of Attention and How It Shapes Consciousness; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2017; ISBN 978-0199658428. [Google Scholar]
- Lutz, A.; Mattout, J.; Pagnoni, G. The Epistemic and Pragmatic Value of Non-Action: A Predictive Coding Perspective on Meditation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deane, G.; Miller, M.; Wilkinson, S. Losing Ourselves: Active Inference, Depersonalization, and Meditation. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 539726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laukkonen, R.; Slagter, H.A. From Many to One: Meditation and the Plasticity of the Predictive Mind; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Ramstead, M.J.; Kirchhoff, M.D.; Friston, K.J. A Tale of Two Densities: Active Inference Is Enactive Inference. Adapt. Behav. 2020, 28, 225–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maister, L.; Tsakiris, M. The plasticity of self-other boundaries. In Conceptual and Interactive Embodiment: Foundations of Embodied Cognition; Fischer, M.H., Coello, Y., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2016; Volume 2, pp. 182–198. [Google Scholar]
- Colombetti, G. The Feeling Body: Affective Science Meets the Enactive Mind; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Kyselo, M. The Body Social: An Enactive Approach to the Self. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kornfield, J. A Path with Heart: The Classic Guide through the Perils And Promises of Spiritual Life; Rev. and updated ed edition; Rider: London, UK, 2002; ISBN 978-0-7126-5780-8. [Google Scholar]
- Fulder, S. The Five Powers: A Guide to Personal Peace and Freedom, 1st ed.; Aster: London, UK, 2020; ISBN 978-1-78325-351-7. [Google Scholar]
- Ataria, Y. When the Body Stands in the Way: Complex Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, Depersonalization, and Schizophrenia. Philos. Psychiatry Psychol. 2019, 26, 19–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ataria, Y.; Horovitz, O. The Destructive Nature of Severe and Ongoing Trauma: Impairments in the Minimal-Self. Philos. Psychol. 2021, 34, 254–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sierra, M.; David, A.S. Depersonalization: A Selective Impairment of Self-Awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 2011, 20, 99–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dor-Ziderman, Y.; Lutz, A.; Goldstein, A. Prediction-based neural mechanisms for shielding the self from existential threat. Neuroimage 2019, 202, 116080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holecek, A. Preparing to Die: Practical Advice and Spiritual Wisdom from the Tibetan Buddhist Tradition; Shambhala Publications: Boulder, CO, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Newen, A.; Gallagher, S.; De Bruin, L. 4E Cognition: Historical Roots, Key Concepts, and Central Issues. In The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition; Newen, A., De Bruin, L., Gallagher, S., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018; pp. 2–16. ISBN 978-0-19-873541-0. [Google Scholar]
- Stewart, J.; Gapenne, O.; Di Paolo, E. Enaction: Toward a New Paradigm for Cognitive Science; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Vörös, S.; Froese, T.; Riegler, A. Introduction to Special Issue on the Diversity of Enactivism and Neurophenomenology. Constr. Found. 2016, 11, 15. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, E. Sensorimotor Subjectivity and the Enactive Approach to Experience. Phenom. Cogn. Sci. 2005, 4, 407–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, E. Look Again: Phenomenology and Mental Imagery. Phenom. Cogn. Sci. 2007, 6, 137–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merleau-Ponty, M. Phenomenology of Perception; Routledge: London, UK, 2013; ISBN 978-0415834339. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, E. Waking, Dreaming, Being: Self and Consciousness in Neuroscience, Meditation, and Philosophy; Columbia University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014; ISBN 978-0-231-13709-6. [Google Scholar]
- Roy, M.J.; Petitot, F.; Pachoud, B.; Varela, F.J. Beyond the gap: An introduction to naturalizing phenomenology. In Naturalizing Phenomenology; Stanford University Press: Stanford, CA, USA, 1999; pp. 1–83. [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher, S. On the Possibility of Naturalizing Phenomenology; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Ramstead, M.J.D. Naturalizing What? Varieties of Naturalism and Transcendental Phenomenology. Phenom. Cogn. Sci. 2015, 14, 929–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albertazzi, L. Naturalizing Phenomenology: A Must Have? Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, M.; Friston, K.J. From Cognitivism to Autopoiesis: Towards a Computational Framework for the Embodied Mind. Synthese 2018, 195, 2459–2482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Nave, O.; Trautwein, F.-M.; Ataria, Y.; Dor-Ziderman, Y.; Schweitzer, Y.; Fulder, S.; Berkovich-Ohana, A. Self-Boundary Dissolution in Meditation: A Phenomenological Investigation. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 819. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11060819
Nave O, Trautwein F-M, Ataria Y, Dor-Ziderman Y, Schweitzer Y, Fulder S, Berkovich-Ohana A. Self-Boundary Dissolution in Meditation: A Phenomenological Investigation. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(6):819. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11060819
Chicago/Turabian StyleNave, Ohad, Fynn-Mathis Trautwein, Yochai Ataria, Yair Dor-Ziderman, Yoav Schweitzer, Stephen Fulder, and Aviva Berkovich-Ohana. 2021. "Self-Boundary Dissolution in Meditation: A Phenomenological Investigation" Brain Sciences 11, no. 6: 819. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11060819
APA StyleNave, O., Trautwein, F. -M., Ataria, Y., Dor-Ziderman, Y., Schweitzer, Y., Fulder, S., & Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2021). Self-Boundary Dissolution in Meditation: A Phenomenological Investigation. Brain Sciences, 11(6), 819. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11060819