Scent of a Woman—Or Man: Odors Influence Person Knowledge
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials
2.3. Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Standardization of Ratings
3.2. Stimulus Group versus Control Ratings
3.3. Descriptive Measures
3.4. Analytical Tests
3.5. Interpretive and Exploratory Analyses
3.5.1. Intensity Analysis
3.5.2. Analysis of Relationships between the Attributes
3.5.3. Analysis of Odorant Influence
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Asch, S.E.; Asch, S.E. Forming impressions of personality. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1946, 41, 258–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willis, J.; Todorov, A. First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 177, 592–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Todorov, A.; Pakrashi, M.; Oosterhof, N.N. Evaluating Faces on Trustworthiness after Minimal Time Exposure. Soc. Cogn. 2009, 27, 813–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofer, M.K.; Chen, F.S.; Schaller, M. What Your Nose Knows: Affective, Cognitive, and Behavioral Responses to the Scent of Another Person. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 29, 617–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tong, F.; Nakayama, K.; Moscovitch, M.; Weinrib, O.; Kanwisher, N. Response Properties of the Human Fusiform Face Area. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2000, 17, 257–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greven, I.M.; Downing, P.; Ramsey, R. Linking person perception and person knowledge in the human brain. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2016, 11, 641–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ferrari, C.; Lega, C.; Vernice, M.; Tamietto, M.; Mende-Siedlecki, P.; Vecchi, T.; Todorov, A.; Cattaneo, Z. The dor-somedial prefrontal cortex plays a causal role in integrating social impressions from faces and verbal descrip-tions. Cereb. Cortex 2016, 26, 156–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ames, D.L.; Fiske, S.T. Outcome dependency alters the neural substrates of impression formation. NeuroImage 2013, 83, 599–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cloutier, J.; Ambady, N.; Meagher, T.; Gabrieli, J. The neural substrates of person perception: Spontaneous use of financial and moral status knowledge. Neuropsychology 2012, 50, 2371–2376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engell, A.; Haxby, J.V.; Todorov, A. Implicit Trustworthiness Decisions: Automatic Coding of Face Properties in the Human Amygdala. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007, 19, 1508–1519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freeman, J.B.; Rule, N.O.; Adams, R.B., Jr.; Ambady, N. Culture shapes a mesolimbic response to signals of dominance and subordination that associates with behavior. NeuroImage 2009, 47, 353–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Whalen, P.J.; Rauch, S.L.; Etcoff, N.L.; McInerney, S.C.; Lee, M.B.; Jenike, M.A. Masked Presentations of Emotional Facial Expressions Modulate Amygdala Activity without Explicit Knowledge. J. Neurosci. 1998, 18, 411–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lübke, K.T.; Croy, I.; Hoenen, M.; Gerber, J.; Pause, B.M.; Hummel, T. Does human body odor represent a signif-icant and rewarding social signal to individuals high in social openness? PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e94314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lundström, J.N.; Boyle, J.A.; Zatorre, R.J.; Jones-Gotman, M. The neuronal substrates of human olfactory based kin recognition. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 30, 2571–2580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Natsch, A.; Emter, R. The specific biochemistry of human axilla odour formation viewed in an evolutionary context. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2020, 375, 20190269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boesveldt, S.; Parma, V. The importance of the olfactory system in human well-being, through nutrition and social behavior. Cell Tissue Res. 2021, 383, 559–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lübke, K.T.; Hoenen, M.; Pause, B.M. Differential processing of social chemosignals obtained from potential partners in regards to gender and sexual orientation. Behav. Brain Res. 2012, 228, 375–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Semin, G.R.; de Groot, J.H. The chemical bases of human sociality. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013, 17, 427–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Penn, D.J.; Oberzaucher, E.; Grammer, K.; Fischer, G.; A Soini, H.; Wiesler, D.; Novotny, M.V.; Dixon, S.J.; Xu, Y.; Brereton, R.G. Individual and gender fingerprints in human body odour. J. R. Soc. Interface 2006, 4, 331–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Allen, C.; Havlicek, J.; Roberts, S.C. Effect of fragrance use on discrimination of individual body odor. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Croy, I.; Mohr, T.; Weidner, K.; Hummel, T.; Junge-Hoffmeister, J. Mother-child bonding is associated with the maternal perception of the child’s body odor. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 198, 151–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Groot, J.H.; Semin, G.R.; Smeets, M.A. On the communicative function of body odors: A theoretical integra-tion and review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 12, 306–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doty, R.L.; Green, P.A.; Ram, C.; Yankell, S.L. Communication of gender from human breath odors: Relationship to perceived intensity and pleasantness. Horm. Behav. 1982, 16, 13–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hold, B.; Schleidt, M. The Importance of Human Odour in Non-verbal Communication. Z. Tierpsychol. 2010, 43, 225–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Doty, R.L.; Orndorff, M.M.; Leyden, J.; Kligman, A. Communication of gender from human axillary odors: Rela-tionship to perceived intensity and hedonicity. Behav. Biol. 1978, 233, 373–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Müller-Schwarze, D. Human Body Odor Discrimination: T-Shirt Experiment. In Hands-On Chemical Ecology; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 121–124. [Google Scholar]
- Sorokowska, A.; Oleszkiewicz, A. Body-odor based assessments of sex and personality—Non-significant differ-ences between blind and sighted odor raters. Phys. Behav. 2019, 210, 112573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Havlicek, J.; Lenochova, P. The Effect of Meat Consumption on Body Odor Attractiveness. Chem. Sens. 2006, 31, 747–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fialová, J.; Roberts, S.C.; Havlíček, J. Consumption of garlic positively affects hedonic perception of axillary body odour. Appetite 2016, 97, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mahmut, M.K.; Stevenson, R.J. Do Single Men Smell and Look Different to Partnered Men? Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hoffmann, H. The aroma of arousal: Effects of menstrual cycle phase and women’s sexual arousal state on men’s responsiveness to women’s body odor. Biol. Psychol. 2019, 142, 54–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sarolidou, G.; Axelsson, J.; Kimball, B.A.; Sundelin, T.; Regenbogen, C.; Lundström, J.N.; Lekander, M.; Olsson, M.J. People expressing olfactory and visual cues of disease are less liked. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2020, 375, 20190272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gaby, J.M.; Zayas, V. Smelling is telling: Human olfactory cues influence social judgments in semi-realistic in-teractions. Chem. Sens. 2017, 42, 405–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Draelos, Z.D. Antiperspirants and the hyperhidrosis patient. Dermatol. Ther. 2001, 14, 220–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirk-Smith, M.D.; Booth, D.A. Chemoreception in human behaviour: Experimental analysis of the social effects of fragrances. Chem. Sens. 1987, 12, 159–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lenochová, P.; Vohnoutova, P.; Roberts, S.C.; Oberzaucher, E.; Grammer, K.; Havlíček, J. Psychology of fra-grance use: Perception of individual odor and perfume blends reveals a mechanism for idiosyncratic effects on fragrance choice. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e33810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zarzo, M. Relevant psychological dimensions in the perceptual space of perfumery odors. Food Qual. Prefer. 2008, 19, 315–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindqvist, A. Perfume Preferences and How They Are Related to Commercial Gender Classifications of Fragrances. Chemosens. Percept. 2012, 5, 197–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zuniga, A.; Stevenson, R.J.; Mahmut, M.; Stephen, I. Diet quality and the attractiveness of male body odor. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2017, 38, 136–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wallace, P. Individual discrimination of human by odor. Physiol. Behav. 1977, 19, 577–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hepper, P.G. The discrimination of human odor by the dog. Perceptional 1988, 17, 549–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wedekind, C.; Füri, S. Body odour preferences in men and women: Do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity? Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 1997, 264, 1471–1479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Linke, C. A Study of Pitch Characteristics of Female Voices and Their Relationship to Vocal Effectiveness. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 1973, 25, 173–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whiteside, S.P. Identification of a Speaker’s Sex: A Study of Vowels. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1998, 86, 579–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruce, V.; Burton, A.M.; Hanna, E.; Healey, P.; Mason, O.; Coombes, A.; Fright, R.; Linney, A. Sex Discrimination: How Do We Tell the Difference between Male and Female Faces? Perception 1993, 22, 131–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dupuis-Roy, N.; Faghel-Soubeyrand, S.; Gosselin, F. Time course of the use of chromatic and achromatic facial information for sex categorization. Vis. Res. 2019, 157, 36–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lippa, R. Androgyny, sex-typing, and the perception of masculinity-femininity in handwritings. J. Res. Pers. 1977, 11, 21–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, D.B.; Sladden, B. An own gender bias and the importance of hair in face recognition. Acta Psychol. 2003, 114, 101–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leone, C.; Robertson, K. Some Effects of Sex-Linked Clothing and Gender Schema on the Stereotyping of Infants. J. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 129, 609–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goffman, E. Gender Advertisements; Harper & Row: New York, NY, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Pomerleau, A.; Bolduc, D.; Malcuit, G.; Cossette, L. Pink or blue: Environmental gender stereotypes in the first two years of life. Sex Roles 1990, 22, 359–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zellner, D.A.; McGarry, A.; Mattern-McClory, R.; Abreu, D. Masculinity/femininity of fine fragrances af-fects color–odor correspondences: A case for cognitions influencing cross-modal correspondences. Chem. Sens. 2008, 33, 211–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Taft, C. Color meaning and context: Comparisons of semantic ratings of colors on samples and objects. Color Res. Appl. 1997, 22, 40–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fiore, A.M. Effect of Composition of Olfactory Cues on Impressions of Personality. Soc. Behav. Pers. Int. J. 1992, 20, 149–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sczesny, S.; Stahlberg, S. The influence of gender-stereotyped perfumes on leadership attribution. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 32, 815–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kerr, K.-L.; Rosero, S.J.; Doty, R.L. Odors and the Perception of Hygiene. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2005, 100, 135–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Havlicek, J.; Dvorakova, R.M.; Bartoš, L.; Flegr, J. Non-Advertized does not Mean Concealed: Body Odour Changes across the Human Menstrual Cycle. Ethology 2006, 112, 81–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davidenko, N. Silhouetted face profiles: A new methodology for face perception research. J. Vis. 2007, 7, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bartoshuk, L.M.; American Psychological Association; Duffy, V.B.; Fast, K.; Green, B.G.; Prutkin, J.; Snyder, D.J. Labeled scales (e.g., category, Likert, VAS) and invalid across-group comparisons: What we have learned from genetic variation in taste. PsycEXTRA Dataset 2013, 2, 125–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lim, J.; Wood, A.; Green, B.G. Derivation and Evaluation of a Labeled Hedonic Scale. Chem. Senses 2009, 34, 739–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bem, S.L. The measurement of psychological androgyny. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1974, 42, 155–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McArthur, L.Z.; Baron, R.M. Toward an ecological theory of social perception. Psychol. Rev. 1983, 903, 215–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bar, M.; Neta, M.; Linz, H. Very first impressions. Emotion 2006, 6, 269–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Saint-Bauzel, R.; Fointiat, V. The Sweet Smell of Coldness: Vanilla and the Warm–Cold Effect. Soc. Behav. Pers. Int. J. 2013, 41, 1635–1640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klinger, L.J.; Hamilton, J.A.; Cantrell, P.J. Children’s Perceptions of Aggressive and Gender-Specific Content in Toy Commercials. Soc. Behav. Pers. Int. J. 2001, 29, 11–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boothroyd, L.; Jones, B.; Burt, D.M.; Perrett, D. Partner characteristics associated with masculinity, health and maturity in male faces. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2007, 43, 1161–1173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Georgesen, J.C.; Harris, M.J. The balance of power: Interpersonal consequences of differential power and ex-pectancies. Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 2000, 26, 1239–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Worrall, N.; Taylor, V.M.; Ricketts, J.P.; Jones, A.P. Personal Hygiene Cues in Impression Formation. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1974, 38, 1269–1270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Degel, J.; Piper, D.; Köster, E.P. Implicit learning and implicit memory for odors: The influence of odor identifica-tion and retention time. Chem. Sens. 2001, 26, 267–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hamilton, D.L.; Sherman, S.J. Perceiving persons and groups. Psychol. Rev. 1996, 103, 336–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Todorov, A.; Uleman, J.S. Spontaneous trait inferences are bound to actors’ faces: Evidence from a false recogni-tion paradigm. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 83, 1051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oosterhof, N.N.; Todorov, A.; Oosterhof, N.N.; Todorov, A. The functional basis of face evaluation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 11087–11092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kraft-Todd, G.T.; Reinero, D.A.; Kelley, J.M.; Heberlein, A.S.; Baer, L.; Riess, H. Empathic nonverbal behavior increases ratings of both warmth and competence in a medical context. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0177758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Clean | Pleasant | Feminine | Unclean | Unpleasant | Masculine |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Generous | Wise | Weak | Ungenerous | Shrewd | Strong |
Good-natured | Happy | Sensitive | Irritable | Unhappy | Realistic |
Pure | Humorous | Delicate | Impure | Humorless | Responsible |
Reliable | Sociable | Feminine | Unreliable | Unsociable | Competent |
Humane | Popular | Sentimental | Ruthless | Unpopular | Professional |
Altruistic | Important | Quiet | Self-centered | Insignificant | Intelligent |
Honest | Good looking | Emotional | Dishonest | Unattractive | Logical |
Persistent | Romantic | Unstable | Serious | ||
Talkative | Sincere | Hard-headed | Career-Oriented | ||
Imaginative |
Odor Group | Focus of Rating | Mean Attribute Ratings | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pleasantness | Masculinity | Femininity | Cleanliness | RMS (SD) | ||
Onion | Hyp. Person | 54.98 | 78.04 | 41.14 | 55.44 | |
Onion | 35.16 | 63.62 | 19.26 | 20.39 | 29.51 (6.97) | |
Lemon | 56.30 | 56.45 | 42.54 | 66.67 | 30.96 (12.94) | |
Amber | 56.02 | 28.41 | 61.05 | 64.10 | 38.52 (12.95) | |
Lily | 70.12 | 22.69 | 61.00 | 83.04 | 39.03 (10.46) | |
Lemon | Hyp. Person | 62.16 | 73.76 | 60.23 | 60.00 | |
Onion | 42.19 | 54.17 | 19.87 | 21.94 | 39.82 (11.70) | |
Lemon | 62.78 | 44.65 | 43.94 | 81.61 | 37.31 (12.01) | |
Amber | 57.01 | 31.52 | 47.90 | 72.97 | 30.63 (11.54) | |
Lily | 70.48 | 12.95 | 73.45 | 91.13 | 34.00 (15.42) | |
Amber | Hyp.Person | 61.58 | 78.97 | 53.11 | 58.47 | |
Onion | 39.19 | 52.43 | 22.20 | 21.88 | 36.22 (12.12) | |
Lemon | 66.56 | 51.79 | 42.54 | 83.79 | 39.98 (9.54) | |
Amber | 57.67 | 37.83 | 54.95 | 67.58 | 31.15 (10.29) | |
Lily | 71.62 | 15.94 | 64.21 | 89.51 | 33.16 (13.19) | |
Lily | Hyp. Person | 61.73 | 80.70 | 60.75 | 61.90 | |
Onion | 37.82 | 51.15 | 20.28 | 13.36 | 41.22 (11.17) | |
Lemon | 59.00 | 51.92 | 38.50 | 69.78 | 37.62 (8.04) | |
Amber | 61.09 | 27.32 | 68.18 | 79.48 | 32.83 (9.61) | |
Lily | 67.13 | 18.21 | 60.26 | 86.69 | 35.06 (9.73) |
Stimulus Group | ANCOVA Same-vs.-Other Disparity Mean | Estimated Intensity Effect | Principal Analysis Mean |
---|---|---|---|
Onion | −7.61 | 0.95 | −6.66 ** |
Lemon | 3.39 | −0.89 | 2.50 |
Amber | −5.76 | 0.45 | −5.31 ** |
Lily | −1.50 | −0.66 | −2.16 |
All Groups | −3.06 | 0.00 | −3.06 ** |
Attributes Correlated | Person (r) | Odor (r) |
---|---|---|
Pleasantness-Masculinity | 0.130 | −0.073 |
Pleasantness-Femininity | 0.333 * | 0.223 ** |
Pleasantness-Cleanliness | 0.437 ** | 0.392 ** |
Masculinity-Femininity | −0.131 | −0.497 ** |
Masculinity-Cleanliness | 0.275 ** | −0.303 ** |
Femininity-Cleanliness | 0.610 ** | 0.327 ** |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Hovis, N.L.; Sheehe, P.R.; White, T.L. Scent of a Woman—Or Man: Odors Influence Person Knowledge. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 955. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11070955
Hovis NL, Sheehe PR, White TL. Scent of a Woman—Or Man: Odors Influence Person Knowledge. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(7):955. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11070955
Chicago/Turabian StyleHovis, Nicole L., Paul R. Sheehe, and Theresa L. White. 2021. "Scent of a Woman—Or Man: Odors Influence Person Knowledge" Brain Sciences 11, no. 7: 955. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11070955
APA StyleHovis, N. L., Sheehe, P. R., & White, T. L. (2021). Scent of a Woman—Or Man: Odors Influence Person Knowledge. Brain Sciences, 11(7), 955. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11070955