Neurobiological Implications of Parent–Child Emotional Availability: A Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The EA Framework
1.2. The EA Scales
1.3. Adult EA Scales
1.4. Child EA Scales
1.5. EA Zones
1.6. EA Self-Report (EA-SR)
1.7. Prenatal EA
2. Methods
2.1. Literature Search
2.2. Study Selection
2.3. Data Extraction
2.4. Quality Evaluation
3. Results
3.1. Stress Physiology, the Neuroendocrine System, and EA
3.1.1. Stress Physiology
3.1.2. Skin Conductance and Heart Rate Variability
3.1.3. Testosterone
3.1.4. Oxytocin
3.2. Genetics and Epigenetics
3.2.1. Genetics
3.2.2. Epigenetics
3.3. Brain Mechanisms
3.3.1. Neglectful Mothers
3.3.2. Child Maltreatment-Exposed Mothers
3.3.3. Normative Samples
3.4. Theoretical Framework
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions and Some Limitations
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Van Hasselt, F.N.; De Visser, L.; Tieskens, J.M.; Cornelisse, S.; Baars, A.M.; Lavrijsen, M.; Krugers, H.J.; van den Bos, R.; Joels, M. Individual Variations in Maternal Care Early in Life Correlate with Later Life Decision-Making and c-Fos Expression in Prefrontal Subregions of Rats. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e37820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mehta, M.A.; Golembo, N.I.; Nosarti, C.; Colvert, E.; Mota, A.; Williams, S.C.R.; Rutter, M.; Sonuga-Barke, E.J.S. Amygdala, hippocampal and corpus callosum size following severe early institutional deprivation: The English and Romanian Adoptees Study Pilot. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2009, 50, 943–951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tottenham, N.; Hare, T.A.; Quinn, B.T.; McCarry, T.W.; Nurse, M.; Gilhooly, T.; Millner, A.; Galvan, A.; Davidson, M.C.; Eigsti, I.-M.; et al. Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Dev. Sci. 2010, 13, 46–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- McLaughlin, K.A.; Lambert, H.K. Child trauma exposure and psychopathology: Mechanisms of risk and resilience. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 14, 29–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- McCrory, E.; De Brito, S.A.; Viding, E. The link between child abuse and psychopathology: A review of neurobiological and genetic research. J. R. Soc. Med. 2012, 105, 151–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pollak, S.D.; Kistler, D.J. Early experience is associated with the development of categorical representations for facial expressions of emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2002, 99, 9072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Neukel, C.; Herpertz, S.C.; Hinid-Attar, C.; Zietlow, A.L.; Fuchs, A.; Moehler, E.; Bermpohl, F.; Bertsch, K. Neural processing of the own child’s facial emotions in mothers with a history of early life maltreatment. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2019, 269, 171–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Belsky, J.; De Haan, M. Annual Research Review: Parenting and children’s brain development: The end of the beginning. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2011, 52, 409–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morgan, J.K.; Shaw, D.S.; Forbes, E.E. Maternal Depression and Warmth During Childhood Predict Age 20 Neural Response to Reward. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2014, 53, 108–117.e101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leblanc, E.; Dégeilh, F.; Daneault, V.; Beauchamp, M.H.; Bernier, A. Attachment Security in Infancy: A Preliminary Study of Prospective Links to Brain Morphometry in Late Childhood. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 2141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sethna, V.; Pote, I.; Wang, S.; Gudbrandsen, M.; Blasi, A.; McCusker, C.; Daly, E.; Perry, E.; Adams, K.P.H.; Kuklisova-Murgasova, M.; et al. Mother–infant interactions and regional brain volumes in infancy: An MRI study. Brain Struct. Funct. 2017, 222, 2379–2388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rifkin-Graboi, A.; Kong, L.; Sim, L.W.; Sanmugam, S.; Broekman, B.F.; Chen, H.; Wong, E.; Kwek, K.; Saw, S.M.; Chong, Y.S.; et al. Maternal sensitivity, infant limbic structure volume and functional connectivity: A preliminary study. Transl. Psychiatry 2015, 5, e668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kok, R.; Thijssen, S.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Jaddoe, V.W.; Verhulst, F.C.; White, T.; van Ijzendoorn, M.H.; Tiemeier, H. Normal Variation in Early Parental Sensitivity Predicts Child Structural Brain Development. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2015, 54, 824–831.e821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lévesque, M.L.; Fahim, C.; Ismaylova, E.; Verner, M.P.; Casey, K.F.; Vitaro, F.; Brendgen, M.; Dionne, G.; Boivin, M.; Tremblay, R.E.; et al. The Impact of the in utero and Early Postnatal Environments on Grey and White Matter Volume: A Study with Adolescent Monozygotic Twins. Dev. Neurosci. 2015, 37, 489–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kok, R.; Prinzie, P.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Verhulst, F.C.; White, T.; Tiemeier, H.; Van Ijzendoorn, M.H. Socialization of prosocial behavior: Gender differences in the mediating role of child brain volume. Child Neuropsychol. 2018, 24, 723–733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hane, A.A.; Fox, N.A. Ordinary Variations in Maternal Caregiving Influence Human Infants’ Stress Reactivity. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 550–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levy, J.; Goldstein, A.; Feldman, R. The neural development of empathy is sensitive to caregiving and early trauma. Nat. Commun. 2019, 10, 1905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lupien, S.J.; McEwen, B.S.; Gunnar, M.R.; Heim, C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 434–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gunnar, M.R.; Adam, E.K. The Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenocortical System and Emotion: Current Wisdom and Future Directions. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2012, 77, 109–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lupien, S.J.; King, S.; Meaney, M.J.; McEwen, B.S. Child’s stress hormone levels correlate with mother’s socioeconomic status and depressive state. Biol. Psychiatry 2000, 48, 976–980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feldman, R.; Gordon, I.; Influs, M.; Gutbir, T.; Ebstein, R.P. Parental Oxytocin and Early Caregiving Jointly Shape Children’s Oxytocin Response and Social Reciprocity. Neuropsychopharmacology 2013, 38, 1154–1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Feldman, R. Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2007, 48, 329–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strathearn, L.; Fonagy, P.; Amico, J.; Montague, P.R. Adult Attachment Predicts Maternal Brain and Oxytocin Response to Infant Cues. Neuropsychopharmacology 2009, 34, 2655–2666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szyf, M. The early life environment and the epigenome. Biochim. Biophys. Acta 2009, 1790, 878–885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nafee, T.M.; Farrell, W.E.; Carroll, W.D.; Fryer, A.A.; Ismail, K.M. Epigenetic control of fetal gene expression. BJOG 2008, 115, 158–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Champagne, F.A.; Curley, J.P. Epigenetic mechanisms mediating the long-term effects of maternal care on development. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2009, 33, 593–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dolinoy, D.C.; Weidman, J.R.; Jirtle, R.L. Epigenetic gene regulation: Linking early developmental environment to adult disease. Reprod. Toxicol. 2007, 23, 297–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meaney, M.J.; Szyf, M.; Seckl, J.R. Epigenetic mechanisms of perinatal programming of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function and health. Trends Mol. Med. 2007, 13, 269–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wurster, H.E.; Biringen, Z. Validity of the Emotional Attachment Zones Evaluation (EA-Z): Assessing attachment style across a developmental spectrum. Perspect. Early Child. Psychol. Educ. 2020, 5, 5–54. [Google Scholar]
- Biringen, Z.; Derscheid, D.; Vliegen, N.; Closson, L.; Easterbrooks, M.A. Emotional availability (EA): Theoretical background, empirical research using the EA Scales and clinical applications. Dev. Rev. 2014, 34, 114–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bohr, Y.; Putnick, D.L.; Lee, Y.; Bornstein, M.H. Evaluating Caregiver Sensitivity to Infants: Measures Matter. Infancy 2018, 23, 730–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cranley, M.S. Development of a Tool for the Measurement of Maternal Attachment During Pregnancy. Nurs. Res. 1981, 30, 281–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Bakel, H.J.; Maas, A.J.; Vreeswijk, C.M.; Vingerhoets, A.J. Pictorial representation of attachment: Measuring the parent-fetus relationship in expectant mothers and fathers. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 2013, 13, 138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Salo, S.; Flykt, M.; Isosävi, S.; Punamäki, R.-L.; Kalland, M.; Biringen, Z.; Pajulo, M. Validating an Observational Measure of Prenatal Emotional Availabillity among Mothers with Depressive Symptoms. J. Prenat. Perinat. Psychol. Health 2019, 34, 55–77. [Google Scholar]
- Hovens, J.G.; Wiersma, J.E.; Giltay, E.J.; Van Oppen, P.; Spinhoven, P.; Penninx, B.W.; Zitman, F.G. Childhood life events and childhood trauma in adult patients with depressive, anxiety and comorbid disorders vs. controls. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2010, 122, 66–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Crews, D. Epigenetics and its implications for behavioral neuroendocrinology. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2008, 29, 344–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ainsworth, M.D.S.; Blehar, M.C.; Waters, E.; Wall, S. Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Wurster, H.E.; Sarche, M.; Trucksess, C.; Morse, B.; Biringen, Z. Parents’ adverse childhood experiences and parent–child emotional availability in an American Indian community: Relations with young children’s social–emotional development. Dev. Psychopathol. 2020, 32, 425–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Isosävi, S.; Diab, S.Y.; Qouta, S.; Kangaslampi, S.; Sleed, M.; Kankaanpää, S.; Puura, K.; Punamäki, R.L. Caregiving representations in war conditions: Associations with maternal trauma, mental health, and mother–infant interaction. Infant Ment. Health J. 2020, 41, 246–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McConnell, M.; Closson, L.; Morse, B.; Wurster, H.; Flykt, M.; Sarche, M.; Biringen, Z. The “EA brief”: A single session of parent feedback and coaching to improve emotional attachment and emotional availability (EA). Infant Ment. Health J. 2020, 41, 783–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vliegen, N.; Luyten, P.; Biringen, Z. A Multimethod Perspective on Emotional Availability in the Postpartum Period. Parenting 2009, 9, 228–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frigerio, A.; Porreca, A.; Simonelli, A.; Nazzari, S. Emotional Availability in Samples of Mothers at High Risk for Depression and With Substance Use Disorder. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- MacMillan, K.K.; Lewis, A.J.; Watson, S.J.; Galbally, M. Maternal depression and the emotional availability of mothers at six months postpartum: Findings from the Mercy Pregnancy and Emotional Wellbeing Study (MPEWS) pregnancy cohort. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 266, 678–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hakanen, H.; Flykt, M.; Sinervä, E.; Nolvi, S.; Kataja, E.L.; Pelto, J.; Karlsson, H.; Karlsson, L.; Korja, R. How maternal pre- and postnatal symptoms of depression and anxiety affect early mother-infant interaction? J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 257, 83–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Higgins, J.P.T.; Thomas, J.; Chandler, J.; Cumpston, M.; Li, T.; Page, M.J.; Welch, V.A. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions; Version 6.2; John Wiley & Sons: Chichester, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Kertes, D.A.; Donzella, B.; Talge, N.M.; Garvin, M.C.; Van Ryzin, M.J.; Gunnar, M.R. Inhibited temperament and parent emotional availability differentially predict young children’s cortisol responses to novel social and nonsocial events. Dev. Psychobiol. 2009, 51, 521–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rickmeyer, C.; Lebiger-Vogel, J.; Leuzinger-Bohleber, M. Transition to Kindergarten: Negative Associations between the Emotional Availability in Mother–Child Relationships and Elevated Cortisol Levels in Children with an Immigrant Background. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Senehi, N.; Biringen, Z.; Laudenslager, M.; Watamura, S.; Sarche, M. Emotional Availability as a moderator of stress for young children and parents in 2 diverse Early Head Start samples. Prev. Sci. in press.
- Philbrook, L.E.; Teti, D.M. Bidirectional associations between bedtime parenting and infant sleep: Parenting quality, parenting practices, and their interaction. J. Fam. Psychol. 2016, 30, 431–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ruttle, P.L.; Serbin, L.A.; Stack, D.M.; Schwartzman, A.E.; Shirtcliff, E.A. Adrenocortical attunement in mother–child dyads: Importance of situational and behavioral characteristics. Biol. Psychol. 2011, 88, 104–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tarullo, A.R.; Tuladhar, C.T.; Kao, K.; Drury, E.B.; Meyer, J. Cortisol and socioeconomic status in early childhood: A multidimensional assessment. Dev. Psychopathol. 2020, 32, 1876–1887. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilissen, R.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Van Ijzendoorn, M.H.; Van Der Veer, R. Parent–child relationship, temperament, and physiological reactions to fear-inducing film clips: Further evidence for differential susceptibility. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2008, 99, 182–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Der Pol, L.D.; Groeneveld, M.G.; Van Berkel, S.R.; Endendijk, J.J.; Hallers-Haalboom, E.T.; Mesman, J. Fathers: The interplay between testosterone levels and self-control in relation to parenting quality. Horm. Behav. 2019, 112, 100–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacKinnon, A.L.; Carter, C.S.; Feeley, N.; Gold, I.; Hayton, B.; Santhakumaran, S.; Zelkowitz, P. Theory of mind as a link between oxytocin and maternal behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2018, 92, 87–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roth, G.; Dicke, U. Chapter 11—Origin and evolution of human cognition. In Progress in Brain Research; Hofman, M.A., Ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2019; Volume 250, pp. 285–316. [Google Scholar]
- Naber, F.; van Ijzendoorn, M.H.; Deschamps, P.; van Engeland, H.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J. Intranasal oxytocin increases fathers’ observed responsiveness during play with their children: A double-blind within-subject experiment. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010, 35, 1583–1586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reichl, C.; Kaess, M.; Fuchs, A.; Bertsch, K.; Bödeker, K.; Zietlow, A.-L.; Dittrich, K.; Hartmann, A.M.; Rujescu, D.; Parzer, P.; et al. Childhood adversity and parenting behavior: The role of oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms. J. Neural Transm. 2019, 126, 777–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lecompte, V.; Robins, S.; King, L.; Solomonova, E.; Khan, N.; Moss, E.; Nagy, C.; Feeley, N.; Gold, I.; Hayton, B.; et al. Examining the role of mother-child interactions and DNA methylation of the oxytocin receptor gene in understanding child controlling attachment behaviors. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2021, 23, 37–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lewis, C.R.; Sowards, H.A.; Huentelman, M.J.; Doane, L.D.; Lemery-Chalfant, K. Epigenetic differences in inflammation genes of monozygotic twins are related to parent-child emotional availability and health. Brain, Behav. Immun. Health 2020, 5, 100084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs). Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/aces/index.html (accessed on 5 May 2021).
- Easterbrooks, M.A.; Biringen, Z. The Emotional Availability Scales: Methodological refinements of the construct and clinical implications related to gender and at-risk interactions. Infant Ment. Health J. 2005, 26, 291–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodrigo, M.J.; León, I.; García-Pentón, L.; Hernández-Cabrera, J.A.; Quiñones, I. Neglectful maternal caregiving involves altered brain volume in empathy-related areas. Dev. Psychopathol. 2020, 32, 1534–1543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- León, I.; Rodrigo, M.J.; Quiñones, I.; Hernández-Cabrera, J.A.; García-Pentón, L. Distinctive Frontal and Occipitotemporal Surface Features in Neglectful Parenting. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rodrigo, M.J.; León, I.; Góngora, D.; Hernández-Cabrera, J.A.; Byrne, S.; Bobes, M.A. Inferior fronto-temporo-occipital connectivity: A missing link between maltreated girls and neglectful mothers. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2016, 11, 1658–1665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Olsavsky, A.K.; Stoddard, J.; Erhart, A.; Tribble, R.; Kim, P. Neural processing of infant and adult face emotion and maternal exposure to childhood maltreatment. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2019, 14, 997–1008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olsavsky, A.K.; Stoddard, J.; Erhart, A.; Tribble, R.; Kim, P. Reported maternal childhood maltreatment experiences, amygdala activation and functional connectivity to infant cry. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2021, 16, 418–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neukel, C.; Bertsch, K.; Fuchs, A.; Zietlow, A.L.; Reck, C.; Moehler, E.; Brunner, R.; Bermpohl, F.; Herpertz, S.C. The maternal brain in women with a history of earlylife maltreatment: An imagination-based fMRI study of conflictual versus pleasant interactions with children. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2018, 43, 273–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mielke, E.L.; Neukel, C.; Bertsch, K.; Reck, C.; Möhler, E.; Herpertz, S.C. Maternal sensitivity and the empathic brain: Influences of early life maltreatment. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2016, 77, 59–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Firk, C.; Dahmen, B.; Lehmann, C.; Herpertz-Dahlmann, B.; Konrad, K. Down-regulation of amygdala response to infant crying: A role for distraction in maternal emotion regulation. Emotion 2018, 18, 412–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, P.; Capistrano, C.G.; Erhart, A.; Gray-Schiff, R.; Xu, N. Socioeconomic disadvantage, neural responses to infant emotions, and emotional availability among first-time new mothers. Behav. Brain Res. 2017, 325, 188–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schneider-Hassloff, H.; Zwönitzer, A.; Künster, A.K.; Mayer, C.; Ziegenhain, U.; Kiefer, M. Emotional Availability Modulates Electrophysiological Correlates of Executive Functions in Preschool Children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Licata, M.; Paulus, M.; Kühn-Popp, N.; Meinhardt, J.; Sodian, B. Infant frontal asymmetry predicts child emotional availability. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2015, 39, 492–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Killeen, L.A.; Teti, D.M. Mothers’ frontal EEG asymmetry in response to infant emotion states and mother–infant emotional availability, emotional experience, and internalizing symptoms. Dev. Psychopathol. 2012, 24, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor-Colls, S.; Pasco Fearon, R.M. The Effects of Parental Behavior on Infants’ Neural Processing of Emotion Expressions. Child Dev. 2015, 86, 877–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Chiao, J.Y.; Immordino-Yang, M.H. Modularity and the Cultural Mind: Contributions of Cultural Neuroscience to Cognitive Theory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 8, 56–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Swain, J.E.; Kim, P.; Ho, S.S. Neuroendocrinology of Parental Response to Baby-Cry. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2011, 23, 1036–1041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kim, S. The mind in the making: Developmental and neurobiological origins of mentalizing. Personal. Disord. 2015, 6, 356–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Citation Number | Article | Target Population | Methods | Results | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | Sample Size | Sample Characteristics | EA Measure | Additional Variables | Evidence of Reliability | |||
[47] | Kertes et al. (2009) | Child age: M = 3.97 years, SD = 0.48 | 274 parent-child dyads | Family income: <USD 25,000 to >USD 200,000 (M = in the USD 76–100,000 range) Parent education: range from high school/GED to professional school/doctorate (median education = bachelor’s degree) 83% of children were white, non-Hispanic | Quality of parental interaction: 30-min videotaped parent–child interactions using the emotional availability scales (EAS; Biringen, Robinson, and Emde, 1998). The four parent scales (sensitivity, structuring, nonintrusiveness, and nonhostility) on the EAS were scored; the 4 measures combined into factor score parenting quality | Three salivary cortisol samples; nonsocial and social inhibition | Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) > 0.80 | Nonsocial and social inhibition could be distinguished; that associations with cortisol response were stressor specific; parenting quality buffered cortisol elevations for extremely socially inhibited children, but not nonsocially inhibited children |
[48] | Rickmeyer, Lebiger, & Leusinger-Bhleber (2017) | Mother age: M = 38.77 years | 24 mother-child dyads | Infants with an immigrant background, “hard to reach” mothers who had not been integrated into Germany yet; mothers from Turkey and Eastern European countries, African countries, and others | 30-min free play session, was assessed with the EA scales, one and a half months before the first cortisol assessment | Hair collection for cortisol (HCC) assessments | 4 raters trained and certified, interrater reliability, ICC = 0.942–0.996 | Children’s cortisol levels were higher after kindergarten entry than before; lower EA before kindergarten entry associated with a rise in HCC (negative correlation); children with low mother intrusiveness and higher child responsiveness associated with lower cortisol increases |
[49] | Senehi et al. (in press) | Child age: 6–36 month old | 10 white English speaking mother–child dyads; 29 Spanish speaking mother–child dyads | Participants were recruited from Denver metro area and were enrolled in early head start (EHS) | Quality of the parent–child relationship was assessed via the emotional availability scales | Adverse childhood experiences questionnaire Hair collection for cortisol (HCC) assessments | Interrater reliability, ICC = Not Yet Reported | In relationships with low EA, maternal ACEs (especially with 4 or more ACEs) were significantly associated with increased hair cortisol in their children. The relationship between maternal ACEs and children’s hair cortisol was not significant within the context of relationships with high EA |
[50] | Philbrook and Teti (2016) | Child age: 3–9-month-old infants | 82 mother–child dyads | Majority white, living with infant’s dad, middle class | Maternal EA during infant bedtime: coded at each age point using the EA scales. Videos set up for one night at 3, 6 and 9 months at family’s home. Coded from the moment camera turned up until 5 min of infant sleep. A composite EA score was calculated | Symptom checklist 90-revised (assesses depressive symptomology); sleep practices questionnaire; infant cortisol using saliva samples | Interrater reliability, ICC = 0.70–0.99 | Higher maternal EA at bedtime associated with lower infant cortisol levels; infant’s stress responsivity often affected by mother’s caregiving; greater maternal responsiveness to non-distress infant cues associated with lower infant cortisol levels; more co-sleeping associated with higher cortisol levels |
[51] | Ruttle et al. (2011) | Child age: M = 4.5 years, SD = 1.16 Mothers age: M = 30.84 years, SD = 2.87 | 75 mother–child dyads | Median family income range CND $8430–152,885; M = $43,918); level of mother education ranged from 4 to 18 years (M = 12 years, SD = 2.41) | Mother–child interaction tasks videotaped including the interactive free-play and interference task coded with the EA scales | Salivary cortisol was sampled three times from mother and child across a home visit | Interrater reliability, ICC = 0.84–0.99 | Mother–child dyads demonstrate attunement of HPA axis activity; attunement prominent during times of increased challenge; factors associated with behavioral sensitivity may influence attunement within the dyad |
[52] | Tarullo et al. (2020) | Child age: 12-month-old infants (86 infants) and 3.5-year-old children (87 children) | 173 parent–child dyads (159 mothers, 14 fathers) | Participants were from the greater Boston metropolitan area | 12-min videotaped parent–child interaction that included 5-min free play, 5-min structured play, and 2-min clean-up. Only the sensitivity subscale was coded | Hair cortisol concentration (HCC); diurnal salivary cortisol sampled 3 different nonconsecutive times; SES multidimensional assessment (income-to-needs ratio, confusion, hubbub, and order scale, neighborhood organization and affiliation scale-revised, household food insecurity access) | Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) for sensitivity subscale infant group ICC = 0.97 3.5-year-old group ICC = 0.81 | Parent education was predictive of infant and child HCC. Neighborhood risk predicted infant HCC. Household chaos was related to bedtime salivary cortisol concentration (SCC). Parental sensitivity did not predict or mediate relations between SES and cortisol levels. Higher child HCC was correlated with greater food insecurity for both the infant and 3.5-year-old child group. |
[53] | Gilisen et al. (2008) | Group 1 at 4 years old: child age: M = 3.8 years, SD = 0.3 Mothers age: M = 35.9 years, SD = 3.9 Fathers age: M = 38.3 years, SD = 5.2 Group 2 at 7 years-old: Child age: M = 7.4 years, SD = 0.3 Mothers age: M = 38.8 years, SD = 3.4 Fathers age: M = 41.1 years, SD = 4.7 | Group 1 at 4 years old: 78 parent–child dyads Group 2 at 7 years old: 92 parent–child dyads (the first-born children of twin pairs) | All participants were born in the Netherlands. Mothers of the 4 year olds had completed 15.9 years (SD = 3.9) of education and the fathers had completed 16.3 years (SD = 3.7) | 10-min free play episode of mother and 4 year old coded using the emotional availability scales | Ambulatory monitoring system to measure skin conductance; parasympathetic influence measured with electrocardiogram (ECG) electrodes; children’s behavior questionnaire (CBQ) to measure child temperamental fearfulness | Interrater reliability, ICC = 0.74–0.87 | 4 and 7 year olds both responded with increases in skin conductance (sympathetic activation) and decreases in heart rate variability (parasympathetic withdrawal) to the fear-inducing film clip. More fearful children were more susceptible to the quality of their relationships with their mothers than less fearful children, irrespective of their ages. |
[54] | Van der Pol et al. (2019) | Child age: M = 4 years, SD = 0.1 Mothers age: M = 35.9 years, SD = 3.9 Fathers age: M = 39.3, SD = 4.7 | 159 father–child (second born) dyads | The majority of fathers had finished academic or higher vocational schooling (75%); were married or had a registered partnership or cohabitation agreement with the mother of the target child (92%) | Each dyad received a bag with toys and was invited to free play for 8 minutes; coded with the fourth edition of the emotional availability scales | Testosterone concentrations from fathers’ salivary samples; computerized go/nogo task used to measure self-control | Interrater reliability, ICC > 0.70 | Higher father basal testosterone levels in the evening were related to less respect for child autonomy (only in fathers with low self-control). Higher father basal testosterone levels in the evening was related to more sensitive parenting (only in fathers with high self-control) |
[55] | MacKinnon et al. 2018 | Mothers age: M = 35.56 years, SD = 4.36 | 189 pregnant women | 91.0% of mothers reported being married or living with their partner, 3.7% became single, divorced, or widowed since participation in the original study | Five minutes of free play with toys was filmed at follow-up at 7–9 weeks postpartum and coded using the emotional availability scales | Meins and Fernyhough’s (2015) procedure for interactional measures of mind-mindedness in the first year of life; reading the mind in the eyes test for theory of mind; maternal speech transcribed verbatim then identified mind-related comments | Interrater reliability, for all scales (ICCs = 0.51–0.71), except for nonhostility (ICC = 0.38) | Mothers’ reading the mind in the eyes test performance at 7–9 weeks postpartum was associated with more structuring and less intrusive maternal behavior at 2–3 years postpartum, while their tendency to use mind-related comments at 7–9 weeks postpartum was associated with greater sensitivity 2–3 years postpartum |
[57] | Naber et al. (2010) | Child age: M = 33.8 months, SD = 11.8 Fathers age: M = 37.9 years, SD = 3.80 | 17 father–toddler dyads | Participants were all healthy volunteers with at least one child between 1.5 and 5 years of age | 2 observed home play sessions of 15 min each coded using the emotional availability scales | single dose of 24 IU oxytocin nasal spray or placebo nasal spray | Interrater reliability average, ICC = 0.95 | In the oxytocin condition fathers were more stimulating of their child’s exploration than in the placebo condition, and they tended to show less hostility |
Citation Number | Article | Target Population | Methods | Results | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | Sample Size | Sample Characteristics | EA Measure | Additional Variables | Evidence of Reliability | |||
[58] | Reichl et al. (2019) | Child age: M = 8.02 years, SD = 1.57 Mothers age: M = 39.28 years, SD = 5.71 | 193 mother–child dyads | Mothers excluded if they had neurological diseases, severe physical or mental disabilities, or if met criteria for an emotional-unstable, anxious-avoidant or antisocial personality disorder. Children excluded if they met the criteria for an autistic disorder or in case of an intelligence score below 70. | Maternal sensitivity assessed with the sensitivity scale of the emotional availability (EA) scales (Biringen 2008). During two situations (free play; dealing with a hardly solvable puzzle task), mothers were asked to interact with their child without the attendance of any other person. | Analyzed three polymorphisms (rs53576, rs1042778, rs2254298) of the OXTR gene and plasma oxytocin | Inter-rater reliability, ICC ≥ 0.81 | Of the three analyzed polymorphisms (rs53576, rs1042778, rs2254298) of the OXTR gene and plasma oxytocin, only the rs53576 was associated with mothers’ parenting behavior, specifically with maternal sensitivity; rs2254298 significantly moderated relations between mothers’ experiences of childhood adversity and parenting behavior; significant relations for mothers homozygous for the G allele; G allele of the rs2254298 was related to increased plasma oxytocin levels |
[59] | Lecompte et al. (2021) | Mother age: ≥18 years | 16 mother–child dyads | Gestational weeks: 12–14 weeks gestation and pregnant with a single baby | Five minutes of free play with parent and child: filmed and coded with the emotional availability scales | Separation–reunion procedure for preschool-age children; buccal swab for children’s DNA methylation analyses using the Oragene TMOG-250 collection kit; child methylation data at the follow-up time-point from the the OXTR exon 3 genomic region area | Interrater reliability, ICC = 0.51–0.71, sensitivity (.71), structuring (0.54). | Lower maternal sensitivity associated with more controlling caregiving behaviors; less maternal structuring associated with more controlling punitive behaviors; hypomethylation of the OXTR gene associated with greater maternal structuring behaviors and with more child controlling caregiving behaviors; no interaction effect found of OXTR gene as a moderator in the association between interactive behaviors and child controlling behaviors |
[60] | Lewis et al. (2020) | Child age: M = 8.5 years, SD = 0.45 | N = 96 sub-sample of monozygotic twins | Monozygotic twins: 51% male; 50% Non-Hispanic white, 14.6% Hispanic/Latinx, 8.3% African American, 4.2% Asian American) | EA measured with a 28-item, abridged version of the EA self-report. Items were rated on a five-point Likert scale from “almost never” to “almost always” (asked separately for each twin). Higher scores indicated higher mother-reported EA in the parent–child relationship | General health composite using items from the parent-reported MacArthur health and behavior questionnaire (HBQ) (Essex et al., 2002) Buccal cells were collected with Mawi iSWAB DNA collection tubes (Mawi DNA Technologies LLC, Hayward, CA) | Cronbach’s alpha was 0.721 and 0.806 at 1 and 2.5 years, respectively | Parental EA at 1 year old was related to multiple immune gene methylations in monozygotic twins at 8 years of age. Twin pairs with discordant health, compared to pairs with similar health, had more differences in immune gene methylation |
Citation Number | Article | Target Population | Methods | Results | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | Sample Size | Sample Characteristics | EA Measure | Additional Variables | Evidence of Reliability | |||
[63] | Rodrigo et al. (2020) | Child age: Neglectful, M = 2.8 years, SD =1.5 Control group M = 2.1 years, SD = 1.8 Mother age: neglectful M = 29.2 years, SD = 7.0; control M = 33.43 years, SD = 3.4 | 48 mother–child dyads (25 neglectful and 23 non-neglectful control mothers) | Mothers with history of neglect of a child in the last 12 months, referral recorded by Child Protective Services, and complied with the indicators of the maltreatment classification system for severe neglect | Mother–child free play; EA scales: six subscales of adult and child EA scales | Mini international neuro-psychiatric interview; T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging with the MPRAGE (magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo): gray and white matter volumes | Inter-rater reliability: sensitivity (0.94), structuring (0.90), nonhostility (0.92), nonintrusiveness (0.87), responsiveness (0.92), and involvement (0.86) | Smaller gray matter volume in the right insula, anterior/middle cingulate, and right inferior frontal gyrus and less white matter volume in bilateral frontal regions in the neglectful mothers compared to non-neglectful mothers |
[64] | León et al. (2021) | Child age: neglectful: M = 2.7 years, SD =1.5; control group M = 2.3 years, SD = 1.9 Mother age: neglectful M = 29.1 years, SD = 7.1; control group M = 33.6, SD = 3.2 | 45 mother–child dyads (24 neglectful and 21 non-neglectful control mothers) | Mothers with history of neglect of a child in the last 12 months and complied with the indicators of the maltreatment classification system for severe neglect | Mother–child free play; EA scales: six subscales of adult and child EA scales | Mini international neuropsychiatric interview; T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging with the MPRAGE (magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo): cortical thickness and surface area | Inter-rater reliability: sensitivity (0.94), structuring (0.90), nonhostility (0.92), nonintrusiveness (0.87), responsiveness (0.92), and involvement (0.86) | Neglectful mothers showed less cortical thickness in the right rostral middle frontal gyrus and a greater surface area in the right lingual and lateral occipital cortices compared to non-neglectful mother; less right rostral middle frontal gyrus thickness, which relates to a lower level of emotional awareness among neglectful mothers |
[65] | Rodrigo et al. (2016) | Child age: neglectful group M = 2.5 years Control group M = 2.3 years Mother age: both groups M = 30 years | 44 mother–child dyads (2 neglectful and 22 control) | Mothers with history of neglect of a child less than 5 years of age in the last 12 months, referral recorded by Child Protective Services | Mother–child free play; EA scales: six subscales of adult and child EA scales | Mini mental state examination; mini international neuro-psychiatric review; T1-General Electric 3T scanner | Inter-rater reliability: sensitivity (0.94) structuring (0.90) nonintrusiveness (0.87) nonhostility (0.92) responsiveness (0.92) involvement (0.86) | Neglectful mothers, compared to the control, had disruptions in the structural organization of connectors between the occipital lobe and the temporal and frontal lobes: ILF-R and bilaterally the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculi (IFO-R and IFO-L). Neglectful mothers, compared to controls, showed reduced volumes in ILF-R and IFO-L. Positive mother–child interactions were predicted by increased volume in the ILF-R; neglectful mothers had a higher likelihood of exposure to early adversity, higher vulnerability to psychopathologies, and lower cognitive integrity compared to the control |
[66] | Olsavsky et al. (2019) | Mother age: 18–40 years | 46 mother–infant dyads (28 childhood maltreatment-exposed and 18 non childhood maltreatment-exposed) | Mothers with childhood maltreatment experiences; first-time mothers | 15-min mother–infant free play observation; EA scales: maternal sensitivity, maternal structuring, maternal noninstrusiveness, and maternal nonhostility (focused on maternal sensitivity) | Risky family questionnaire; infant face task; functional MRI: amygdala reactivity | ICC = 0.84 | Mothers reporting more childhood maltreatment experiences had greater bilateral amygdala reactivity to infant faces compared to mothers who did not experience childhood maltreatment (observation not exhibited when the childhood maltreatment-exposed mothers were shown adult faces) |
[67] | Olsavsky et al. (2021) | Mother age: 18–40 years | 61 mothers with childhood maltreatment experiences | Mothers with childhood maltreatment experiences; 18–40 years old; one-time mothers | 15-min mother–infant behavioral observations; EA scales: focused on maternal sensitivity, maternal noninstrusiveness, and maternal nonhostility | Risky family questionnaire; infant cry paradigm; functional MRI: amygdala reactivity psychophysiological interaction analyses | ICC = 0.84 | Mothers reporting more childhood maltreatment experiences had greater bilateral amygdala response to their own infant’s cry compared to other infant’s cry or white noise; mothers with higher amygdala activation may have decreased intrusive behaviors |
[68] | Neukel et al. (2017) | Child age: 7–11 years | 47 mother–child dyads (22 mothers with a history of physical and/or sexual childhood abuse and 25 without) | Mothers with a history of physical and/or sexual childhood abuse | Mother–child free play; EA scales: six subscales of adult and child EA scales | Childhood experience of care and abuse interview; structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I; international personality disorder examination; Hamilton rating scale for depression; functional MRI | Not specified | Mothers with history of physical and/or sexual abuse or neglect showed greater activation in amygdala, insula and hippocampus; showed less functional connectivity between regions of salience and mentalizing network; mothers with history of physical and/or sexual abuse showed higher maternal sensitivity related to greater bilateral insula and amygdala activations to conflictual versus pleasant interactions |
[69] | Mielke et al. (2016) | Mother with early life maltreatment (ELM): M = 38.8 years, SD = 6.7 Mother without ELM: M = 39.1 years, SD = 4.5 | 25 mother–child dyads with ELM and 28 mother-child dyads without ELM | Mothers are absent of any known substance abuse or neurological disease or dementia or severe physical impairments, or any contraindications for MRI measurements | 15-min mother–child free play and 6-min problem-solving task; EA scales: maternal sensitivity | Interpersonal reactivity index; structural magnetic resonance imaging with unbiased voxel-based morphometry | Inter-rate reliability: good Cronbach alpha (0.81–0.88) | Mothers with ELM were less sensitive when interacting with their own child, compared to mothers without ELM; for mothers with ELM, maternal sensitivity was positively associated with the volumes of left superior frontal gyrus extending to the superior medial frontal gyrus and middle frontal gyrus, core regions of the cognitive empathy network; maternal sensitivity was negatively associated with the volume of posterior cingulate cortex |
[70] | Firk et al. (2018) | Mother age: M = 27 years SD = 5.3 | 26 mother–infant dyads | Healthy full-term infants, mothers without any genetic syndrome or severe disease | 12-min mother–infant free play, EA scales: maternal sensitivity, maternal nonhostility, and maternal nonintrusiveness | Functional MRI; infant cry stimuli | Interrater agreement: maternal sensitivity (0.96), maternal nonhostility (0.94), and maternal nonintrusiveness (0.92) | Higher maternal sensitivity and higher maternal nonhostility were associated with lower amygdala activation during mother–infant interaction; self-distraction decreased subjective emotional intensity and bilateral activations in the amygdala |
[71] | Kim et al., (2017) | Mother age: M = 24.41 SD = 5.22 | 39 mother–child dyads | First-time new mothers in metro Denver areas recruited from midwifery clinics, Women Infant and Children (WIC) and Colorado State Prenatal Plus programs 46% of sample lived in poverty or near poverty (as determined by an income-to-needs ratio <1 or <2) | Mother–child 15-min free-play was observed and coded using the EA 4th edition scales | Beck depression inventory; | Interrater reliability, ICC = 0.713 | Socioeconomic disadvantage was associated with neural sensitivity to infant positive and negative emotions. Lower income-to-needs (ITN) ratio was correlated to reduced responses to positive infant faces. There was evidence of elevated amygdala responses related to negative infant faces. Heightened responses to infant faces was associated with mothers’ intrusiveness. |
[72] | Schneider-Hassloff, Zwonitzer, Kunster, Mayer, Ziegenhain and Kiefer [72] (2016) | Mother age: M = 39 years SD = 4.0 Child age: M = 58.7 months, SD = 6.6 | 27 mother–child dyads | Children absent of any known psychiatric or neurological disease or severe developmental delay | 20-min mother–child free play; EA scales: six subscales of adult and child EA scales and EA zones | Head-toes-knees-shoulders task; delay of gratification task; strengths and difficulties questionnaire; colored progressive matrices task | Inter-rater reliability: good (ICC > 0.89) for adult structuring, adult nonintrusiveness, adult nonhostility, child responsiveness, and child involvement; inter-rater reliability: acceptable for adult sensitivity (ICC = 0.67), inter-rater reliability: low for EA CS (ICC = 0.55) | Higher EA nonintrusiveness was associated with the behavioral aspects of executive functioning; maternal structuring and nonintrusiveness were associated with electrophysiological correlates of EF |
[73] | Licata et al. (2015) | Child age: accessed at 7, 14 and 50 months Final sample M = 6.95 months, SD = 0.22 | Sample of 28 children (15 girls) | Mothers diagnosed with postpartum depression and/or anxiety disorders | EA scales, 10-min videotaped interaction, only maternal sensitivity, maternal structuring, child responsiveness and child involvement subscales were used | Vulnerable attachment style questionnaire; structured clinical interview; theory of mind | At 7 months of child’s age: inter-rater reliability maternal sensitivity (0.89), child responsiveness (0.88), and child involvement (0.78) At 50 months of child’s age: maternal sensitivity (0.85), child responsiveness (0.84), and child involvement (0.89) | Low maternal attachment style insecurity and high theory of mind skills predict maternal EA sensitivity |
[74] | Killeen & Teti (2012) | Mother age: M = 30.7 yearsInfant age: M = 6.94 months | 27 mother–infant dyads | Right-handed mothers | 30-min mother–infant free play; EA scales: maternal sensitivity, maternal structuring, maternal nonintrusiveness, maternal nonhostility, child responsiveness, and child involvement | Electroencephalogram; infant emotion videos; SCL-90-R depression and anxiety subscales; maternal self-reported emotional experience | ICC: 0.668–0.738 for maternal sensitivity, maternal structuring, maternal nonintrusiveness, child responsiveness, and child involvement, 0.411 for maternal nonhostility | EA or mother-reported emotional experience in response to infant emotion cues was not related to the greater relative right frontal activity at rest; greater mother–infant EA was associated with a shift toward greater relative right frontal activation in response to infant emotion cues |
[75] | Taylor-Colls & Fearon (2015) | Child age: 7 month old (24 males) | 40 mother-child dyads | Healthy infants’ absence of low birth weight or premature birth | 3-min mother–child free play; EA scales: maternal sensitivity, maternal structuring, maternal noninstrusiveness, and maternal nonhostility | Event-related potentials; infant behavior questionnaire | Inter-rater reliability: reasonable (ICC = 0.71–0.75) | Higher maternal sensitivity was related to infants’ greater amplitudes to positive facial expressions, relative to fearful and neutral expressions. |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Clark, E.L.M.; Jiao, Y.; Sandoval, K.; Biringen, Z. Neurobiological Implications of Parent–Child Emotional Availability: A Review. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1016. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11081016
Clark ELM, Jiao Y, Sandoval K, Biringen Z. Neurobiological Implications of Parent–Child Emotional Availability: A Review. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(8):1016. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11081016
Chicago/Turabian StyleClark, Emma L. M., Yuqin Jiao, Karen Sandoval, and Zeynep Biringen. 2021. "Neurobiological Implications of Parent–Child Emotional Availability: A Review" Brain Sciences 11, no. 8: 1016. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11081016
APA StyleClark, E. L. M., Jiao, Y., Sandoval, K., & Biringen, Z. (2021). Neurobiological Implications of Parent–Child Emotional Availability: A Review. Brain Sciences, 11(8), 1016. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11081016