Crossmodal Semantic Congruence Interacts with Object Contextual Consistency in Complex Visual Scenes to Enhance Short-Term Memory Performance
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Experiment 1
2.1. Methods
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Stimuli and Materials
2.1.3. Procedure
2.1.4. Data Analysis
2.2. Results and Discussion
3. Experiment 2
3.1. Methods
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Stimuli and Materials
3.1.3. Procedure and Data Analysis
3.2. Results and Discussion
4. General Discussion
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Hillyard, S.A.; Störmer, V.S.; Feng, W.; Martinez, A.; McDonald, J.J. Cross-modal orienting of visual attention. Neuropsychologia 2016, 83, 170–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Koelewijn, T.; Bronkhorst, A.; Theeuwes, J. Attention and the multiple stages of multisensory integration: A review of audiovisual studies. Acta Psychol. 2010, 134, 372–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Santangelo, V.; Macaluso, E. Spatial attention and audiovisual processing. In The New Handbook of Multisensory Processes; Stein, B.E., Ed.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2012; pp. 359–370. [Google Scholar]
- Spence, C. Crossmodal spatial attention. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2010, 1191, 182–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spence, C.; Soto-Faraco, S. Crossmodal Attention Applied: Lessons for/from Driving. Cambridge Elements of Attention; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Talsma, D.; Senkowski, D.; Soto-Faraco, S.; Woldorff, M.G. The multifaceted Interplay between attention and multisensory integration. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010, 14, 400–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tang, X.; Wu, J.; Shen, Y. The interactions of multisensory integration with endogenous and exogenous attention. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016, 61, 208–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Driver, J.; Spence, C. Crossmodal attention. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1998, 8, 245–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spence, C.; Driver, J. Crossmodal Space and Crossmodal Attention; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Spence, C. Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2011, 73, 971–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Spence, C.; Deroy, O. How automatic are crossmodal correspondences? Conscious. Cogn. 2013, 22, 245–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mastroberardino, S.; Santangelo, V.; Macaluso, E. Crossmodal semantic congruence can affect visuo-spatial processing and activity of the fronto-parietal attention networks. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chen, Y.-C.; Spence, C. When hearing the bark helps to identify the dog: Semantically-congruent sounds modulate the identification of masked pictures. Cognition 2010, 114, 389–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Y.-C.; Spence, C. Crossmodal semantic priming by naturalistic sounds and spoken words enhances visual sensitivity. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2011, 37, 1554–1568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iordanescu, L.; Guzman-Martinez, E. Characteristic sounds facilitate visual search. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2008, 15, 548–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Iordanescu, L.; Grabowecky, M.; Franconeri, S. Characteristic sounds make you look at target objects more quickly. Atten. Percet. Psychophys. 2010, 72, 1736–1741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Knoeferle, K.; Knoeferle, P.; Velasco, C.; Spence, C. Multisensory brand search: How the meaning of sounds guides consumers’ visual attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2016, 22, 196–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kvasova, D.; Garcia-Vernet, L.; Soto-Faraco, S. Characteristic sounds facilitate object search in real-life scenes. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Seigneuric, A.; Durand, K.; Jiang, T.; Baudouin, J.-Y.; Schaal, B. The nose tells it to the eyes: Crossmodal associations between olfaction and vision. Perception 2010, 39, 1541–1554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Seo, H.S.; Roidl, E.; Müller, F.; Negoias, S. Odors enhance visual attention to congruent objects. Appetite 2010, 54, 544–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heikkilä, J.; Alho, K.; Hyvönen, H.; Tiippana, K. Audiovisual semantic congruency during encoding enhances memory performance. Exp. Psychol. 2015, 62, 123–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heikkilä, J.; Tiippana, K. School-aged children can benefit from audiovisual semantic congruency during memory encoding. Exp. Brain Res. 2016, 234, 1199–1207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Murray, M.M.; Foxe, J.J.; Wylie, G.R. The brain uses single-trial multisensory memories to discriminate without awareness. NeuroImage 2005, 27, 473–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Matusz, P.J.; Wallace, M.T.; Murray, M.M. A multisensory perspective on object memory. Neuropsychologia 2017, 105, 243–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Botta, F.; Santangelo, V.; Raffone, A.; Sanabria, D.; Lupiáñez, J.; Olivetti Belardinelli, M. Multisensory integration affects visuo-spatial working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2011, 37, 1099–1109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brunetti, R.; Indraccolo, A.; Mastroberardino, S.; Spence, C.; Santangelo, V. The impact of crossmodal correspondences on working memory performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2017, 43, 819–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brunetti, R.; Indraccolo, A.; Del Gatto, C.; Spence, C.; Santangelo, V. Are crossmodal correspondences relative or absolute? Sequential effects on speeded classification. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2018, 80, 527–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kwok, S.C.; Fantoni, C.; Tamburini, L.; Wang, L.; Gerbino, W. A biphasic effect of cross-modal priming on visual shape recognition. Acta Psychol. 2018, 183, 43–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Makovac, E.; Kwok, S.C.; Gerbino, W. Attentional cueing by cross-modal congruency produces both facilitation and inhibition on short-term visual recognition. Acta Psychol. 2014, 152, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mastroberardino, S.; Santangelo, V.; Botta, F.; Marucci, F.S.; Olivetti Belardinelli, M. How the bimodal format of presentation affects working memory: An overview. Cogn. Process. 2008, 9, 69–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Itti, L.; Koch, C.; Niebur, E. A model of saliency-based visual attention for rapid scene analysis. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 1998, 20, 1254–1259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Henderson, J.M.; Weeks, P.A.; Hollingworth, A. The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1999, 25, 210–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Kesteren, M.T.; Ruiter, D.J.; Fernández, G.; Henson, R.N. How schema and novelty augment memory formation. Trends Neurosci. 2012, 35, 211–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coco, M.I.; Malcom, G.L.; Keller, F. The interplay of bottom-up and top-down mechanisms in visual guidance during object naming. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2014, 67, 1096–1120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- de Graef, P.; Christiaens, D.; D’Ydewalle, G. Perceptual effects of scene context on object identification. Psychol. Res. 1990, 52, 317–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hollingworth, A.; Henderson, J.M. Does consistent scene context facilitate object perception? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1998, 127, 398–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- LaPointe, M.R.P.; Milliken, B. Semantically incongruent objects attract eye gaze when viewing scenes for change. Vis. Cogn. 2016, 24, 63–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ortiz-Tudela, J.; Martín-Arévalo, E.; Chica, A.B.; Lupiáñez, J. Semantic incongruity attracts attention at a pre-conscious level: Evidence from a TMS study. Cortex 2018, 102, 96–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Preston, T.J.; Guo, F.; Das, K.; Giesbrecht, B.; Eckstein, M.P. Neural representation of contextual guidance in visual search of real-world scenes. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 7846–7855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Spotorno, S.; Tatler, B.W.; Faure, S. Semantic versus perceptual salience in visual scenes: Findings from change detection. Acta Psychol. 2013, 142, 168–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stirk, J.A.; Underwood, G. Low-level visual saliency does not predict change detection in natural scenes. J. Vis. 2007, 7, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Underwood, G.; Foulsham, T. Visual saliency and semantic incongruency influence eye movements when inspecting pictures. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2006, 59, 1931–1949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Furtak, M.; Doradzińska, Ł.; Ptashynska, A.; Mudrik, L.; Nowicka, A.; Bola, M. Automatic attention capture by threatening, but not by semantically incongruent natural scene images. Cereb. Cortex 2020, 30, 4158–4168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mack, A.; Clarke, J.; Erol, M.; Bert, J. Scene incongruity and attention. Conscious. Cogn. 2017, 48, 87–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Santangelo, V.; Di Francesco, S.A.; Mastroberardino, S.; Macaluso, E. Parietal cortex integrates contextual and saliency signals during the encoding of natural scenes in working memory. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015, 36, 5003–5017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Silva, M.M.; Groeger, J.A.; Bradshaw, M.F. Attention-memory interactions in scene perception. Spat. Vis. 2006, 19, 9–19. [Google Scholar]
- Ortiz-Tudela, J.; Milliken, B.; Botta, F.; LaPointe, M.; Lupiañez, J. A cow on the prairie vs. a cow on the street: Long-term consequences of semantic conflict on episodic encoding. Psychol. Res. 2017, 81, 1264–1275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ortiz-Tudela, J.; Milliken, B.; Jiménez, L.; Lupiáñez, J. Attentional influences on memory formation: A tale of a not-so-simple story. Mem. Cognit. 2018, 46, 544–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Santangelo, V.; Macaluso, E. Visual salience improves spatial working memory via enhanced parieto-temporal functional connectivity. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 4110–4117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Santangelo, V.; Bordier, C. Large-scale brain networks underlying successful and unsuccessful encoding, maintenance, and retrieval of everyday scenes in visuospatial working memory. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snodgrass, J.G.; Corwin, J. Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1988, 117, 34–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iordanescu, L.; Grabowecky, M.; Suzuki, S. Object-based auditory facilitation of visual search for pictures and words with frequent and rare targets. Acta Psychol. 2011, 137, 252–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bundesen, C. A theory of visual attention. Psychol. Rev. 1990, 97, 523–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Desimone, R.; Duncan, J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 1995, 18, 193–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nardo, D.; Santangelo, V.; Macaluso, E. Stimulus-driven orienting of visuo-spatial attention in complex dynamic environments. Neuron 2011, 69, 1015–1028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Nardo, D.; Santangelo, V.; Macaluso, E. Spatial orienting in complex audiovisual environments. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2014, 35, 1597–1614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Spence, C.; Parise, C. Prior-entry: A review. Conscious. Cogn. 2010, 19, 364–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Santangelo, V. Forced to remember: When memory is biased by salient information. Behav. Brain Res. 2015, 283, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Henderson, J.M.; Hayes, T.R. Meaning-based guidance of attention in scenes as revealed by meaning maps. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2017, 1, 743–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hwang, A.D.; Wang, H.C.; Pomplun, M. Semantic guidance of eye movements in real-world scenes. Vision Res. 2011, 51, 1192–1205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Loftus, G.R.; Mackworth, N.H. Cognitive determinants of fixation location during picture viewing. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1978, 4, 565–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Võ, M.L.; Henderson, J.M. Object-scene inconsistencies do not capture gaze: Evidence from the flash-preview moving-window paradigm. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2011, 73, 1742–1753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Henderson, J.M. Human gaze control during real-world scene perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 498–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Henderson, J.M.; Hollingworth, A. High-level scene perception. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 1999, 50, 243–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Le-Hoa Võ, M.; Wolfe, J.M. The role of memory for visual search in scenes. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2015, 1339, 72–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Shomstein, S.; Malcolm, G.L.; Nah, J.C. Intrusive effects of task-irrelevant information on visual selective attention: Semantics and size. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2019, 29, 153–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wolfe, J.M.; Võ, M.L.; Evans, K.K.; Greene, M.R. Visual search in scenes involves selective and nonselective pathways. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wu, C.; Wick, F.A.; Pomplun, M. Guidance of visual attention by semantic information in real-world scenes. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Awh, E.; Anllo-Vento, L.; Hillyard, S.A. The role of spatial selective attention in working memory for locations: Evidence from event-related potentials. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2000, 12, 840–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Botta, F.; Santangelo, V.; Raffone, A.; Lupiáñez, J.; Belardinelli, M.O. Exogenous and endogenous spatial attention effects on visuospatial working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2010, 63, 1590–1602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Awh, E.; Vogel, E.K.; Oh, S.H. Interactions between attention and working memory. Neuroscience 2006, 139, 201–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chun, M.M.; Turk-Browne, N.B. Interactions between attention and memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2007, 17, 177–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bundesen, C.; Habekost, T.; Kyllingsbaek, S. A neural theory of visual attention: Bridging cognition and neurophysiology. Psychol. Rev. 2005, 112, 291–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bundesen, C.; Habekost, T.; Kyllingsbaek, S. A neural theory of visual attention and short-term memory (NTVA). Neuropsychologia 2011, 49, 1446–1457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gazzaley, A.; Nobre, A.C. Top-down modulation: Bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 129–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chen, Y.-C.; Spence, C. Assessing the role of the ‘unity assumption’ on multisensory integration: A review. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Chen, Y.-C.; Spence, C. Dissociating the time courses of the cross-modal semantic priming effects elicited by naturalistic sounds and spoken words. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 1138–1146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Treisman, A.; Riley, J.G.A. Is selective attention selective perception or selective response? A further test. J. Exp. Psychol. 1969, 79, 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Potter, M.C. Very short-term conceptual memory. Mem. Cognit. 1993, 21, 156–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Potter, M.C. Understanding sentences and scenes: The role of conceptual short-term memory. In Fleeting Memories: Cognition of Brief Visual Stimuli; Coltheart, V., Ed.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999; pp. 13–46. [Google Scholar]
- Thelen, A.; Talsma, D.; Murray, M.M. Single-trial multisensory memories affect later auditory and visual object discrimination. Cognition 2015, 138, 148–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Intraub, H. Understanding and remembering briefly glimpsed pictures: Implications for visual scanning and memory. In Fleeting Memories: Cognition of Brief Visual Stimuli; Coltheart, V., Ed.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999; pp. 47–70. [Google Scholar]
- Thelen, A.; Murray, M.M. The efficacy of single-trial multisensory memories. Multisens. Res. 2013, 26, 483–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Sternberg, S. High-speed scanning in human memory. Science 1966, 153, 652–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kwok, S.C.; Macaluso, E. Exogenous features versus prior experiences modulate different subregions of the right IPL during episodic memory retrieval. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 11248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Almadori, E.; Mastroberardino, S.; Botta, F.; Brunetti, R.; Lupiáñez, J.; Spence, C.; Santangelo, V. Crossmodal Semantic Congruence Interacts with Object Contextual Consistency in Complex Visual Scenes to Enhance Short-Term Memory Performance. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1206. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091206
Almadori E, Mastroberardino S, Botta F, Brunetti R, Lupiáñez J, Spence C, Santangelo V. Crossmodal Semantic Congruence Interacts with Object Contextual Consistency in Complex Visual Scenes to Enhance Short-Term Memory Performance. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(9):1206. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091206
Chicago/Turabian StyleAlmadori, Erika, Serena Mastroberardino, Fabiano Botta, Riccardo Brunetti, Juan Lupiáñez, Charles Spence, and Valerio Santangelo. 2021. "Crossmodal Semantic Congruence Interacts with Object Contextual Consistency in Complex Visual Scenes to Enhance Short-Term Memory Performance" Brain Sciences 11, no. 9: 1206. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091206
APA StyleAlmadori, E., Mastroberardino, S., Botta, F., Brunetti, R., Lupiáñez, J., Spence, C., & Santangelo, V. (2021). Crossmodal Semantic Congruence Interacts with Object Contextual Consistency in Complex Visual Scenes to Enhance Short-Term Memory Performance. Brain Sciences, 11(9), 1206. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091206