Eye Tracking Research on the Influence of Spatial Frequency and Inversion Effect on Facial Expression Processing in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- 1.
- The spatial frequency paradigm mainly uses different spatial filters to transform facial expression images [27]. The change of spatial frequency would cause the change of expression features in the facial image, which would have an impact on different facial expression processing methods. It is generally believed that after the low spatial frequency (LSF) filter blurs the facial image, the configural information of the face is retained, which is beneficial to the configural processing method. The high spatial frequency (HSF) filter highlights the local features of the face, which is beneficial to the featural processing method. Additionally, the broad spatial frequency (BSF) is the original image itself [28]. Exploring the performance of individuals under different spatial frequency conditions is helpful to analyze their facial expression processing methods.
- 2.
- The inversion paradigm adopts the method of inverting the entire facial image, and then asks the participants to perceive and process [32]. Since the facial image is inverted, it breaks the original layout of the face and has a greater impact on the configural processing method [33]. Participants need to reintegrate featural information from various areas of the face, such as eyes and mouth. Therefore, participants have difficulty in recognizing inverted facial images compared to upright facial images. There is a huge contrast in their reactions, known as the inversion effect [34]. If there is an inversion effect, it can be inferred that this participant mainly adopts a configural processing method.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Design
2.3. Materials
2.4. Equipment
2.5. Procedure
2.6. Data Analysis Indicators
3. Results
3.1. Facial Expression Recognition Rate
3.2. Eye Tracking Data
3.2.1. Fixation Counts on the Target Image
3.2.2. Fixation Counts on the Areas of Interest
3.2.3. Fixation Duration on the Target Image
3.2.4. Fixation Duration on the Areas of Interest
3.3. Eye Tracking Visualization
4. Discussion
4.1. Overall Analysis
4.2. The Influence of Spatial Frequency on Facial Expression Processing
4.3. The Influence of Inversion Effect on Facial Expression Processing
5. Conclusions
- The facial expression processing ability of children with ASD was significantly weaker than that of TD children, that is, the facial expression recognition rate of children with ASD under various experimental conditions (spatial frequency, orientation) was significantly lower than that of TD children.
- The facial expression processing disorders of children with ASD were mainly due to their atypical facial expression processing methods and strategies. TD children paid more visual attention to the eyes area. However, children with ASD preferred the features of the mouth area and lacked visual attention and processing of the eyes area, which might lead to their relatively weaker ability to process and recognize facial expressions than TD children.
- Children with ASD mainly used the featural processing method to process facial expression information. HSF highlighted the local feature information of the face, which was more conducive to the use of the featural processing method for children with ASD, reflected in the increase in visual attention to facial feature areas and the improvement in expression recognition rate.
- TD children had the inversion effect under all three spatial frequency conditions, which was manifested as a significant decrease in expression recognition rate, indicating that TD children mainly used configural processing method. However, children with ASD only had the inversion effect under LSF condition, indicating that children with ASD had the capacity of configural processing under the LSF condition. Therefore, the weak central coherence theory was not applicable under this condition.
- When the face was inverted or facial feature information was weakened, both children with ASD and TD children would adjust their facial expression processing strategies accordingly, to increase the visual attention and information processing of their respective preferred processing areas. The fixation counts and fixation duration of TD children on the eyes area increased significantly, while the fixation duration of children with ASD on the mouth area increased significantly.
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Lu, A.; Perkowski, M. Deep Learning Approach for Screening Autism Spectrum Disorder in Children with Facial Images and Analysis of Ethnoracial Factors in Model Development and Application. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maenner, M.J.; Shaw, K.A.; Bakian, A.V.; Bilder, D.A.; Durkin, M.S.; Esler, A.; Furnier, S.M.; Hallas, L.; Hall-Lande, J.; Hudson, A.; et al. Prevalence and Characteristics of Autism Spectrum Disorder Among Children Aged 8 Years—Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2018. Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Report. Surveill. Summ. 2021, 70, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carmona-Serrano, N.; López-Belmonte, J.; López-Núñez, J.A.; Moreno-Guerrero, A.J. Trends in Autism Research in the Field of Education in Web of Science: A Bibliometric Study. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 1018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lord, C.; Brugha, T.S.; Charman, T.; Cusack, J.; Dumas, G.; Frazier, T.; Jones, E.J.; Jones, R.M.; Pickles, A.; State, M.W.; et al. Autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 2020, 6, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Briot, K.; Pizano, A.; Bouvard, M.; Amestoy, A. New Technologies as Promising Tools for Assessing Facial Emotion Expressions Impairments in ASD: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 634756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hwang, K. Eyebrows, Facial Expression, and “Poem to the Eyebrow (Blason du Sourcil)”. J. Craniofacial Surg. 2021, 32, E784–E787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cong, Y.Q.; Junge, C.; Aktar, E.; Raijmakers, M.; Franklin, A.; Sauter, D. Pre-verbal infants perceive emotional facial expressions categorically. Cogn. Emot. 2019, 33, 391–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rashidan, M.A.; Sidek, S.N.; Yusof, H.M.; Khalid, M.; Dzulkarnain, A.A.A.; Ghazali, A.S.; Zabidi, S.A.M.; Sidique, F.A.A. Technology-Assisted Emotion Recognition for Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Children: A Systematic Literature Review. IEEE Access 2021, 9, 33638–33653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, S.W.; Abbott, L.; Wieckowski, A.T.; Capriola-Hall, N.N.; Aly, S.; Youssef, A. Feasibility of Automated Training for Facial Emotion Expression and Recognition in Autism. Behav. Ther. 2018, 49, 881–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martinez, A.; Du, S.C. A Model of the Perception of Facial Expressions of Emotion by Humans: Research Overview and Perspectives. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 2012, 13, 1589–1608. [Google Scholar]
- Eisenbarth, H.; Alpers, G.W. Happy Mouth and Sad Eyes: Scanning Emotional Facial Expressions. Emotion 2011, 11, 860–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Guarnera, M.; Magnano, P.; Pellerone, M.; Cascio, M.I.; Squatrito, V.; Buccheri, S.L. Facial expressions and the ability to recognize emotions from the eyes or mouth: A comparison among old adults, young adults, and children. J. Genet. Psychol. 2018, 179, 297–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ekman, P.; Friesen, W. Facial Action Coding System: A Technique for the Measurement of Facial Movement; Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Donato, G.; Bartlett, M.S.; Hager, J.C.; Ekman, P.; Sejnowski, T.J. Classifying facial actions. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 1999, 21, 974–989. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Namba, S.; Sato, W.; Osumi, M.; Shimokawa, K. Assessing Automated Facial Action Unit Detection Systems for Analyzing Cross-Domain Facial Expression Databases. Sensors 2021, 21, 4222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muratori, F.; Billeci, L.; Calderoni, S.; Boncoddo, M.; Lattarulo, C.; Costanzo, V.; Turi, M.; Colombi, C.; Narzisi, A. How Attention to Faces and Objects Changes Over Time in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Preliminary Evidence from An Eye Tracking Study. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dalton, K.M.; Nacewicz, B.M.; Johnstone, T.; Schaefer, H.S.; Gernsbacher, M.A.; Goldsmith, H.H.; Alexander, A.L.; Davidson, R. Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nat. Neurosci. 2005, 8, 519–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- He, Y.Y.; Su, Q.; Wang, L.; He, W.X.; Tan, C.; Zhang, H.; Ng, M.L.; Yan, N.; Chen, Y. The Characteristics of Intelligence Profile and Eye Gaze in Facial Emotion Recognition in Mild and Moderate Preschoolers with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wagner, J.B.; Hirsch, S.B.; Vogel-Farley, V.K.; Redcay, E.; Nelson, C.A. Eye-Tracking, Autonomic, and Electrophysiological Correlates of Emotional Face Processing in Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2013, 43, 188–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lahaie, A.; Mottron, L.; Arguin, M.; Berthiaume, C.; Jemel, B.; Saumier, D. Face perception in high-functioning autistic adults: Evidence for superior processing of face parts, not for a configural face-processing deficit. Neuropsychology 2006, 20, 30–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Qiu, T.; Du, X.; Cai, Y.; Zhang, W.; Zhang, L. Influence of changing expression information characteristics on facial visual scanning of autistic children. J. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 37, 756–761. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
- Schwaninger, A.; Lobmaier, J.S.; Wallraven, C.; Collishaw, S. Two Routes to Face Perception: Evidence from Psychophysics and Computational Modeling. Cogn. Sci. 2009, 33, 1413–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cribb, S.J.; Olaithe, M.; Di Lorenzo, R.; Dunlop, P.D.; Maybery, M.T. Embedded Figures Test Performance in the Broader Autism Phenotype: A Meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016, 46, 2924–2939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Happe, F.; Frith, U. The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2006, 36, 5–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Curby, K.M.; Moerel, D. Behind the face of holistic perception: Holistic processing of Gestalt stimuli and faces recruit overlapping perceptual mechanisms. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2019, 81, 2873–2880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guha, T.; Yang, Z.J.; Grossman, R.B.; Narayanan, S.S. A Computational Study of Expressive Facial Dynamics in Children with Autism. IEEE Trans. Affect. Comput. 2018, 9, 14–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deruelle, C.; Rondan, C.; Salle-Collemiche, X.; Bastard-Rosset, D.; Fonseca, D. Attention to low- and high-spatial frequencies in categorizing facial identities, emotions and gender in children with autism. Brain Cogn. 2008, 66, 115–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vanmarcke, S.; Wagemans, J. Individual differences in spatial frequency processing in scene perception: The influence of autism-related traits. Vis. Cogn. 2016, 24, 115–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vlamings, P.H.J.M.; Jonkman, L.M.; van Daalen, E.; van der Gaag, R.J.; Kemner, C. Basic Abnormalities in Visual Processing Affect Face Processing at an Early Age in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2010, 68, 1107–1113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deruelle, C.; Rondan, C.; Gepner, B.; Tardif, C. Spatial frequency and face processing in children with autism and asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2004, 34, 199–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Goffaux, V.; Hault, B.; Michel, C.; Vuong, Q.C.; Rossion, B. The respective role of low and high spatial frequencies in supporting configural and featural processing of faces. Perception 2005, 34, 77–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hedley, D.; Brewer, N.; Young, R. The Effect of Inversion on Face Recognition in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015, 45, 1368–1379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kuraguchi, K.; Kanari, K. Face Inversion Effect on Perceived Cuteness and Pupillary Response. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 558478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, D.; Macrae, C.N. Processing style and person recognition: Exploring the face inversion effect. Vis. Cogn. 2010, 18, 161–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Langdell, T. Recognition of faces: An approach to the study of autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1978, 19, 255–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Falck-Ytter, T. Face Inversion Effects in Autism: A Combined Looking Time and Pupillometric Study. Autism Res. 2008, 1, 297–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cleary, L.; Brady, N.; Fitzgerald, M.; Gallagher, L. Holistic processing of faces as measured by the Thatcher illusion is intact in autism spectrum disorders. Autism 2015, 19, 451–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pallett, P.M.; Cohen, S.J.; Dobkins, K.R. Face and object discrimination in autism, and relationship to IQ and age. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014, 44, 1039–1054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kikuchi, Y.; Senju, A.; Hasegawa, T.; Tojo, Y.; Osanai, H. The effect of spatial frequency and face inversion on facial expression processing in children with autism spectrum disorder. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 2013, 55, 118–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Childers, J.S.; Durham, T.W.; Wilson, S. Relation of performance on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test--Revised among preschool children. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1994, 79, 1195–1199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krasileva, K.E.; Sanders, S.J.; Bal, V.H. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test: Proxy for Verbal IQ in Genetic Studies of Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2017, 47, 1073–1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Publishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; pp. 55–59. ISBN 978-0-89042-554-1. [Google Scholar]
- Yin, L.; Wei, X.; Sun, Y.; Wang, J.; Rosato, M.J. A 3D facial expression database for facial behavior research. In Proceedings of the IEEE 7th International Conference on Automatic Face and Gesture Recognition, Southampton, UK, 10–12 April 2006; pp. 211–216. [Google Scholar]
- Yin, L.; Chen, X.; Sun, Y.; Worm, T.; Reale, M. A high-resolution 3D dynamic facial expression database. In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Automatic Face and Gesture Recognition, Amsterdam, Netherlands, 17–19 September 2008; p. 66. [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka, J.W.; Sung, A. The “Eye Avoidance” Hypothesis of Autism Face Processing. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016, 46, 1538–1552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cuve, H.C.; Gao, Y.; Fuse, A. Is it avoidance or hypoarousal? A systematic review of emotion recognition, eye-tracking, and psychophysiological studies in young adults with autism spectrum conditions. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2018, 55, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Jong, M.C.; van Engeland, H.; Kemner, C. Attentional effects of gaze shifts are influenced by emotion and spatial frequency, but not in autism. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2008, 47, 443–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Keita, L.; Guy, J.; Berthiaume, C.; Mottron, L.; Bertone, A. An early origin for detailed perception in autism spectrum disorder: Biased sensitivity for high-spatial frequency information. Sci. Rep. 2014, 4, 5475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Group | Orientation | Broad Spatial Frequency (BSF) | Low Spatial Frequency (LSF) | High Spatial Frequency (HSF) |
---|---|---|---|---|
ASD group | Upright | 0.48 ± 0.19 | 0.24 ± 0.14 | 0.57 ± 0.16 |
Inverted | 0.45 ± 0.14 | 0.16 ± 0.08 | 0.47 ± 0.16 | |
TD group | Upright | 0.93 ± 0.08 | 0.77 ± 0.12 | 0.82 ± 0.16 |
Inverted | 0.73 ± 0.17 | 0.53 ± 0.14 | 0.66 ± 0.17 | |
p-value of Mann–Whitney U test | Upright | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.002 |
Inverted | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.023 |
Group | Orientation | Broad Spatial Frequency (BSF) | Low Spatial Frequency (LSF) | High Spatial Frequency (HSF) |
---|---|---|---|---|
ASD group | Upright | 6.17 ± 2.58 | 4.17 ± 2.04 | 8.75 ± 1.91 |
Inverted | 7.58 ± 3.03 | 4.17 ± 2.29 | 9.41 ± 2.35 | |
TD group | Upright | 11.73 ± 2.37 | 8.45 ± 2.21 | 11.18 ± 3.16 |
Inverted | 8.82 ± 3.02 | 9.72 ± 2.57 | 10.09 ± 2.54 | |
p-value of t-test | Upright | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.035 |
Inverted | 0.340 | 0.000 | 0.517 |
Group | Orientation | Area of Interest (AOI) | Broad Spatial Frequency (BSF) | Low Spatial Frequency (LSF) | High Spatial Frequency (HSF) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
ASD group | Upright | Eyes | 1.67 ± 1.72 | 1.00 ± 0.85 | 2.17 ± 1.12 |
Mouth | 2.83 ± 1.40 | 1.83 ± 1.34 | 4.08 ± 1.83 | ||
Inverted | Eyes | 1.25 ± 1.76 | 0.83 ± 0.93 | 1.75 ± 1.05 | |
Mouth | 3.33 ± 0.49 | 2.92 ± 1.78 | 5.75 ± 1.42 | ||
TD group | Upright | Eyes | 4.09 ± 0.94 | 4.36 ± 1.86 | 4.18 ± 1.66 |
Mouth | 3.18 ± 1.40 | 1.82 ± 1.66 | 2.00 ± 1.41 | ||
Inverted | Eyes | 5.45 ± 0.93 | 6.82 ± 1.67 | 5.73 ± 1.95 | |
Mouth | 2.36 ± 1.12 | 1.45 ± 0.93 | 1.91 ± 1.22 | ||
p-value of t-test | Upright | Eyes | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.002 |
Mouth | 0.641 | 0.983 | 0.006 | ||
Inverted | Eyes | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | |
Mouth | 0.020 | 0.024 | 0.000 |
Group | Orientation | Broad Spatial Frequency (BSF) | Low Spatial Frequency (LSF) | High Spatial Frequency (HSF) |
---|---|---|---|---|
ASD group | Upright | 1.19 ± 0.46 | 0.80 ± 0.24 | 1.82 ± 0.76 |
Inverted | 1.27 ± 0.48 | 0.71 ± 0.27 | 1.80 ± 0.52 | |
TD group | Upright | 1.92 ± 0.40 | 1.77 ± 0.43 | 1.92 ± 0.42 |
Inverted | 1.57 ± 0.31 | 1.51 ± 0.28 | 1.46 ± 0.27 | |
p-value of t-test | Upright | 0.001 | 0.000 | 0.707 |
Inverted | 0.087 | 0.000 | 0.070 |
Group | Orientation | Area of Interest (AOI) | Broad Spatial Frequency (BSF) | Low Spatial Frequency (LSF) | High Spatial Frequency (HSF) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
ASD group | Upright | Eyes | 0.24 ± 0.18 | 0.16 ± 0.11 | 0.23 ± 0.17 |
Mouth | 0.42 ± 0.22 | 0.23 ± 0.16 | 0.75 ± 0.40 | ||
Inverted | Eyes | 0.28 ± 0.21 | 0.15 ± 0.08 | 0.18 ± 0.16 | |
Mouth | 0.67 ± 0.36 | 0.47 ± 0.25 | 1.33 ± 0.65 | ||
TD group | Upright | Eyes | 0.66 ± 0.32 | 0.75 ± 0.29 | 0.71 ± 0.30 |
Mouth | 0.32 ± 0.29 | 0.38 ± 0.25 | 0.40 ± 0.33 | ||
Inverted | Eyes | 0.97 ± 0.36 | 1.00 ± 0.29 | 0.89 ± 0.38 | |
Mouth | 0.32 ± 0.20 | 0.35 ± 0.20 | 0.40 ± 0.19 | ||
p-value of t-test | Upright | Eyes | 0.001 | 0.000 | 0.000 |
Mouth | 0.367 | 0.119 | 0.033 | ||
Inverted | Eyes | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | |
Mouth | 0.010 | 0.238 | 0.000 |
Area of Interest (AOI) | Orientation | Spatial Frequency | Fixation Counts | Fixation Duration | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pearson Correlation | Sig. (2-Tailed) | Pearson Correlation | Sig. (2-Tailed) | |||
Eyes | Upright | BSF | 0.589 ** | 0.003 | 0.532 ** | 0.009 |
LSF | 0.688 ** | 0.000 | 0.708 ** | 0.000 | ||
HSF | 0.367 | 0.085 | 0.505 * | 0.014 | ||
Inverted | BSF | 0.528 ** | 0.010 | 0.550 ** | 0.007 | |
LSF | 0.793 ** | 0.000 | 0.692 ** | 0.000 | ||
HSF | 0.544 ** | 0.007 | 0.218 | 0.318 | ||
Mouth | Upright | BSF | 0.000 | 1.000 | −0.320 | 0.136 |
LSF | −0.100 | 0.651 | 0.398 | 0.060 | ||
HSF | −0.679 ** | 0.000 | −0.462 * | 0.026 | ||
Inverted | BSF | −0.427 * | 0.042 | −0.485 * | 0.019 | |
LSF | −0.409 | 0.053 | −0.215 | 0.325 | ||
HSF | −0.423 * | 0.044 | −0.595 ** | 0.003 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Zhang, K.; Yuan, Y.; Chen, J.; Wang, G.; Chen, Q.; Luo, M. Eye Tracking Research on the Influence of Spatial Frequency and Inversion Effect on Facial Expression Processing in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 283. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020283
Zhang K, Yuan Y, Chen J, Wang G, Chen Q, Luo M. Eye Tracking Research on the Influence of Spatial Frequency and Inversion Effect on Facial Expression Processing in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(2):283. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020283
Chicago/Turabian StyleZhang, Kun, Yishuang Yuan, Jingying Chen, Guangshuai Wang, Qian Chen, and Meijuan Luo. 2022. "Eye Tracking Research on the Influence of Spatial Frequency and Inversion Effect on Facial Expression Processing in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder" Brain Sciences 12, no. 2: 283. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020283
APA StyleZhang, K., Yuan, Y., Chen, J., Wang, G., Chen, Q., & Luo, M. (2022). Eye Tracking Research on the Influence of Spatial Frequency and Inversion Effect on Facial Expression Processing in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Brain Sciences, 12(2), 283. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020283