Passive Voice Comprehension during Thematic-Role Assignment in Russian-Speaking Children Aged 4–6 Is Reflected in the Sensitivity of ERP to Noun Inflections
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Syntax in Speech Comprehension
1.2. Language-Specific Syntax
NP1 | V | NP2 | |
(noun phrase 1) | (verb) | (noun phrase 2) | |
(a) Active voice (AV) | Subject (noun) | Action (verb) | Object (noun) |
“The grandson (Nom) | hugged | the grandfather (Acc).” | |
/Vnuk | obnjal | Deda./ | |
(b) Passive voice (PV) | Object (noun) | Action (participle) | Subject (noun) |
“The grandson (Nom) | is hugged by | the grandfather (Inst).” | |
/Vnuk | obnjat | Dedom./ |
1.3. Neural Correlates to the Complex Syntax Processing
1.4. The Current Research
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Subjects
2.2. Speech Examination
2.3. Materials
2.3.1. Linguistic Stimuli
2.3.2. Visual Stimuli
2.3.3. Sound Files
2.3.4. Validation
- No more than 10% of children made mistakes in comprehending the situation expressed in the active voice.
- No more than 10% of adults made mistakes in the same situations expressed in the passive voice.
- Only those situations that corresponded to both of the first conditions were chosen.
- Only those situations that corresponded with all three conditions also in the reversed situation: four sentences for the situation “The grandfather hugged the grandson: and four sentences for the “The grandson hugged the grandfather”.
2.4. Procedure
2.5. Data Processing and Analysis
2.5.1. Behavioral Data
2.5.2. EEG Data
3. Results
3.1. Behavioral Results
3.2. ERP Results
3.3. Age Differences
4. Discussion
4.1. Behavioral Results
4.2. ERP Results
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Lempert, H. Extrasyntactic Factors Affecting Passive Sentence Comprehension by Young Children. Child Dev. 1978, 49, 694–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jackendoff, R. Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Kuperberg, G.R. Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: Challenges to syntax. Brain Res. 2007, 1146, 23–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strotseva-Feinschmidt, A.; Schipke, C.S.; Gunter, T.C.; Brauer, J.; Friederici, A.D. Young children’s sentence comprehension: Neural correlates of syntax-semantic competition. Brain Cogn. 2019, 134, 110–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wu, C.Y.; Vissiennon, K.; Friederici, A.D.; Brauer, J. Preschoolers’ brains rely on semantic cues prior to the mastery of syntax during sentence comprehension. Neuroimage 2016, 126, 256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lindner, K. The development of sentence-interpretation strategies in monolingual German-learning children with and without specific language impairment. Linguistics 2003, 41, 213–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dittmar, M.; Abbot-Smith, K.; Lieven, E.; Tomasello, M. German children’s comprehension of word order and case marking in causative sentences. Child Dev. 2008, 79, 1152–1167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schanerwolles, C. Strategies in acquiring grammatical relations in German—Word-order or case marking. Folia Linguist. 1989, 23, 131–156. [Google Scholar]
- Schipke, C.S.; Friederici, A.D.; Oberecker, R. Brain responses to case-marking violations in German preschool children. NeuroReport 2011, 22, 850–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schipke, C.S.; Knoll, L.J.; Friederici, A.D.; Oberecker, R. Preschool children’s interpretation of object-initial sentences: Neural correlates of their behavioral performance. Dev. Sci. 2012, 15, 762–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fox, D.; Grodzinsky, Y. Children’s passive: A view from the by-phrase. Linguist. Inq. 1998, 29, 311–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Emerson, H.F.; Gekoski, W.L. Development of comprehension of sentences with “because” or “if”. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1980, 29, 202–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, H.L.; Chapman, R.S. Children’s judgment and recall of causal connectives: A developmental study of “because”, “so”, and “and”. J. Psycholinguis. Res. 1980, 9, 243–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pyykönen, P.; Niemi, J.; Järvikivi, J. Sentence structure, temporal order and linearity: Slow emergence of adult-like syntactic performance in Finnish. SKY J. Linguist. 2003, 16, 113–138. [Google Scholar]
- de Ruiter, L.E.; Theakston, A.L.; Brandt, S.; Lieven, E.V.M. Iconicity affects children’s comprehension of complex sentences: The role of semantics, clause order, input and individual differences. Cognition 2018, 171, 202–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Skeide, M.A.; Brauer, J.; Friederici, A.D. Syntax gradually segregates from semantics in the developing brain. NeuroImage 2014, 100, 106–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friederici, A.D. The Brain Basis of Language Processing: From Structure to function. Physiol. Rev. 2011, 91, 1357–1393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stankova, E.P.; Guillemard, D.M.; Galperina, E.I. Morpho-Functional Basis of Complex Sentence Processing in Adults and Children. Hum. Physiol. 2020, 46, 332–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garcia, R.; Garrido, G.; Kidd, R.E. Developmental effects in the online use of morphosyntactic cues in sentence processing: Evidence from Tagalog. Cognition 2021, 216, 104859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, Y.T.; Zheng, X.; Meng, X.; Snedeker, J. Children’s assignment of grammatical roles in the online processing of Mandarin passive sentences. J. Mem. Lang. 2013, 69, 589–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Slobin, D.I.; Bever, T.G. Children use canonical sentence schemas: A crosslinguistic study of word order and inflections. Cognition 1982, 12, 229–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Janssen, B.; Meir, N.; Baker, A.; Armon-Lotem, S. On-line Comprehension of Russian Case Cues in Monolingual Russian and Bilingual Russian-Dutch and Russian-Hebrew Children. In BUCLD 39: Proceedings of the 39th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Somerville, MA, USA, 7–9 November 2014; Grillo, E., Jepson, K., Eds.; Cascadilla Press: Somerville, MA, USA, 2015; pp. 266–278. [Google Scholar]
- Timberlake, A.A. Reference Grammar of Russian; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2004; p. 503. [Google Scholar]
- Ambridge, B.; Kidd, E.; Rowland, C.F.; Theakston, A.L. The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. J. Child Lang. 2015, 42, 239–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vasilyeva, M.; Huttenlocher, J.; Waterfall, H. Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels in preschoolers. Dev. Psychol. 2006, 42, 164–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roa-Rojas, P.; Grinstead, J.; Silva-Pereyra, J.; Fernández, T.; Rodríguez-Camacho, M. Syntactic Gender Agreement Processing on Direct-Object Clitics by Spanish-Speaking Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Evidence from ERP. Children 2021, 8, 175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Akhutina, T.V.; Korneev, A.A.; Matveeva, E.Y. Age-related dynamics of comprehension of logico-grammatical constructions in junior schoolchildren and its brain mechanisms. Spec. Educ. 2017, 3, 15–31. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Kline, M.; Demuth, K. Factors facilitating implicit learning: The case of the Sesotho Passive. Lang. Acquis. 2010, 17, 220–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vasilyeva, M.; Waterfall, H. Beyond syntactic priming: Evidence for activation of alternative syntactic structures. J. Child Lang. 2012, 39, 258–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, A.N. The role of modeling and automatic reinforcement in the construction of the passive voice. Anal. Verbal. Behav. 2006, 22, 153–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Allen, S.E.M.; Crago, M.B. Early passive acquisition in Inuktitut. J. Child Lang. 1996, 23, 129–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, E.V. First Language Acquisition; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Höhle, B.; Schmitz, M.; Santelmann, L.M.; Weissenborn, J. The recognition of discontinuous verbal dependencies by German 19-month-olds: Evidence for lexical and structural influences on children’s early processing capacities. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2006, 2, 277–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Akhutina, T.V.; Panikratova, Y.R.; Korneev, A.A.; Matveeva, E.Y.; Vlasova, R.M. Understanding of Active and Passive Constructions in 7- to 10-Year-Old Russian-Speaking Children: Reliance on Inflections or Word Order. Psychol. Rus. State Art 2019, 1, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, M. Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 2000, 74, 209–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vigneau, M.; Beaucousin, V.; Hervé, P.Y.; Duffau, H.; Crivello, F.; Houdé, O.; Mazoyer, B.; Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. Neuroimage 2006, 30, 1414–1432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vigneau, M.; Beaucousin, V.; Hervé, P.Y.; Jobard, G.; Petit, L.; Crivello, F.; Mellet, E.; Zago, L.; Mazoyer, B.; Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. What is right-hemisphere contribution to phonological, lexico-semantic, and sentence processing? Insights from a meta-analysis. Neuroimage 2011, 54, 577–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Walenski, M.; Europa, E.; Caplan, D.; Thompson, C.K. Neural networks for sentence comprehension and production: An ALE-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019, 40, 2275–2304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Progovac, L.; Rakhlin, N.; Angell, W.; Liddane, R.; Tang, L.; Ofen, N. Neural Correlates of Syntax and Proto-Syntax: Evolutionary Dimension. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Skeide, M.A.; Friederici, A.D. The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016, 17, 323–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Humphries, C.; Love, T.; Swinney, D.; Hickok, G. Response of anterior temporal cortex to prosodic and syntactic manipulations during sentence processing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005, 26, 128–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Humphries, C.; Binder, J.R.; Medler, D.A.; Liebenthal, E. Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during auditory sentence comprehension. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 2006, 18, 665–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bonner, M.F.; Price, A.R. Where Is the anterior temporal lobe and what does it do? J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 4213–4215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Love, T.; Haist, F.; Nicol, J.; Swinney, D. A functional neuroimaging investigation of the roles of structural complexity and task-demand during auditory sentence processing. Cortex 2006, 42, 577–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zhou, X.; Jiang, X.; Ye, Z.; Zhang, Y.; Lou, K.; Zhan, W. Semantic integration processes at different levels of syntactic hierarchy during sentence comprehension: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 1551–1562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gattei, C.A.; Tabullo, Á.; París, L.; Wainselboim, A.J. The role of prominence in Spanish sentence comprehension: An ERP study. Brain Lang. 2015, 150, 22–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Szewczyk, J.M.; Schriefers, H. Is animacy special? ERP correlates of semantic violations and animacy violations in sentence processing. Brain Res. 2011, 1368, 208–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yang, Y.; Wu, F.; Zhou, X. Semantic Processing Persists despite Anomalous Syntactic Category: ERP Evidence from Chinese Passive Sentences. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0131936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fromont, L.A.; Steinhauer, K.; Royle, P. Verbing nouns and nouning verbs: Using a balanced design provides ERP evidence against “syntax-first” approaches to sentence processing. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0229169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Oberecker, R.; Friedrich, M.; Friederici, A.D. Neural correlates of syntactic processing in two-year-olds. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 1667–1678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hahne, A.; Eckstein, K.; Friederici, A.D. Brain signatures of syntactic and semantic processes during children’s language development. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 1302–1318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crandall, M. The ELAN Event-Related Potential in Children 5 to 12 Years of Age. All Theses and Dissertations, A thesis of Brigham Young University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science. 2010, Volume 2151, pp. 1–74. Available online: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd (accessed on 22 March 2022).
- Clahsen, H.; Luck, M.; Hahne, A. How children process over-regularizations: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. J. Child Lang. 2007, 34, 601–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Atchley, R.A.; Rice, M.L.; Betz, S.K.; Kwasnya, K.M.; Serenoc, J.A.; Jongmanc, A. A comparison of semantic and syntactic event related potentials generated by children and adult. Brain Lang. 2006, 99, 236–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNealy, K.; Dapretto, M.; Bookheimer, S. Language and the Developing Brain: Insights from Neuroimaging. In Neuroimaging in Developmental Clinical Neuroscience; Judith, M., Ernst, R., Ernst, R., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2009; pp. 91–107. [Google Scholar]
- Mahlstedt, A. The acquisition of case marking information as a cue to argument interpretation in German: An electrophysiological investigation with pre-school children. MPI Ser. Cognit. Neurosci. 2008, 99. [Google Scholar]
- Silva-Pereyra, J.; Rivera-Gaxiola, M.; Kuhl, P.K. An event-related brain potential study of sentence comprehension in preschoolers: Semantic and morphosyntactic processing. Cognit. Brain Res. 2005, 23, 247–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amunts, K.; Schleicher, A.; Ditterich, A.; Zilles, K. Broca’s region: Cytoarchitectonic asymmetry and developmental changes. J. Comp. Neurol. 2003, 465, 72–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vissiennon, K.; Friederici, A.D.; Brauer, J.; Wu, C.-Y. Functional organization of the language network in three- and six-year-old children. Neuropsychologia 2017, 98, 24–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Matzke, M.; Mai, H.; Nager, W.; Rüsseler, J.; Münte, T. The costs of freedom: An ERP-study of non-canonical sentences. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2002, 113, 844–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shtyrov, Y.; Filippova, M.; Blagovechtchenski, E.; Kirsanov, A.; Nikiforova, E.; Shcherbakova, O. Electrophysiological Evidence of Dissociation Between Explicit Encoding and Fast Mapping of Novel Spoken Words. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 571673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kruchinina, O.V.; Stankova, E.P.; Guillemard, D.M.; Galperina, E.I. The Level of Passive Voice Comprehension in the 4–5 Years old Russian Children Reflects in the ERP’s. J. Evol. Biochem. Physiol. 2022, 58, 395–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nishcheva, N.V. A Comprehensive Preschool Educational Program for Children with Severe Speech Disorders (General Underdevelopment of Speech) from Ages 3 To 7, 3rd ed.; Detstvo-Press: St-Petersburg, Russia, 2018; pp. 1–80. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Zajceva, E.C.; Sheptynova, B.K. Test Methodology of Speech Examination of Children Aged 4–7 Years; KARO: St-Petersburg, Russia, 2012; pp. 1–72. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Raven, D.; Raven, D.K.; Kort, D.K.H. Guide to Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Dictionary Scales. Section 3: Standard Progressive Matrices (Including Parallel and Plus Versions); Kogito-Center: Moscow, Russia, 2012; pp. 1–144. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Panasyuk, A.Y. Adapted Version of the Wechsler Method (WISC); Institute of Hygiene for Children and Adolescents: Moscow, Russia, 1973; pp. 1–40. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Russian National Corpus. Available online: https://ruscorpora.ru/new/ (accessed on 14 May 2018).
- Chaumon, M.; Bishop, D.V.; Busch, N.A. A practical guide to the selection of independent components of the electroencephalogram for artifact correction. J. Neurosci. Methods 2015, 250, 47–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Estevan, R.A.C. La evolución de la transformación pasiva en castellano: Génesis de una situación de modificación morfo-sintáctica. Rev. Español. Ling. Apl. 1985, 1, 9–25. [Google Scholar]
- Nicoladis, E.; Sajeev, S. Developing Abstract Representations of Passives: Evidence from Bilingual Children’s Interpretation of Passive Constructions. Front Psychol. 2020, 11, 545360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guillemard, D.; Galperina, E.I.; Panasevich, E.A.; Kats, E.E.; Mekler, A.A.; Nagornova, Z.V.; Shemyakina, N.V. EEG-correlates of complex syntactic constructions processing in Russian-speaking children of 5-6 years old. Clin. Pathophysiol. 2016, 22, 80–93. [Google Scholar]
- Pierce, A.E. The Acquisition of Passives in Spanish and the Question of A-Chain Maturation. Lang. Acquisit. 1992, 2, 55–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Molinaro, N.; Vespignani, F.; Zamparelli, R.; Job, R. Why brother and sister are not just siblings: Repair processes in agreement computation. J. Mem. Lang. 2011, 64, 211–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, M.; Badecker, W.; Osterhout, L. Morphological analysis in sentence processing: An ERP study. Lang. Cognit. Process. 2003, 18, 405–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kutas, M.; Federmeier, K.D. Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2011, 62, 621–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I.; Schlesewsky, M. An alternative perspective on “semantic P600” effects in language comprehension. Brain Res. Rev. 2008, 59, 55–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Canseco-Gonzalez, E. Using the recording of event-related brain potentials in the study of sentence processing. In Foundations of Neuropsychology, Language and the Brain; Grodzinsky, Y., Shapiro, L.P., Swinney, D., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 229–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aurnhammer, C.; Delogu, F.; Schulz, M.; Brouwer, H.; Crocker, M.W. Retrieval (N400) and integration (P600) in expectation-based comprehension. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0257430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brouwer, H.; Fitz, H.; Hoeks, J. Getting real about semantic illusions: Rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension. Brain Res. 2012, 1446, 127–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dröge, A.; Fleischer, J.; Schlesewsky, M.; Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. Neural mechanisms of sentence comprehension based on predictive processes and decision certainty: Electrophysiological evidence from non-canonical linearization’s in a flexible word order language. Brain Res. 2016, 1633, 149–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Molinaro, N.; Barber, H.A.; Caffarra, S.; Carreiras, M. On the left anterior negativity (LAN): The case of morphosyntactic agreement: A Reply to Tanner et al. Cortex 2015, 66, 156–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gupta, N.; Henry, R.G.; Strober, J.; Kang, S.M.; Lim, D.A.; Bucci, M.; Caverzasi, E.; Gaetano, L.; Mandelli, M.L.; Ryan, T.; et al. Neural stem cell engraftment and myelination in the human brain. Sci. Transl. Med. 2012, 4, 155ra137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paus, T. Growth of white matter in the adolescent brain: Myelin or axon? Brain Cogn. 2010, 72, 26–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scholz, J.; Klein, M.C.; Behrens, T.E.; Johansen-Berg, H. Training induces changes in white-matter architecture. Nat. Neurosci. 2009, 12, 1370–1371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
4-year-old | 5-year-old | 6-year-old | |
The percentage of correct identifications | |||
4-year-old | - | PV: n.s. | PV: U = 47, p = 0.001 |
5-year-old | AV: U = 77, p = 0.05 | - | PV: U = 77, p = 0.01 |
6-year-old | AV: U = 29.5, p = 0.000 | AV: U = 98.5, p = 0.01 | - |
Reaction times | |||
4-year-old | - | PV: n.s. | PV: U = 78, p = 0.05 |
5-year-old | AV: n.s. | - | PV: n.s. |
6-year-old | AV: U = 66, p = 0.01 | AV: U = 98.5, p = 0.05 | - |
Age | Behavioral | ERP (NP2) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
% | RT | FC | TP (Left) | TP (Right) | |
4 y.o. | + | − | TW 300–400 ms | TW 400–600 ms | TW 300–400 ms |
5 y.o. | + | + | TW 200–400 ms TW 500–600 ms | TW 200–400 ms | TW 200–400 ms |
6 y.o. | + | + | TW 200–300 ms | TW 200–300 ms | TW 200–300 ms TW 500–700 ms |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kruchinina, O.; Stankova, E.; Guillemard, D.; Galperina, E. Passive Voice Comprehension during Thematic-Role Assignment in Russian-Speaking Children Aged 4–6 Is Reflected in the Sensitivity of ERP to Noun Inflections. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 693. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12060693
Kruchinina O, Stankova E, Guillemard D, Galperina E. Passive Voice Comprehension during Thematic-Role Assignment in Russian-Speaking Children Aged 4–6 Is Reflected in the Sensitivity of ERP to Noun Inflections. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(6):693. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12060693
Chicago/Turabian StyleKruchinina, Olga, Ekaterina Stankova, Diana Guillemard, and Elizaveta Galperina. 2022. "Passive Voice Comprehension during Thematic-Role Assignment in Russian-Speaking Children Aged 4–6 Is Reflected in the Sensitivity of ERP to Noun Inflections" Brain Sciences 12, no. 6: 693. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12060693
APA StyleKruchinina, O., Stankova, E., Guillemard, D., & Galperina, E. (2022). Passive Voice Comprehension during Thematic-Role Assignment in Russian-Speaking Children Aged 4–6 Is Reflected in the Sensitivity of ERP to Noun Inflections. Brain Sciences, 12(6), 693. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12060693