ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Memory in Bilinguals during L1 and L2 Processing
Abstract
:1. Introduction
Aims and Hypotheses
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Design
2.3. Procedure and Materials
2.3.1. Prospective Memory Task
2.3.2. EEG Recording and Pre-Processing
3. Data Analyses and Results
3.1. Data Analysis
3.1.1. Behavioural Analyses
3.1.2. Electrophysiological Data Analysis
3.2. Behavioural Results
3.2.1. Ongoing Activity Performance
- Response Times. The analysis yielded a statistically significant main effect of focality condition, F(1,29) = 10.464; p < 0.05; = 0.265, showing faster response times for the ongoing activity in the focal condition (M = 832.158, SD = 122.252) than in the non-focal condition (M = 878.308, SD = 154.869). However, the main effect of language F(1,29) = 0.247; p = 0.623; = 0.008, and the focality by language interaction F(1,29) = 0.240; p = 0.628; = 0.008 did not reach significance.
- Accuracy. The analysis showed a significant main effect of focality, F(1,29) = 87.435; p < 0.01; = 0.751, indicating higher accuracy rates in the focal condition (M = 0.96, SD = 0.03) than in the non-focal condition (M = 0.93, SD = 0.04). Furthermore, there was a significant main effect of language, F(1,29) = 14.128; p < 0.001; = 0.328, with more accuracy in L1 (M = 0.956; SD = 0.031) than in L2 (M = 0.934; SD = 0.04). However, the language by focality interaction was not significant F(1,29) = 0.867; p = 0.360; = 0.029.
3.2.2. PM Task Performance
- Response Times. The main effects of focality, F(1,28) = 1.951 p = 0.173; = 0.065, language F(1,28) = 0.705; p = 0.408; = 0.025, and the focality by language interaction F(1,28) = 3.236; p = 0.083; = 0.104 did not reach the statistical significance.
- Accuracy. The main effect of focality reached statistical significance F(1,28) = 31.080; p < 0.01; = 0.526, indicating greater accuracy in the focal (M = 0.884, SD = 0.149) than in the non-focal condition (M = 0.763, SD = 0.119). However, the main effect of language F(1,28) = 2.207; p = 0.149; = 0.073 and the focality by language interaction were not significant F(1,28) = 0.833; p = 0.083; = 0.104.
3.3. Electrophysiological Results
3.3.1. Electrophysiological Results: P3b
3.3.2. Electrophysiological Results: N300
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Holmes, K.J.; Wolff, P. Simulation from Schematics: Dorsal Stream Processing and the Perception of Implied Motion. Proc. Annu. Meet. Cogn. Sci. Soc. 2010, 32, 2704–2709. [Google Scholar]
- Thierry, G. Neurolinguistic Relativity: How Language Flexes Human Perception and Cognition. Lang. Learn. 2016, 66, 690–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Davies, I.R.L.; Corbett, G.G. A Cross-Cultural Study of Colour Grouping: Evidence for Weak Linguistic Relativity. Br. J. Psychol. 1997, 88, 493–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- He, H.; Li, J.; Xiao, Q.; Jiang, S.; Yang, Y.; Zhi, S. Language and Color Perception: Evidence From Mongolian and Chinese Speakers. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bobb, S.C.; Von Holzen, K.; Mayor, J.; Mani, N.; Carreiras, M. Co-Activation of the L2 during L1 Auditory Processing: An ERP Cross-Modal Priming Study. Brain Lang. 2020, 203, 104739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iniesta, A.; Paolieri, D.; Serrano, F.; Bajo, M.T. Bilingual Writing Coactivation: Lexical and Sublexical Processing in a Word Dictation Task. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2021, 24, 902–917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blumenfeld, H.K.; Marian, V. Parallel Language Activation and Cognitive Control during Spoken Word Recognition in Bilinguals. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 25, 547–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Athanasopoulos, P. Cognitive Representation of Colour in Bilinguals: The Case of Greek Blues. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2009, 12, 83–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chabal, S.; Marian, V. Speakers of Different Languages Process the Visual World Differently. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2015, 144, 539–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, P.; Chung-Fat-Yim, A.; Marian, V. Cultural Experience Influences Multisensory Emotion Perception in Bilinguals. Languages 2022, 7, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marian, V.; Bartolotti, J.; van den Berg, A.; Hayakawa, S. Costs and Benefits of Native Language Similarity for Non-Native Word Learning. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 651506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, R.E.; Hunt, R.R.; McVay, J.C.; McConnell, M.D. The Cost of Event-Based Prospective Memory: Salient Target Events. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2007, 33, 734–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bisiacchi, P.S.; Schiff, S.; Ciccola, A.; Kliegel, M. The Role of Dual-Task and Task-Switch in Prospective Memory: Behavioural Data and Neural Correlates. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 1362–1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scullin, M.K.; Mullet, H.G.; Einstein, G.O.; McDaniel, M.A. Prospective Memory. In International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDaniel, M.A.; Einstein, G.O. Strategic and Automatic Processes in Prospective Memory Retrieval: A Multiprocess Framework. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 14, S127–S144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Einstein, G.O.; McDaniel, M.A.; Thomas, R.; Mayfield, S.; Shank, H.; Morrisette, N.; Breneiser, J. Multiple Processes in Prospective Memory Retrieval: Factors Determining Monitoring Versus Spontaneous Retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2005, 134, 327–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kaan, E.; Kheder, S.; Kreidler, A.; Tomić, A.; Valdés Kroff, J.R. Processing Code-Switches in the Presence of Others: An ERP Study. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- López-Rojas, C.; Rossi, E.; Marful, A.; Bajo, M.T. Prospective Memory in Bilinguals and Monolinguals: ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Processing in Bilinguals. Brain Lang. 2022, 225, 105059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- West, R. The Temporal Dynamics of Prospective Memory: A Review of the ERP and Prospective Memory Literature. Neuropsychologia 2011, 49, 2233–2245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- López-Rojas, C.; Marful, A.; Pérez, A.I.; Bajo, M.T. Prospective memory in bilinguals: Recalling future intentions in first and second language contexts. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2023, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayakawa, S.; Costa, A.; Foucart, A.; Keysar, B. Using a Foreign Language Changes Our Choices. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016, 20, 791–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morales, J.; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Bajo, M.T. Dual Mechanisms of Cognitive Control in Bilinguals and Monolinguals. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 25, 531–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morales, J.; Yudes, C.; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Bajo, M.T. Bilingualism Modulates Dual Mechanisms of Cognitive Control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia 2015, 66, 157–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pérez, A.; Duñabeitia, J.A. Speech Perception in Bilingual Contexts: Neuropsychological Impact of Mixing Languages at the Inter-Sentential Level. J. Neurolinguistics 2019, 51, 258–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kliegel, M.; Jäger, T.; Phillips, L.H. Adult Age Differences in Event-Based Prospective Memory: A Meta-Analysis on the Role of Focal versus Nonfocal Cues. Psychol. Aging 2008, 23, 203–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polich, J. Updating P300: An Integrative Theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007, 118, 2128–2148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- West, R.; Wymbs, N.; Jakubek, K.; Herndon, R.W. Effects of Intention Load and Background Context on Prospective Remembering: An Event-Related Brain Potential Study. Psychophysiology 2003, 40, 260–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- West, R. The Influence of Strategic Monitoring on the Neural Correlates of Prospective Memory. Mem. Cogn. 2007, 35, 1034–1046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zhang, J.; Xue, T.; Zhang, Z. Salient Cues Facilitate Automatic Processes in Prospective Memory. Neuroreport 2021, 32, 864–868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cona, G.; Bisiacchi, P.S.; Moscovitch, M. The Effects of Focal and Nonfocal Cues on the Neural Correlates of Prospective Memory: Insights From ERPs. Cereb. Cortex 2014, 24, 2630–2646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Meier, B.; Zimmermann, T.D. Loads and Loads and Loads: The Influence of Prospective Load, Retrospective Load, and Ongoing Task Load in Prospective Memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Horiba, Y.; Fukaya, K. Reading and learning from L2 text: Effects of reading goal, topic familiarity, and language proficiency. Read. A Foreign Lang. 2015, 27, 22–46. [Google Scholar]
- Hunter Ball, B.; Bugg, J.M. Context Cue Focality Influences Strategic Prospective Memory Monitoring. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 1405–1415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cejudo, A.B.; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Bajo, M.T. The Cost of Prospective Memory in Children: The Role of Cue Focality. Front. Psychol. 2019, 9, 2738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wang, L.; Altgassen, M.; Liu, W.; Xiong, W.; Akgün, C.; Kliegel, M. Prospective Memory across Adolescence: The Effects of Age and Cue Focality. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 47, 226–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McDaniel, M.A.; Umanath, S.; Einstein, G.O.; Waldum, E.R. Dual Pathways to Prospective Remembering. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 286–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- PANGEA: Power Analysis for General Anova Designs. Available online: https://jakewestfall.shinyapps.io/pangea/ (accessed on 23 January 2023).
- Marian, V.; Blumenfeld, H.K.; Kaushanskaya, M. The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing Language Profiles in Bilinguals and Multilinguals. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2007, 50, 940–967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Balota, D.A.; Yap, M.J.; Hutchison, K.A.; Cortese, M.J.; Kessler, B.; Loftis, B.; Neely, J.H.; Nelson, D.L.; Simpson, G.B.; Treiman, R. The English Lexicon Project. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 445–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Guasch, M.; Boada, R.; Ferré, P.; Sánchez-Casas, R. NIM: A Web-Based Swiss Army Knife to Select Stimuli for Psycholinguistic Studies. Behav. Res. 2013, 45, 765–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delorme, A.; Makeig, S. EEGLAB: An Open Source Toolbox for Analysis of Single-Trial EEG Dynamics Including Independent Component Analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 2004, 134, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Czernochowski, D.; Horn, S.; Bayen, U.J. Does Frequency Matter? ERP and Behavioral Correlates of Monitoring for Rare and Frequent Prospective Memory Targets. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 67–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cejudo, A.B.; López-Rojas, C.; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Bajo, M.T. ERP Correlates of Prospective Memory and Cue Focality in Children. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oostenveld, R.; Fries, P.; Maris, E.; Schoffelen, J.-M. FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2010, 2011, 156869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melby-Lervåg, M.; Lervåg, A. Reading Comprehension and its Underlying Components in Second-language learners: A meta-analysis of studies comparing first-and second-language learners. Psychol. Bull. 2014, 140, 409–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Antoniou, M. The Advantages of Bilingualism Debate. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2019, 5, 395–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, Y.J.; Thierry, G. Fast Modulation of Executive Function by Language Context in Bilinguals. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 13533–13537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- West, R.; McNerney, M.W.; Travers, S. Gone but Not Forgotten: The Effects of Cancelled Intentions on the Neural Correlates of Prospective Memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2007, 64, 215–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Y.; Cao, X.; Cui, J.; Shum, D.H.K.; Chan, R.C.K. The Relation between Prospective Memory and Working Memory: Evidence from Event-Related Potential Data. PsyCh. J. 2013, 2, 113–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wilson, J.; Cutmore, T.R.H.; Wang, Y.; Chan, R.C.K.; Shum, D.H.K. Effects of Cue Frequency and Repetition on Prospective Memory: An ERP Investigation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013, 90, 250–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Patel, S.H.; Azzam, P.N. Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected Studies of the Event-Related Potential. Int. J. Med. Sci. 2005, 2, 147–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Morrison, C.; Kamal, F.; Taler, V. The Influence of Bilingualism on Working Memory Event-Related Potentials. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2019, 22, 191–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hockey, A.; Cutmore, T. Inhibitory Control in Prospective Memory: An Event Related Potential Comparison of Task-Switch and Dual Task Processing. Neuropsychologia 2021, 158, 107906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Folstein, J.R.; Van Petten, C. Influence of Cognitive Control and Mismatch on the N2 Component of the ERP: A Review. Psychophysiology 2008, 45, 152–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cousens, R.; Cutmore, T.; Wang, Y.; Wilson, J.; Chan, R.C.K.; Shum, D.H.K. Effects of Perceptual and Semantic Cues on ERP Modulations Associated with Prospective Memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2015, 98, 151–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cruz, G.; Miyakoshi, M.; Makeig, S.; Kilborn, K.; Evans, J. ERPs and Their Brain Sources in Perceptual and Conceptual Prospective Memory Tasks: Commonalities and Differences between the Two Tasks. Neuropsychologia 2016, 91, 173–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
L1 | L2 | |
---|---|---|
Proportion of current exposure to the language | 0.64 * (0.16) | 0.30 * (0.14) |
Proportion of preference to read in each language | 0.51 * (0.23) | 0.43 * (0.21) |
Proportion of preference to speak in each language | 0.54 * (0.29) | 0.35 * (0.22) |
Mean age of beginning acquisition (years) | 4.71 (2.66) | |
Mean age of becoming fluent (years) | 12.87 (4.94) | |
Mean level of self-competence (from 0–10) | 8.5 (1.26) | |
Mean level of language exposure with family or friends (from 0–10) | 3.87 (3.25) | |
Mean level of reading exposure (from 0–10) | 5.93 (3.69) | |
Mean level of language exposure by TV or radio (from 0–10) | 8.58 (2.19) | |
Mean level of language exposure by self-learning (from 0–10) | 2.54 (3.29) |
L1 | L2 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Type of Trial | ACC | RT | ACC | RT |
ON trials focal | 0.97 (0.03) | 836 (142) | 0.95 (0.03) | 828 (102) |
ON trials non focal | 0.94 (0.03) | 889 (184) | 0.92 (0.04) | 868 (126) |
PM trials focal | 0.85 (0.19) | 1059 (256) | 0.92 (0.1) | 1002 (199) |
PM trials non focal | 0.75 (0.14) | 1069 (206) | 0.78 (0.1) | 1071 (189) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
López-Rojas, C.; Csilinkó, A.; Bajo, M.T.; Marful, A. ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Memory in Bilinguals during L1 and L2 Processing. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 365. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020365
López-Rojas C, Csilinkó A, Bajo MT, Marful A. ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Memory in Bilinguals during L1 and L2 Processing. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(2):365. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020365
Chicago/Turabian StyleLópez-Rojas, Cristina, Anikó Csilinkó, Mª Teresa Bajo, and Alejandra Marful. 2023. "ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Memory in Bilinguals during L1 and L2 Processing" Brain Sciences 13, no. 2: 365. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020365
APA StyleLópez-Rojas, C., Csilinkó, A., Bajo, M. T., & Marful, A. (2023). ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Memory in Bilinguals during L1 and L2 Processing. Brain Sciences, 13(2), 365. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020365