Next Article in Journal
An Analysis of the Correlation between the Asymmetry of Different EEG-Sensor Locations in Diverse Frequency Bands and Short-Term Subjective Well-Being Changes
Next Article in Special Issue
The Role of Consciousness in Coupling Emotions, Motivations, and Behaviors
Previous Article in Journal
Dopamine Concentration Changes Associated with the Retrodialysis of Methylone and 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) into the Caudate Putamen
Previous Article in Special Issue
Modulating Consciousness through Awareness Training Program and Its Impacts on Psychological Stress and Age-Related Gamma Waves
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Perspective

Do Individual Differences in Perception Affect Awareness of Climate Change?

1
Department of Surgical, Medical, Molecular and Critical Area Pathology, University of Pisa, 56126 Pisa, Italy
2
Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, 5015 Bergen, Norway
3
Clinical Psychology Branch, Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria Pisana, 56126 Pisa, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(3), 266; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030266
Submission received: 30 November 2023 / Revised: 4 March 2024 / Accepted: 6 March 2024 / Published: 9 March 2024

Abstract

:
One significant obstacle to gaining a widespread awareness of the ongoing climate change is the nature of its manifestations in relation to our perception: climate change effects are gradual, distributed, and sometimes seemingly contradictory. These features result in a lag in collective climate action and sometimes foster climate skepticism and climate denial. While the literature on climate change perception and belief has thoroughly explored its sociocultural and sociopolitical aspects, research on the potential contribution of psychophysiological factors remains scarce. In this perspective paper, we outline evidence and arguments for the involvement of psychophysiological systems such as thermoception, hygroreception, and interoception in modulating climate change awareness. We discuss psychophysiological mechanisms of climate change awareness in animals and humans, as well as possible sources of individual variance in climate change perception. We conclude by suggesting novel research questions which would be worthwhile to pursue in future studies.

1. Introduction

The United Nations Framework Convention on Climate Change (UNFCCC), in its Article 1, defines climate change as “[…] a change of climate which is attributed directly or indirectly to human activity that alters the composition of the global atmosphere and which is in addition to natural climate variability observed over comparable time periods.” [1]. Climate change is mostly a gradual phenomenon, geographically distributed, and sometimes seemingly contradictory in its manifestations [2]. These perceptual features of climate change and its effects are an obstacle to fostering awareness, understanding, and action toward climate change in the general population. Conscious awareness of climate change effects is critical to promoting pro-environmental behavior. The perceptual nature of climate change induces individuals to approach it in abstract terms or through ideological lenses [3]; for this reason, the sociocultural factors implicated in climate change perception have been thoroughly explored, while the scientific literature on the strictly psychophysiological aspects of the climate change processes remains scant. Raising temperatures and their consequences pose a threat to the neurobiological systems of both humans and animals [4,5], and novel psychopathological disorders are arising as a consequence of climate change, such as climate anxiety [6]. These health effects of climate change could benefit from a clearer picture of the involvement of psychophysiological systems.
In this perspective paper, we have gathered evidence and arguments for the possibility of a psychophysiological source of variance in climate change perception, with the intent of contributing to the body of literature on the determinants of belief in climate change [7,8,9]. While it is important to remark that gaining awareness of a phenomenon like climate change requires multiple stages of elaboration and the involvement of a diverse array of cognitive processes, including semantic recognition of a particular perception as “climate change”, the scope of this perspective paper is strictly focused on the early physiological stages of this process of gaining awareness. We discuss evidence for climate perception in animals, referencing the field of sensory ecology, and humans, by exploring the different sensory modalities through which humans interface with the climate: thermoception, hygroreception (i.e., perception of wetness/dryness), and interoception. We then discuss possible individual differences in sensory and perceptual processing that might influence a sizable share of climate change awareness, and we propose a series of programmatic research questions to stimulate further developments. While it is not a systematic review, this paper represents the initial attempt to exclusively review psychophysiological data concerning human perceptions of climate change.

2. Perception of Climate Change in Non-Human Animals

The survival fitness of organisms is highly dependent on available resources and environmental conditions. Organisms evolved their mechanisms to anticipate and respond to environmental changes, such as those occurring due to the alternation of seasons. Animals employ adaptive behaviors according to sensory cues gathered and processed from their environment; these sensory cues are the dominion of sensory ecology, the study of how animals use perceptual information in their lives and how sensory systems influence their evolution [10]. Animals react to environmental changes threatening their survival by deploying adaptive behaviors such as migration, hibernation, and encystment. We can infer the detection of climate change in animals by examining the modifications of these behavioral responses in association with phenomena associated with climate change [11], such as changing median temperatures, changes in humidity, and frequency of extreme weather events (for a comprehensive review on the impacts of climate change, we direct the reader to reports by the International Panel on Climate Change [12]).
In the specific case of migration, for example, while this behavior is highly dependent on non-climate related phenomena, such as variations in photoperiod due to the alternations of seasons [13,14], it is still subjected to plasticity due to weather conditions [15,16]. Changes in temperatures and humidity seem to be the strongest climate-related triggers of migration in many taxonomic groups (e.g., [17,18]) Moreover, non-anthropogenic climate phenomena such as the North Atlantic Oscillation (NAO) and the El Niño-Southern Oscillation Index (ENSO), have been linked to migratory behaviors (for a review, see [19]). Indeed, migration onset, which is in part driven by spring temperatures in migratory birds, is altered as a consequence of climate change [20]; an increase in median temperature has resulted in behavioral change, with earlier departures [21].
In this context, the effects of climate change can be conceived as a form of sensory pollution, that is, how anthropogenic stimuli interact with the senses of other organisms [22,23]. While less studied than sensory pollution due to artificial lights, noise, or synthetic chemicals [23], the sensory pollution introduced by climate change affecting thermoception, hygroreception, or indirect effects of global warming might introduce strong, unexpected effects on animal behavior. Sensory pollution due to climate change may also manifest in temperature-driven second-order effects on other sensory modalities, such as olfaction [24].
Animal adaptation to their environment can take the form of highly specialized sensory and physiological systems that humans may not possess (e.g., arthropods such as insects and arachnids possess multiple types of “hygroreceptors”—specific receptor cells for the detection of variation in humidity in the environment—which are absent in other taxonomic groups [25]). In the case of thermoception, many snake species have the ability to detect temperature differences in their environment using infrared radiation. These animals possess a “pit organ”, consisting of a cavity with a thin membrane covered in nerve endings, which contain “Transient Receptor Potential Ankyrin 1” (TRPA1) temperature-sensitive ion channels [26]. Transient Receptor Potential (i.e., TRP) channels are a common feature of thermoreceptive cells in vertebrates, including humans, as we will discuss later [27].
Besides sensory pollution, climate change can influence adaptation behavior in animals through non-sensory physiological pathways [28]. Temperature changes impact the physiology of ectotherms, such as invertebrates and fishes. This susceptibility might alter their migratory behavior not through strictly sensory processes but through their physiology [20]. One such example is the effect of temperatures on migratory behaviors in North American salmon due to metabolic alteration [29,30]. In this case, reactions to climate change are driven by strictly physiological regulatory processes.

3. Sensory Perception of Climate Change in Humans

So far in our discussion, we have established that animals possess multiple systems to detect changes in climate and react to them and that these systems are rooted in their physiology and underpin complex behaviors; but what about humans? The current literature on climate change perception in humans emphasizes the importance of top-down processes (from higher cognition to lower levels of processing). Meaning the influence of beliefs, attitudes, norms, and ideologies in shaping climate change perception [8,9]. Within this context, a neuropsychological causal model for top-down modulation of perception has recently been proposed: the “Motivated Attention” model of climate change perception and action [31] describes how our personal stance on climate change diverts our attention to fixate on elements that align with our preconceptions. This dynamic ends up generating a positive feedback loop: noticing things that are in line with our ideas further increases the strength of our convictions; The motivated attention framework is convincing, and indeed it can explain many of the perceptual phenomena surrounding climate change, such as the fact that people with higher pre-existing knowledge of the phenomenon of climate change are more likely to notice its effects in everyday life.
Like other animal species, humans rely on sensory cues from their environment to adapt to environmental conditions and possess physiologically determined regulatory systems attuned to environmental cues and natural rhythms, such as circadian and ultradian cycles following variations in the daylight period [14,32]. Across the globe, human societies adopted sedentary or nomadic lifestyles. Both of these required attunements to environmental conditions to increase survival. On this note, there is an accumulating body of evidence suggesting the idea that the human brain is competent in detecting evolutionarily stimuli that resulted from evolutionary pressure, such as some specific phobias for natural threats, even on a pre-conscious level [33,34]. For example, visual stimuli possessing snake-like perceptual features modulate specific attention-related brain activities, even in the absence of subjectively perceived phobias toward the animals [35]. Moreover, sensory stimuli associated with safe, resource-rich natural environments, such as hearing birdsong, or smelling vegetation or rain, frequently evoke positive affective and physiological responses [36,37,38].
The most conspicuous effects of climate change are principally constituted by alterations in weather patterns in the form of altered precipitations or extreme weather events, such as heat waves or wet-bulb events [12]. The perception of these effects is underpinned by human sensory systems involved in sensing temperature (thermoception), and sensing humidity/dryness (hygroreception; see Figure 1).

4. Thermoception

Global warming, the principal driver of climate change, has led to an increase in average global temperatures, as well as an increase in the frequency of deadly heat waves, which constitute a critical health hazard [12]. Temperature regulates all biochemical reactions via thermodynamics; for this reason, humans and all other living beings possess thermoreceptive and thermoregulatory systems to maintain thermal homeostasis [39]. Human thermoception relies on thermoreceptor cells that convey alterations in environmental temperature via temperature-sensitive cation channels of the TRP (Transient Receptor Potential) family [27,40]. In particular, two channel proteins, “Transient Receptor Potential Melastin 8” (TRPM8) and “Transient Receptor Potential Vanilloid 1” (TRPV1), appear to be involved in cold and heat sensitivity, respectively. Menthol is an agonist for TRPM8, while Capsaicin is an agonist for TRPV1. These two compounds are appreciated as flavoring agents worldwide and are known for their capacity to evoke sensations of cold or heat.
TRPM8 and TRPV1 are mostly expressed in distinct “cold” and “hot” afferent neurons, whose cell body is localized in the Dorsal Root Ganglia (for thermoception on the body surface), and the Trigeminal Ganglia (for thermoception of face and head). Through the spinal cord, these neuronal populations project to the preoptic area of the hypothalamus, the somatosensory thalamic nuclei, and the parabrachial nuclei in the brainstem. These are thought to undercut the thermoregulatory, somatotopic, and affective/hedonic qualities of thermoception, respectively [41]. From these centers, thermal information is then relayed to the primary and secondary somatosensory cortices, and the anterior insular cortex [39].
Human thermoception appears to be able to detect both absolute temperature and temperature differences. However, while both “cold” and “warm” cells encode for absolute skin sensation, “cold” cells are often activated by dynamic cooling processes [38,40].
Since temperature alterations due to climate change are appreciable only on a scale of months or years, the perception of these temperature differences requires the reliance on memory for somatic states. However, this recollection is principally operated in reference to proximal information, such as the past few years, rather than historical temperature values [42].
In general, humans use readily available memory to assess temperature information on climate change, rather than more analytical forms of cognition [43]. Even current daily temperatures influence belief in temperature change due to global warming and willingness to act towards it [44], an effect known as the “local warming” effect. In addition, even the instantaneous visceral thermoreceptive state of an individual seems to influence the saliency of global warming as a problem, as evidenced by a study by Lewandowski and colleagues [45] in which they manipulated the visceral state of the participants by administering menthol flavored chewing gums, thus stimulating TRPM8 receptors.
The preponderance of readily available sensory information, as opposed to analytical, long-term memory recall and reasoning, increases the difficulty of associating one’s actions (e.g., CO2-releasing behaviors) and long-term global warming effects.
Indeed, the rules of contingency on which the most basic forms of learning are based clash with the extremely gradual nature of most climate change phenomena [46].

5. Hygroreception

Together with heat waves, climate change has led to an increase in “wet-bulb events” [12,47]. These are extreme weather phenomena in which the combination of temperature and relative humidity in the air impedes human thermoregulation, constituting a critical hazard for human health and survival, especially in urbanized environments in temperate and tropical regions [12].
There is no evidence for the existence of specialized human hygroreception systems [48]. Humans likely rely on multisensory integration to detect differences in wetness or humidity; tactile sensations are the primary drivers of wetness perception, with a considerable contribution from thermoception [48]. Wetness is conveyed to the spinal column via afferent myelinated Aβ fibers coding for mechanosensation and Aδ coding for cold thermosensation [48]. Molecular thermoreceptors for cold perception (e.g., TRPM8) have been implicated in the thermoreceptive aspect of wetness sensation [49].
Together with this initial multisensory integration, other sensory systems contribute to the feeling of wetness: specific patterns of sound can be interpreted by the brain as signifying dryness or wetness, as evidenced by the “parchment–skin illusion”. The illusion can be evoked by asking participants to hear a pitch-shifted recording of their hands rubbing one against the other. When the pitch was increased to higher frequencies, participants reported that the sound of their hands felt significantly drier, “like parchment paper” [50]. Vision also contributes to the perception of wetness: objects can be perceived as either wet or dry entirely based on their visual properties, such as variations in color and brightness [51]. Moreover, visual stimuli, in combination with electrotactile stimulation, are capable of producing convincing wetness effects in virtual environments [52].
Another intriguing possibility for wetness perception in humans might come through olfaction; a common experience after rainfall is the perception of “petrichor”, a musty, earthy smell, often associated with pleasant affective states and relaxation [53,54]. The smell of petrichor is induced by the inhalation of geosmin, a metabolite of some microorganisms, especially cyanobacteria. When these organisms die in dry periods, large quantities of geosmin are deposited in the soil [55]. When the soil is disturbed, such as during precipitations, geosmin is liberated into the air in aerosol form. Humans are extremely sensitive to concentrations of geosmin in the air, with estimations going as low as 9.5 parts per trillion [56]. Evidence also shows that smelling this compound is capable of inducing psychophysiological states of relaxation, even in the absence of other wetness sensory cues [36].

6. Interoception

Besides external temperature and wetness, mounting evidence suggests that interoception appears to play a significant role in shaping climate change perception. “Interoception” is a broad term encompassing sensory systems that relay information from the internal milieu of the body, unlike others that gather information from the external environment. Interoception includes visceral sensations like hunger and pain and plays a vital role in maintaining homeostasis [57]. Internal sensory information reaches the central nervous system (CNS) through two major afferent pathways: a vagal pathway, and a spinal pathway. These connections reach the brainstem nuclei, like the parabrachial nucleus (PBN) and the solitary tract nuclei (NTS). These in turn make contact with cortical areas, which include the somatic sensory cortex, the insula, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the prefrontal cortex [58].
Risen and Critcher demonstrated in a series of six experimental studies that manipulating visceral states increases temperature and dryness assessment, as well as belief in global warming [59]. The authors argue that current visceral states create a bias in assessment which favors judgments that are congruent with the current interoceptive state, in what they call the “visceral fit” effect. In their paper, individuals subjected to higher temperatures (both indoors and outdoors) were more likely to endorse climate change. Interestingly, this effect was independent of the political orientation of the individuals and influenced right-wing and left-wing individuals in the same manner. Similarly, participants to whom a state of thirst was previously induced rated the risk of drought and water scarcity as higher than those who were not thirsty [59]. In addition to the aforementioned study by Lewandowski and colleagues [45], it appears that current interoceptive states do indeed influence the perception of climate change.
It is important to note that, besides sociocultural variables like those mentioned in the previous sections, psychophysiological perceptual systems are influenced by numerous, non-sensory factors, such as mood, emotions [60], and physiological processes (e.g., thirst, see the “Interoception” section later in the text). Emotions and mood states can affect low-level perceptual processing in multiple ways [60], including affective states presenting as sensory phenomena or visceral sensations (e.g., shortness of breath due to anxiety). We could speculate that affective states can modulate climate perception through the aforementioned “visceral fit”. Indeed, mounting evidence shows that affective states, particularly negative ones, play an important role in shaping climate perception and action (for a review, see [61]).
The interoceptive sensation of pain also plays a role in climate perception; pain is integrated mainly with thermoception [62]. This is evidenced by the classic cold pressor test, used to evoke a pain response by hand immersion in containers filled with water at different temperatures [63], with many TRP receptors also serving as pain receptors [64]. An association between chronic pain exacerbation and weather changes has been established for millennia, and it has been experimentally verified [65]. However, a clear mechanistic explanation for this phenomenon remains to be established [66]. In addition, there seems to be a neurological cross-talk between the sensation of pain and thirst which might constitute a further avenue through which a climate-related stimulus can exert modulation through interoception [67,68].
Considering these lines of evidence, as well as the other instances of interaction and superposition of sensory systems that we mentioned so far in our discussion, it appears plausible that the perception of climate change effects arises as a result of multisensory integration, rather than single-sensory channels. While these sensory systems are shared by every human, a significant variance in the subjective perception of climate change still exists. Besides variance due to sociocultural factors, in the next section, we illustrate some likely sources of variation in the climate change perception of a psychophysiological nature.

7. Psychophysiological Individual Differences and Climate Change Perception

There are many possible sources of psychophysiological variations in climate change perception. These can intervene at every step of the perceptual chain, from genetic polymorphisms to individual differences in multisensory integration. Humans exhibit a wide range of individual sensitivity for both temperature and pain [69,70]. Genetic variants of genes coding for TRP channels correlate with individual differences in sensitivity to heat [71], and several TRP-coding gene polymorphisms have been associated with both heat sensitivity and chronic pain conditions (Table 1). Variants of the capsaicin-sensitive TRPV1, like the single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) rs8065080, are associated with variations in sensitivity to hot and cold pain [72]. Individuals homozygous for this allele also show remarkable sensitivity to heat which is not reduced by the application of capsaicin [73]. Meanwhile, rs57716901 and rs61387317 SNP variants are associated with burning pain sensitivity [74]. Individuals carrying another polymorphism for the TRPV1 gene display altered tolerance to thermal pain, which is also moderated by personality traits [75]. Genetic variants of the TRPM8 cold receptor gene are particularly notable: these polymorphisms not only are associated with sensitivity to cold and painful cold [76,77], but their prevalence in populations is associated with local climate and average winter temperatures [78,79]. In particular, the prevalence of the rs17862920 variant appears to follow a latitude gradient, with lower concentrations around the equator (5% in Nigerian samples), and higher towards the pole (88% in Finnish samples) [78]. We might speculate that these variants constituted an advantageous sensory adaptation to specific climates that increased population fitness.
Besides structural genetic variants, epigenetic alterations in promoters of TRP channels, such as methylation, have also been associated with pain sensitivity [81]. Findings such as these might translate into the possibility that a degree of genetic plasticity exists in climate change perception; increasing adaptive pressure towards climate change effects might push for adaptations that increase individual sensitivity to alterations in climate. Indeed, epigenetic adaptive and maladaptive alterations are known to arise as a consequence of heat exposure and heat stress [82]. It might be possible that populations that have been subjected to extreme weather events related to climate change, such as heat waves or flooding, may carry epigenetic markers on genes related to thermoregulation.
There is still no evidence of an association between individual differences in interoceptive dimensions and perception of climate change. However, a study shows that individuals who practice techniques that improve interoceptive awareness, such as mindfulness meditation [83], possess higher levels of belief in climate change factors [84]. Although this effect might be ascribable to personality variables, we might speculate that the practice of mindfulness in itself may induce a sensitization effect. These contemplative techniques, when practiced consistently, are known to induce structural psychophysiological changes, such as alterations in brain connectivity with the insula, a major interoceptive hub [85].
Some individual differences in attentional styles and variables have already been associated with climate change attitudes and perceptions. For example, individuals possessing a global-to-local attentional style, as measured by the Navon task, tend to hold higher beliefs in the existence of climate change [86]. This individual preference for global or local perceptual styles has been associated with differences in multisensory integration, which might bias attention either towards detail or towards a more holistic perception [87].

8. Conclusions and Further Research

On the basis of the scientific findings presented in our paper, we propose several research questions that deserve further exploration in future studies (Table 2):
#1: Do populations carrying TRP gene variants experience climate change differently?
Research on these gene variants could reveal vulnerability factors for disorders that arise from climate change distress. Addressing these vulnerabilities could aid in preventing and treating these disorders.
#2 Are there evolutionarily determined climate change sensory cues in humans?
Given the specificity of the sensory effect of some stimuli (such as geosmin molecules), do they constitute innate, evolutionarily driven adaptations towards climate change? Do individuals experiencing climate anxiety or other climate-related affective states have a higher degree of sensitivity to these environmental cues? Besides increasing awareness, these stimuli might be employed as part of novel supporting therapies for distress and psychopathological disorders due to climate change.
#3 What is the degree and speed of climate change that is necessary to happen so that humans can become aware of it?
Arching back to the boiled frog apologue, it would be worthwhile determining something similar to the Just Noticeable Difference (JND; [88]) for a given climate change stimulus that would make an individual evaluate it as anomalous. For example, is there a minimal temperature difference that can be detected as out-of-season, or as caused by climate change? Individuating such a threshold could help in crafting awareness campaigns pointing out specific anomalies as they happen.
#4 Can individual differences in perceptual awareness of climate change be measured?
While differences in climate change beliefs and attitudes have been studied consistently, and are backed by validated psychometric scales, perceptual awareness of climate change remains to be measured in the general population. Developing a specific psychometric scale for this purpose might be worthwhile to investigate this aspect of climate change perception.
#5 How does interoception affect climate perception?
Much like research on genetic factors, research on the involvement of interoceptive factors in climate change perception could open new avenues for body awareness-based treatment applicable to emerging psychopathological disorders like climate anxiety. In this sense, the development of novel specialized mindfulness-based protocols could be promising. In addition, exploring interoceptive correlates of decision-making—such as the somatic marker hypothesis—in the context of sustainability-related choices, may uncover new avenues of research to foster pro-environmental decisions in everyday life.
#6 How are the objective sensory data and subjective perceptions related to climate change represented in the brain?
So far, we have detailed how receptors and low-level perceptual processing can interface with climate change. However, it remains to be investigated how the multisensory integration underlying the perception of climate change manifests itself on a cortical level. Are there specific neural signatures—such as local activations or functional networks—for the perception of climate change effects? On a similar note, it remains to be determined how top-down processes—such as motivated attention—modulate the perception of climate change on a neural level. This can help elucidate how variables ascribable to subjectivity influence the objective factors in climate change perception.
To conclude, we believe that this perspective paper highlights the need to open new avenues of research based on the scientific literature from different disciplines that have been linked here. We underline that further studies are necessary to ascertain whether individual differences in low-level receptors, psychophysiological systems, and cognitive styles can result in differences in climate change perception and whether this has any bearing on attitudes and behaviors relating to climate change. Furthermore, we believe that connecting the physiological knowledge of circadian and ultradian rhythms to that of phenology (i.e., the study of cyclical environmental phenomena [89]) and exploring human–environment interactions in the framework of sensory ecology and sensory pollution might be pivotal to expanding the knowledge of human awareness and reaction to climate change. Pursuing these new avenues might unveil new tools for engagement in climate action, and to improve adaptation, well-being, and healthcare in the changing climate.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.C., A.G. and D.M.; investigation, E.C.; writing—original draft preparation, E.C.; writing—review and editing, D.M., S.G. and S.F.; visualization, E.C.; supervision, D.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. United Nations Framework Convention on Climate Change. Available online: https://unfccc.int/resource/ccsites/zimbab/conven/text/art01.htm (accessed on 4 March 2024).
  2. Luo, Y.; Zhao, J. Attentional and Perceptual Biases of Climate Change. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2021, 42, 22–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Spence, A.; Poortinga, W.; Pidgeon, N. The Psychological Distance of Climate Change. Risk Anal. 2012, 32, 957–972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. O’Donnell, S. The Neurobiology of Climate Change. Sci. Nat. 2018, 105, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Sisodiya, S.M. Hot Brain: Practical Climate Change Advice for Neurologists. Pract. Neurol. 2023, 24, 28–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Clayton, S. Climate Anxiety: Psychological Responses to Climate Change. J. Anxiety Disord. 2020, 74, 102263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Hornsey, M.J.; Harris, E.A.; Bain, P.G.; Fielding, K.S. Meta-Analyses of the Determinants and Outcomes of Belief in Climate Change. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2016, 6, 622–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Weber, E.U. What Shapes Perceptions of Climate Change? WIREs Clim. Change 2010, 1, 332–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Weber, E.U. What Shapes Perceptions of Climate Change? New Research since 2010. WIREs Clim. Chang. 2016, 7, 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Stevens, M. Sensory Ecology, Behaviour, and Evolution; OUP Oxford: Oxford, UK, 2013; ISBN 978-0-19-960178-3. [Google Scholar]
  11. Beever, E.A.; Hall, L.E.; Varner, J.; Loosen, A.E.; Dunham, J.B.; Gahl, M.K.; Smith, F.A.; Lawler, J.J. Behavioral Flexibility as a Mechanism for Coping with Climate Change. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2017, 15, 299–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Intergovernmental Panel On Climate Change (IPCC). Climate Change 2022—Impacts, Adaptation and Vulnerability: Working Group II Contribution to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, 1st ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2023; ISBN 978-1-00-932584-4.
  13. Lennox, R.J.; Chapman, J.M.; Souliere, C.M.; Tudorache, C.; Wikelski, M.; Metcalfe, J.D.; Cooke, S.J. Conservation Physiology of Animal Migration. Conserv. Physiol. 2016, 4, cov072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bradshaw, W.E.; Holzapfel, C.M. Light, Time, and the Physiology of Biotic Response to Rapid Climate Change in Animals. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 2010, 72, 147–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Gwinner, E. Bird Migration: Physiology and Ecophysiology; Springer Science & Business Media: Berlin, Germany, 2012; ISBN 978-3-642-74542-3. [Google Scholar]
  16. Ramenofsky, M. Reconsidering the Role of Photoperiod in Relation to Effects of Precipitation and Food Availability on Spring Departure of a Migratory Bird. Proc. R. Soc. B 2012, 279, 15–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Boone, R.B.; Thirgood, S.J.; Hopcraft, J.G.C. Serengeti Wildebeest Migratory Patterns Modeled from Rainfall and New Vegetation Growth. Ecology 2006, 87, 1987–1994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Chapman, J.W.; Reynolds, D.R.; Wilson, K. Long-Range Seasonal Migration in Insects: Mechanisms, Evolutionary Drivers and Ecological Consequences. Ecol. Lett. 2015, 18, 287–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Shaw, A.K. Drivers of Animal Migration and Implications in Changing Environments. Evol. Ecol. 2016, 30, 991–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Seebacher, F.; Post, E. Climate Change Impacts on Animal Migration. Clim. Chang. Responses 2015, 2, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Marra, P.P.; Francis, C.M.; Mulvihill, R.S.; Moore, F.R. The Influence of Climate on the Timing and Rate of Spring Bird Migration. Oecologia 2005, 142, 307–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Halfwerk, W.; Slabbekoorn, H. Pollution Going Multimodal: The Complex Impact of the Human-Altered Sensory Environment on Animal Perception and Performance. Biol. Lett. 2015, 11, 20141051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Dominoni, D.M.; Halfwerk, W.; Baird, E.; Buxton, R.T.; Fernández-Juricic, E.; Fristrup, K.M.; McKenna, M.F.; Mennitt, D.J.; Perkin, E.K.; Seymoure, B.M.; et al. Why Conservation Biology Can Benefit from Sensory Ecology. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2020, 4, 502–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Martín, J.; López, P. Effects of Global Warming on Sensory Ecology of Rock Lizards: Increased Temperatures Alter the Efficacy of Sexual Chemical Signals. Funct. Ecol. 2013, 27, 1332–1340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Merrick, C.; Filingeri, D. The Evolution of Wetness Perception: A Comparison of Arachnid, Insect and Human Models. J. Therm. Biol. 2019, 85, 102412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Gracheva, E.O.; Ingolia, N.T.; Kelly, Y.M.; Cordero-Morales, J.F.; Hollopeter, G.; Chesler, A.T.; Sánchez, E.E.; Perez, J.C.; Weissman, J.S.; Julius, D. Molecular Basis of Infrared Detection by Snakes. Nature 2010, 464, 1006–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Peier, A.M.; Moqrich, A.; Hergarden, A.C.; Reeve, A.J.; Andersson, D.A.; Story, G.M.; Earley, T.J.; Dragoni, I.; McIntyre, P.; Bevan, S.; et al. A TRP Channel That Senses Cold Stimuli and Menthol. Cell 2002, 108, 705–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Michaiel, A.M.; Bernard, A. Neurobiology and Changing Ecosystems: Toward Understanding the Impact of Anthropogenic Influences on Neurons and Circuits. Front. Neural Circuits 2022, 16, 995354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Eliason, E.J.; Clark, T.D.; Hague, M.J.; Hanson, L.M.; Gallagher, Z.S.; Jeffries, K.M.; Gale, M.K.; Patterson, D.A.; Hinch, S.G.; Farrell, A.P. Differences in Thermal Tolerance among Sockeye Salmon Populations. Science 2011, 332, 109–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Martin, B.T.; Nisbet, R.M.; Pike, A.; Michel, C.J.; Danner, E.M. Sport Science for Salmon and Other Species: Ecological Consequences of Metabolic Power Constraints. Ecol. Lett. 2015, 18, 535–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Luo, Y.; Zhao, J. Motivated Attention in Climate Change Perception and Action. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Wirz-Justice, A.; Skene, D.J.; Münch, M. The Relevance of Daylight for Humans. Biochem. Pharmacol. 2021, 191, 114304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Frumento, S.; Menicucci, D.; Hitchcott, P.K.; Zaccaro, A.; Gemignani, A. Systematic Review of Studies on Subliminal Exposure to Phobic Stimuli: Integrating Therapeutic Models for Specific Phobias. Front. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 654170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Frumento, S.; Gemignani, A.; Menicucci, D. Perceptually Visible but Emotionally Subliminal Stimuli to Improve Exposure Therapies. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Grassini, S.; Railo, H.; Valli, K.; Revonsuo, A.; Koivisto, M. Visual Features and Perceptual Context Modulate Attention towards Evolutionarily Relevant Threatening Stimuli: Electrophysiological Evidence. Emotion 2019, 19, 348–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Kim, M.; Sowndhararajan, K.; Kim, T.; Kim, J.E.; Yang, J.E.; Kim, S. Gender Differences in Electroencephalographic Activity in Response to the Earthy Odorants Geosmin and 2-Methylisoborneol. Appl. Sci. 2017, 7, 876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Michels, N.; Boudrez, S.; Lamprea Pineda, P.A.; Walgraeve, C. Nature-Related Odors Influence Stress and Eating Behavior: A Laboratory Experiment With Pine and Grass Volatiles. Environ. Behav. 2023, 55, 433–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Stobbe, E.; Sundermann, J.; Ascone, L.; Kühn, S. Birdsongs Alleviate Anxiety and Paranoia in Healthy Participants. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 16414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Xiao, R.; Xu, X.Z.S. Temperature Sensation: From Molecular Thermosensors to Neural Circuits and Coding Principles. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 2021, 83, 205–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Voets, T.; Droogmans, G.; Wissenbach, U.; Janssens, A.; Flockerzi, V.; Nilius, B. The Principle of Temperature-Dependent Gating in Cold- and Heat-Sensitive TRP Channels. Nature 2004, 430, 748–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Vellei, M.; de Dear, R.; Inard, C.; Jay, O. Dynamic Thermal Perception: A Review and Agenda for Future Experimental Research. Build. Environ. 2021, 205, 108269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Moore, F.C.; Obradovich, N.; Lehner, F.; Baylis, P. Rapidly Declining Remarkability of Temperature Anomalies May Obscure Public Perception of Climate Change. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 4905–4910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Zaval, L.; Keenan, E.A.; Johnson, E.J.; Weber, E.U. How Warm Days Increase Belief in Global Warming. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2014, 4, 143–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Li, Y.; Johnson, E.J.; Zaval, L. Local Warming: Daily Temperature Change Influences Belief in Global Warming. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 22, 454–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lewandowski, G.W.; Ciarocco, N.J.; Gately, E.L. The Effect of Embodied Temperature on Perceptions of Global Warming. Curr. Psychol. 2012, 31, 318–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Frumento, S.; Gemignani, A.; Menicucci, D. Psychological Mechanisms for Gaining Awareness of (and React-Ing to) Climate Change Challenges: Emotional Levers for Cognitive Remodeling. 2020. Available online: https://hdl.handle.net/11568/1034423 (accessed on 4 March 2024).
  47. Raymond, C.; Matthews, T.; Horton, R.M. The Emergence of Heat and Humidity Too Severe for Human Tolerance. Sci. Adv. 2020, 6, eaaw1838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Filingeri, D.; Fournet, D.; Hodder, S.; Havenith, G. Why Wet Feels Wet? A Neurophysiological Model of Human Cutaneous Wetness Sensitivity. J. Neurophysiol. 2014, 112, 1457–1469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Buoite Stella, A.; Filingeri, D.; Garascia, G.; D’Acunto, L.; Furlanis, G.; Granato, A.; Manganotti, P. Skin Wetness Sensitivity across Body Sites Commonly Affected by Pain in People with Migraine. Headache J. Head Face Pain 2022, 62, 737–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Jousmäki, V.; Hari, R. Parchment-Skin Illusion: Sound-Biased Touch. Curr. Biol. 1998, 8, R190–R191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Sawayama, M.; Adelson, E.H.; Nishida, S. Visual Wetness Perception Based on Image Color Statistics. J. Vis. 2017, 17, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Risso, G.; Preatoni, G.; Valle, G.; Marazzi, M.; Bracher, N.M.; Raspopovic, S. Multisensory Stimulation Decreases Phantom Limb Distortions and Is Optimally Integrated. iScience 2022, 25, 104129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Bear, I.J.; Thomas, R.G. Nature of Argillaceous Odour. Nature 1964, 201, 993–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Robinson, P. Petrichor. Aust. New Zealand J. Public Health 2017, 41, 327–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Liato, V.; Aïder, M. Geosmin as a Source of the Earthy-Musty Smell in Fruits, Vegetables and Water: Origins, Impact on Foods and Water, and Review of the Removing Techniques. Chemosphere 2017, 181, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Polak, E.H.; Provasi, J. Odor Sensitivity to Geosmin Enantiomers. Chem. Senses 1992, 17, 23–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Craig, A.D. How Do You Feel? Interoception: The Sense of the Physiological Condition of the Body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002, 3, 655–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Chen, W.G.; Schloesser, D.; Arensdorf, A.M.; Simmons, J.M.; Cui, C.; Valentino, R.; Gnadt, J.W.; Nielsen, L.; Hillaire-Clarke, C.S.; Spruance, V.; et al. The Emerging Science of Interoception: Sensing, Integrating, Interpreting, and Regulating Signals within the Self. Trends Neurosci. 2021, 44, 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Risen, J.L.; Critcher, C.R. Visceral Fit: While in a Visceral State, Associated States of the World Seem More Likely. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 100, 777–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Zadra, J.R.; Clore, G.L. Emotion and Perception: The Role of Affective Information. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 2011, 2, 676–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Brosch, T. Affect and Emotions as Drivers of Climate Change Perception and Action: A Review. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2021, 42, 15–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Kuhtz-Buschbeck, J.P.; Andresen, W.; Göbel, S.; Gilster, R.; Stick, C. Thermoreception and Nociception of the Skin: A Classic Paper of Bessou and Perl and Analyses of Thermal Sensitivity during a Student Laboratory Exercise. Adv. Physiol. Educ. 2010, 34, 25–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Velasco, M.; Gómez, J.; Blanco, M.; Rodriguez, I. The Cold Pressor test: Pharmacological and Therapeutic Aspects. Am. J. Ther. 1997, 4, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Cortright, D.N.; Krause, J.E.; Broom, D.C. TRP Channels and Pain. Biochim. Et Biophys. Acta (BBA)—Mol. Basis Dis. 2007, 1772, 978–988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Jamison, R.N.; Anderson, K.O.; Slater, M.A. Weather Changes and Pain: Perceived Influence of Local Climate on Pain Complaint in Chronic Pain Patients. Pain 1995, 61, 309–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Horvath, G.; Nagy, K.; Tuboly, G.; Nagy, E. Pain and Weather Associations—Action Mechanisms; Personalized Profiling. Brain Res. Bull. 2023, 200, 110696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Adams, J.D.; Darcy, C.; DeGrasse, A.G.; Jordan, R.; Boscia, C.S. Crosstalk of Pain and Thirst Perception: A Brief Review. Chem. Percept. 2021, 14, 75–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Farrell, M.J.; Egan, G.F.; Zamarripa, F.; Shade, R.; Blair-West, J.; Fox, P.; Denton, D.A. Unique, Common, and Interacting Cortical Correlates of Thirst and Pain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 2416–2421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Green, B.G.; Akirav, C. Individual Differences in Temperature Perception: Evidence of Common Processing of Sensation Intensity of Warmth and Cold. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2007, 24, 71–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Nielsen, C.S.; Staud, R.; Price, D.D. Individual Differences in Pain Sensitivity: Measurement, Causation, and Consequences. J. Pain 2009, 10, 231–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Kim, H.; Mittal, D.P.; Iadarola, M.J.; Dionne, R.A. Genetic Predictors for Acute Experimental Cold and Heat Pain Sensitivity in Humans. J. Med. Genet. 2006, 43, e40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Binder, A.; May, D.; Baron, R.; Maier, C.; Tölle, T.R.; Treede, R.-D.; Berthele, A.; Faltraco, F.; Flor, H.; Gierthmühlen, J.; et al. Transient Receptor Potential Channel Polymorphisms Are Associated with the Somatosensory Function in Neuropathic Pain Patients. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e17387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Forstenpointner, J.; Förster, M.; May, D.; Hofschulte, F.; Cascorbi, I.; Wasner, G.; Gierthmühlen, J.; Baron, R. Short Report: TRPV1-Polymorphism 1911 A>G Alters Capsaicin-Induced Sensory Changes in Healthy Subjects. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0183322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Okamoto, N.; Okumura, M.; Tadokoro, O.; Sogawa, N.; Tomida, M.; Kondo, E. Effect of Single-Nucleotide Polymorphisms in TRPV1 on Burning Pain and Capsaicin Sensitivity in Japanese Adults. Mol. Pain 2018, 14, 1744806918804439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Kim, H.; Neubert, J.K.; San Miguel, A.; Xu, K.; Krishnaraju, R.K.; Iadarola, M.J.; Goldman, D.; Dionne, R.A. Genetic Influence on Variability in Human Acute Experimental Pain Sensitivity Associated with Gender, Ethnicity and Psychological Temperament. Pain 2004, 109, 488–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Gavva, N.R.; Sandrock, R.; Arnold, G.E.; Davis, M.; Lamas, E.; Lindvay, C.; Li, C.-M.; Smith, B.; Backonja, M.; Gabriel, K.; et al. Reduced TRPM8 Expression Underpins Reduced Migraine Risk and Attenuated Cold Pain Sensation in Humans. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 19655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Kozyreva, T.V.; Tkachenko, E.Y.; Potapova, T.A.; Romashchenko, A.G.; Voevoda, M.I. Single-Nucleotide Polymorphism Rs11562975 of the Thermosensitive Ion Channel TRPM8 Gene and Human Sensitivity to Cold and Menthol. Hum. Physiol. 2011, 37, 188–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Key, F.M.; Abdul-Aziz, M.A.; Mundry, R.; Peter, B.M.; Sekar, A.; D’Amato, M.; Dennis, M.Y.; Schmidt, J.M.; Andrés, A.M. Human Local Adaptation of the TRPM8 Cold Receptor along a Latitudinal Cline. PLoS Genet. 2018, 14, e1007298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Soeda, M.; Ohka, S.; Nishizawa, D.; Hasegawa, J.; Nakayama, K.; Ebata, Y.; Ichinohe, T.; Fukuda, K.; Ikeda, K. Cold Pain Sensitivity Is Associated with Single-Nucleotide Polymorphisms of PAR2/F2RL1 and TRPM8. Mol. Pain 2021, 17, 17448069211002009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Igoshin, A.V.; Gunbin, K.V.; Yudin, N.S.; Voevoda, M.I. Searching for Signatures of Cold Climate Adaptation in TRPM8 Gene in Populations of East Asian Ancestry. Front. Genet. 2019, 10, 759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Bell, J.T.; Loomis, A.K.; Butcher, L.M.; Gao, F.; Zhang, B.; Hyde, C.L.; Sun, J.; Wu, H.; Ward, K.; Harris, J.; et al. Differential Methylation of the TRPA1 Promoter in Pain Sensitivity. Nat. Commun. 2014, 5, 2978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Murray, K.O.; Clanton, T.L.; Horowitz, M. Epigenetic Responses to Heat: From Adaptation to Maladaptation. Exp. Physiol. 2022, 107, 1144–1158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Lima-Araujo, G.L.d.; Júnior, G.M.d.S.; Mendes, T.; Demarzo, M.; Farb, N.; Araujo, D.B.d.; Sousa, M.B.C.d. The Impact of a Brief Mindfulness Training on Interoception: A Randomized Controlled Trial. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0273864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Panno, A.; Giacomantonio, M.; Carrus, G.; Maricchiolo, F.; Pirchio, S.; Mannetti, L. Mindfulness, Pro-Environmental Behavior, and Belief in Climate Change: The Mediating Role of Social Dominance. Environ. Behav. 2018, 50, 864–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Sharp, P.B.; Sutton, B.P.; Paul, E.J.; Sherepa, N.; Hillman, C.H.; Cohen, N.J.; Kramer, A.F.; Prakash, R.S.; Heller, W.; Telzer, E.H.; et al. Mindfulness Training Induces Structural Connectome Changes in Insula Networks. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 7929. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Sacchi, S.; Riva, P.; Aceto, A. Myopic about Climate Change: Cognitive Style, Psychological Distance, and Environmentalism. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2016, 65, 68–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Johnston, P.R.; Alain, C.; McIntosh, A.R. Individual Differences in Multisensory Processing Are Related to Broad Differences in the Balance of Local versus Distributed Information. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2022, 34, 846–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Stern, M.K.; Johnson, J.H. Just Noticeable Difference. In The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2010; pp. 1–2. ISBN 978-0-470-47921-6. [Google Scholar]
  89. Inouye, D.W. Climate Change and Phenology. WIREs Clim. Chang. 2022, 13, e764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Model for the psychophysiological factors affecting climate change perception in humans. Sensory systems such as interoception, thermoception, and hygroreception interact with each other and share physiological elements (such as TRP receptors). Sensory systems influence climate change awareness through bottom-up processes, such as visceral fit. Sensory systems are themselves influenced by top-down processes, such as the allocation of attentional resources due to motivated attention.
Figure 1. Model for the psychophysiological factors affecting climate change perception in humans. Sensory systems such as interoception, thermoception, and hygroreception interact with each other and share physiological elements (such as TRP receptors). Sensory systems influence climate change awareness through bottom-up processes, such as visceral fit. Sensory systems are themselves influenced by top-down processes, such as the allocation of attentional resources due to motivated attention.
Brainsci 14 00266 g001
Table 1. TRP Genetic variants associated with sensitivity.
Table 1. TRP Genetic variants associated with sensitivity.
TypeSNP CodeFindingsRef.
TRPM8rs10166942Correlated with geographical latitude.[78]
TRPM8rs10166942[C]More prevalent in hotter climates. Decreases TRP8 expression. Reduced sensitivity to cold.[76]
TRPM8rs10166942[T]More prevalent in colder climates.[76]
TRPM8rs11562975Heterozygous individuals show an increased sensitivity to cold.[77]
TRPM8rs12992084Association with cold pain sensitivity.[79]
TRPM8rs17862920Allelic correlation with average winter temperatures.[80]
TRPM8rs7577262Allelic correlation with average winter temperatures.[80]
TRPV1rs57716901Associated with burning pain sensitivity[74]
TRPV1rs61387317Associated with burning pain sensitivity[74]
TRPV1rs8065080Cold hypoalgesia. Less heat hyperalgesia. Less pinprick hyperalgesia. Mechanical hypoesthesia.[72]
TRPV1rs8065080After capsaicin application: Less warm-detection in heterozygotes/WT. Gain in heat-pain sensitivity in heterozygotes/WT. [73]
SNP: Single nucleotide polymorphism; WT: wild type.
Table 2. Further research questions.
Table 2. Further research questions.
N.Research Question
#1Do populations carrying TRP gene variants experience climate change differently?
#2Are there evolutionarily determined climate change sensory cues in humans?
#3What is the degree and speed of climate change that is necessary to happen so that humans can become aware of it?
#4Can individual differences in perceptual awareness of climate change be measured?
#5How does interoception affect climate perception?
#6How are the objective sensory data and subjective perceptions related to climate change represented in the brain?
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Cipriani, E.; Frumento, S.; Grassini, S.; Gemignani, A.; Menicucci, D. Do Individual Differences in Perception Affect Awareness of Climate Change? Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 266. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030266

AMA Style

Cipriani E, Frumento S, Grassini S, Gemignani A, Menicucci D. Do Individual Differences in Perception Affect Awareness of Climate Change? Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(3):266. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030266

Chicago/Turabian Style

Cipriani, Enrico, Sergio Frumento, Simone Grassini, Angelo Gemignani, and Danilo Menicucci. 2024. "Do Individual Differences in Perception Affect Awareness of Climate Change?" Brain Sciences 14, no. 3: 266. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030266

APA Style

Cipriani, E., Frumento, S., Grassini, S., Gemignani, A., & Menicucci, D. (2024). Do Individual Differences in Perception Affect Awareness of Climate Change? Brain Sciences, 14(3), 266. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030266

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop