Next Article in Journal
The Enduring Challenge of Literacy Issues in Adulthood: Investigating Spelling Deficits among Dyslexic Italian University Students
Previous Article in Journal
Effects of Adapted Physical Activity on White Matter Integrity in Patients with Schizophrenia
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Adapting Minds: Exploring Cognition to Threatened Stimuli in the Post-COVID-19 Landscape Comparing Old and New Concerns about Pandemic

1
Department of Dynamic, Clinical Psychology and Health Studies, “Sapienza” University of Rome, 00185 Rome, Italy
2
Department of Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome, 00185 Rome, Italy
3
Department of Experimental Psychology & Mind, Brain and Behaviour Research Centre (CIMCYC), University of Granada, 18011 Granada, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(7), 711; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070711
Submission received: 24 June 2024 / Revised: 2 July 2024 / Accepted: 11 July 2024 / Published: 15 July 2024

Abstract

:
The global population has been significantly affected by the pandemic in terms of physical and mental health. According to transactional theory, individuals have undergone an adaptation process influenced by cognitive control abilities. Emotional responses to COVID-19-related stimuli may interfere with top-down attentional processes, thereby hindering adaptation. This study aimed to investigate the impact of COVID-19-related stimuli on attentional processing and to determine whether psychological factors could modulate these effects. A sample of 96 healthy undergraduate students participated in an emotional Stroop task in which they were presented with a series of stimuli, including both neutral and negative COVID-19-related as well as non-COVID-19 stimuli. COVID-19-related PTSD, as an index of distress (PTSS), and trait anxiety were evaluated. Results showed that participants were more accurate in identifying COVID-19-related stimuli compared to non-COVID-19 stimuli. Being female and having higher retrospective PTSS scores related to COVID-19 were predictive of faster reaction times for both neutral and negative COVID-19-related stimuli. This heightened attentional bias toward COVID-19-related stimuli suggests that individuals may be more sensitive to stimuli associated with the pandemic. The results suggest that the association between COVID-19 stimuli and attentional biases extends beyond emotional valence, being retrospectively influenced by mental health, suggesting potential pathways to future mental health challenges.

1. Introduction

Despite the passage of time since the 2020 outbreak of COVID-19, it is clear that the consequent pandemic has had a profound impact on the global population. A lasting effect on both physical and mental health is evident in many individuals worldwide [1,2]. A substantial body of the literature has documented a dramatic increase and exacerbation of depression, sleep disturbances, and post-traumatic stress symptoms compared to the pre-COVID-19 era across various populations [3,4]. These conditions were observed not only in patients who contracted the illness but also in the general population. These effects have been attributed to exposure to COVID-19-related stressors, which include social isolation, changes in interpersonal relationships, and health-related concerns such as uncertainty about the pandemic’s duration and fear of infection and its consequences [5,6,7,8]. In this regard, prolonged exposure to COVID-19-related content has also been shown to impact cognitive functioning, particularly concerning the monitoring and control of behaviors [9]. This is supported by evidence highlighting the detrimental effects of persistent worry, anxiety, and distress over an extended period [10,11,12].
In line with these topics, researchers continue to investigate the ongoing role of the COVID-19 pandemic on well-being, given the emergence of new concerns and global crises since the pandemic. The objective is to understand how this period continues to impact our mental health, cognition, and behaviors [12,13].
According to transactional theory [14,15], the process of individual adaptation to significant sources of stress involves appraisals of different degrees of cognitive control. While responses to trauma-related stressors vary widely among individuals [16], it is plausible that such exposure may not only shape our immediate adaptive responses but also influence long-term reactions to stimuli perceived as threatening through brain and cognitive plasticity. This perspective is particularly relevant within the context of the pandemic, given the direct and indirect consequences of the spread of the virus (i.e., social lockdown, social distancing, isolation, changes in daily habits [17,18,19]), which have been confirmed as distressors and causes of post-traumatic symptoms (PTSSs) [20,21,22]. Consequently, emotional responses to pandemic-related stimuli, influenced by both environmental contexts (explicit factors) and individual backgrounds (implicit factors), may interfere with top-down cognitive processes, such as attentional control. This influence could potentially continue to resonate, affecting our lives and well-being to varying degrees over time [23]. Accordingly, examining the processing of pandemic-related stimuli is crucial for understanding the ongoing impact of the pandemic experience. In this context, the concept of attentional bias becomes valuable. The term “attentional bias” refers to the tendency to focus more on negative information, indicating a heightened allocation of attention towards negative stimuli compared to neutral stimuli [24,25,26]. Recent studies have investigated the impact of exposure to negative information about the novel coronavirus (COVID-19) on attentional bias as a result of exposure to negative information about COVID-19 (i.e., news or rumors), which in turn affects levels of anxiety and health outcomes [27,28]. These findings highlight the importance of focusing on these aspects. A study by Cannito and colleagues [29] reported an attentional bias toward virus-related stimuli associated with increased health anxiety during the initial phases of the emergency. Similarly, a recent study by Rubin and Evans [30] found that individuals experiencing grief due to the COVID-19 pandemic showed dysregulation of affective attentional processes, influencing the processing of COVID-19-related stimuli [30]. Building upon these premises and aiming to investigate the potential long-term effects of the traumatic experience of the pandemic, this study explored the impact of stimuli related to the pandemic on attentional control.
Based on previous studies on attentional bias towards threatening stimuli [28,29,31] and considering the highly negative impact of the pandemic experience, this study hypothesized that COVID-19-related stimuli affect attentional processing, given that they are emotionally salient and have a negative valence. Moreover, it was expected that psychological dimensions that have been demonstrated to modulate emotional attentional processing (e.g., anxiety [25]) would play a role in influencing the processing of these stimuli.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The participants were recruited via the dissemination of banners advertising this research through the main social media of the “Sapienza” University of Rome. The G-Power analysis (es = 0.20, alpha = 0.05, beta = 0.80; tested for planned analysis) indicated an adequate sample size of at least 75 respondents. Accordingly, we planned to recruit 100 participants to eventually include a suitable number of participants reporting all the data, reducing the risk of missing value. Of the recruited volunteers, 96 respondents (women: 58.3% of the sample; age range: 19–35; mean age: 24.1; SD = 3.02) met the inclusion criteria of this study (i.e., no diagnosis of COVID-19, no psychopathological or medical conditions, no current medications) and participated in this study. The main characteristics of the sample are shown in Table 1.

2.2. Measures

The demographic and COVID-19-related information were collected through two sections of a survey. The first section gathered demographic details such as gender (male or female), age, education, and medical and psychopathological history in order to assess the individual’s eligibility to participate in this study. The second section focused on the assessment via self-questionnaires of the variables of distress and anxiety.

2.2.1. Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Related to the COVID-19 Questionnaire (COVID-19-PTSD)

The COVID-19-PTSD [21] is a self-report measure specifically designed to assess post-traumatic stress symptoms (PTSSs) in relation to the COVID-19 pandemic. This questionnaire comprises 19 items referring to experiences within the previous seven days, with responses recorded on a 5-point Likert scale. The internal consistency analysis demonstrated excellent reliability for the selected items (Cronbach’s α = 0.94). In order to achieve the objective of this study, the COVID-19-PTSD was administered in two different versions: (i) a first version that requested a retrospective self-reported evaluation, referring to the first six months of the pandemic; (ii) a second version that requested the current self-reported state according to the questionnaire.

2.2.2. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-Y)

The trait scale of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; [32,33]) is a questionnaire designed to evaluate self-reported anxiety on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = not at all; 4 = very much). It assesses both state (current condition) and trait (anxiety as a stable trait) anxiety. Higher scores on the STAI indicate greater anxiety levels. Considering the aim of this study, only trait anxiety (20 items) was assessed. The STAI-Y demonstrated excellent internal consistency of the items (Cronbach’s α values ranging between 0.90 and 0.93).

2.2.3. Emotional Stroop Task

A computerized version of the emotional Stroop task was employed to evaluate executive attention. The target stimuli consisted of colored words (font: Arial; font size: 20; colors: yellow, red, blue, green) that were semantically related to two different categories (type of stimuli: COVID-19; non-COVID-19). Each of them had two different emotional valences (negative, neutral). The stimuli were selected according to an independent validation process. An independent sample of 43 respondents (43% males; mean age = 39.5, SD = 13.5) evaluated 88 words according to emotional valence (from neutral = 0 to negative = 10) and to the association with the COVID-19 pandemic (from not linked to the pandemic = 0 to linked to the pandemic = 10). Table 1 shows the evaluation of each condition of the stimuli included in the emotional Stroop task: (i) COVID-19-related with negative valence; (ii) COVID-19-related with neutral valence; (iii) non-COVID-19 with negative valence; and (iv) non-COVID-19 with neutral valence. Participants were recruited via the dissemination of banners advertising this research through the main social media of the “Sapienza” University of Rome. Of the recruited volunteers, 96 respondents (women: 58.3% of the sample; age range: 19–35; mean age: 24.1; SD = 3.02) met the inclusion criteria of this study (i.e., no diagnosis of COVID-19, no psychopathological or medical conditions, no current medications) and participated in this study. The main characteristics of the sample are shown in Table 2.
The task required participants to respond as quickly and accurately as possible to the color of the ink in which the word on the screen appeared. Participants were instructed to press the key corresponding to the initial letter of the ink color in Italian (key “R” = red; key “V” = green; key “B” = blue; key “G” = yellow). After a brief explanation of the task and a short trial block with stimuli not involved in the experimental procedure, participants completed a block of 160 randomly presented trials (40 for each condition; each word occurs the same number of times for each color). Each trial began with a fixation cross (duration: 500 ms), followed by the presentation of the target stimulus for 2000 ms or until the participant responded. Reaction times (RTs) and proportion of accuracy (number of correct responses/total number of trials) were recorded for the four conditions. The emotional Stroop effect for each type of stimulus was obtained by subtracting the RTs of the neutral trials from the RTs of the negative trials. Two emotional indices (emotional COVID-19, emotional non-COVID-19) were then obtained.

2.3. Procedure

The experiment took place in the Laboratory of Health Psychology at the Sapienza University of Rome. Following the signing of the informed consent form and the presentation of this study, each participant completed the survey for the collection of general information, one version of the COVID-19-PTSD (the retrospective or actual version), and the STAI. Then, the emotional Stroop task was administered in a controlled and quiet setting. Finally, participants completed the other version of the COVID-19-PTSD. This study adhered to the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki, and the entire study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee for Transdisciplinary Research of the Sapienza University of Rome (Resolution No. 87/2023). Participants were permitted to withdraw from this study at any time without providing justification, and no data were retained.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

The primary research question of interest was determining the role of COVID-19-related stimuli on attentional bias. To address this question, a mixed 2 × 2 ANOVA was conducted, considering the type of stimuli (COVID-19/non-COVID-19) and the valence of the stimuli (negative/neutral) on both reaction times and proportion of accuracy. Moreover, an ANOVA considering the emotional Stroop effect in COVID-19 and non-COVID-19 trials was conducted. To assess whether psychological dimensions may affect emotional attentional processing, regression models were fitted for each condition of the emotional Stroop task (negative COVID-19, neutral COVID-19, negative non-COVID-19, neutral non-COVID-19). The model included as predictors the retrospective self-reported COVID-19-PTSD score, the current levels of self-reported COVID-19-PTSD, the STAI score, and sex. For all statistical analyses, significance was set at p < 0.05. The analyses were conducted using Statistica software (version 10.0, Dell, Round Rock, TX, USA).

3. Results

The ANOVA on the reaction times of the emotional Stroop task did not reveal a significant effect for the type of stimuli (COVID-19; non-COVID-19; F1,4 = 2.01; p = 0.16), for the valence (negative; neutral; F1,94 = 1.38; p = 0.25), nor the type of stimuli x valence interaction (F2,94 < 1; p = 0.37).
ANOVA on the proportion of accuracy showed higher accuracy in identifying COVID-19-related stimuli than non-COVID-19 stimuli (F1,94 = 4.78; p = 0.03). However, no other significant effects were highlighted (all F < 1). For further details, see Table 3.

Regression Models

The single predictive models for each condition of the emotional Stroop task (see Table 3) showed that the models were significant for COVID-19-related stimuli in both negative and neutral valence. Specifically, for negative valence, the four predictors accounted for 14% of the total variance in the outcome (F 4, 94 = 3.14, p = 0.02). Faster reaction times for COVID-19-related negative stimuli were significantly associated with higher retrospective COVID-19-PTSD (b = −1.51, SE = 0.57, t = −2.62, p = 0.01) and in females (b = 34.83, SE = 13.91, t = 2.50, p = 0.01). (see Table 3). Also, for COVID-19-related neutral stimuli, the four predictors accounted for 13% of the total variance in the outcome (F 4, 94 = 2.79, p = 0.02). Faster reaction times for COVID-19-related neutral stimuli were significantly associated with higher retrospective COVID-19-PTSD (b = −1.25, SE = 0.61, t = −2.05, p = 0.04) and in females (b = 40.65, SE = 14.82, t = 2.74, p = 0.01). (see Table 4).

4. Discussion

The global impact of the COVID-19 pandemic was undoubtedly profound and multifaceted, particularly in terms of mental health, behavioral patterns (i.e., sleep [34,35]), and social interactions (e.g., [17,36,37]). Individuals exhibited heightened sensitivity to any event that could be linked back to the pandemic, although this sensitivity manifested in varying degrees of severity across the globe. From this premise, our study investigated cognitive responses to threatened or possibly threatening stimuli in the post-COVID-19 landscape, comparing old and new concerns. The importance of delineating the manner in which individuals respond to negative stimuli, particularly those associated with stressful situations, has been extensively reported in the literature. This response is thought to be associated with the role of attentional bias in the development of maladaptive coping mechanisms in response to negative life experiences [38,39].
Consequently, it is important to verify whether and to what extent cognitive responses are still affected by the traumatic resonance of the pandemic. This is relevant for understanding how significantly an unexpected and sudden event can influence long-term perspectives. Conversely, this study may provide direct evidence regarding the similarities and differences in stimuli, the elaboration of negative emotional stimuli, and the specific and direct experiences of events that have been overexposed in the media.
The results indicate that high selective attention to COVID-19-related stimuli, as evidenced by faster reaction times at the emotional Stroop task for both neutral and negative valence, was consistently correlated with higher retrospective COVID-19-PTSS but not with current concerns. To elucidate these findings, it is crucial to revisit the events during the pandemic’s onset. The unprecedented changes, such as lockdowns, social distancing measures, and economic disruptions, have led to elevated stress, anxiety, and subsequent PTSD worldwide [21,40,41]. Our previous study reported a significant increase in mental distress and the emergence of COVID-19-PTSS, estimating an increase during the first months of the pandemic [17,20,22,34,35], as similarly reported by our retrospective data in this study. This surge can be attributed to a number of factors, including fear of infection, grief from the loss of loved ones, and uncertainty about the future [42,43,44]. However, as many authors have suggested, the extent to which this is the case may depend on the high dissemination of news and the worrying tone adopted by the media, which have caused an increase in concern among the population. The present study also suggests a moderate attentional bias toward COVID-19-related stimuli. Specifically, although no differences in reaction times were observed in the performance of the task, individuals were more accurate in responding to COVID-19-related stimuli. This indicates a tendency for attention to be drawn towards these stimuli when presented alongside non-COVID-19-related stimuli. This tendency is consistent with the fact that selective attention is powerfully biased toward threat-related information as an evolutionarily adaptive response in environments where dangers constantly threaten survival and reproductive advantage [45,46]. However, it is notable that the valence of the stimuli did not influence the performance, suggesting that we should reflect on the role of threat in attentional processing. This implies that all COVID-19-related stimuli have assumed greater salience, regardless of their valence. This also provides an interesting hypothesis regarding the potential influence of overstimulation with COVID-19-related stimuli rather than the threat component itself. Given the timing of this study, it is surprising that this effect was observed across the sample. An intriguing finding is that experiential and demographic factors influenced the salience of COVID-19-related stimuli, resulting in a varied pattern of attentional bias. Specifically, we found that variables such as sex and experience of PTSS related to COVID-19 were significant predictors of attentional bias.
These variables would be the causes of the increased sensitivity to negative stimuli associated with COVID-19 [29]. Research indicates that continuous exposure to distressing aspects of the pandemic exacerbated anxiety and depression [41,47]. Also, Holmes et al. [48] highlighted that the pervasive sense of threat and uncertainty during the pandemic heightened alertness and fear [49,50,51]. In this context, as previously suggested by our studies, the pandemic-related symptomatology associated with COVID-19-related post-traumatic stress disorder was associated with deficits in inhibiting preponderant responses, indicating an executive function deficit affecting goal-directed actions in general, particularly in at-risk populations [9]. Moreover, as reported by Cannito’s study [52], an increase in anxiety levels would result in an attentional bias toward virus-related stimuli, influencing perceptions of future consequences [52]. Additionally, individuals experiencing higher pandemic-related stress showed lower attentional bias towards COVID-19-related information in the presence of higher levels of alexithymia [53]. Our findings support and reinforce these observations, highlighting that responses to COVID-19 stimuli, irrespective of valence and whether or not they are activating, reflect genuine reactions to the stimulus itself, shaped more by concerns during the pandemic period than current worries about the virus, as well as a general personological trait of anxiety. This suggests that COVID-19-related distress has been reduced, as evidenced by the reduction in PTSS scores reported by participants (differences in the scores between retrospective and current PTSS). It also indicates that past experiences related to the pandemic have had a lasting impact on perceptions and reactions. Finally, it highlights the long-term psychological and behavioral effects of past emergencies.
In general, the results of this study should be interpreted within the context of a comprehensive theoretical and empirical framework. While there is a lack of specific models that elucidate how attentional processes interact with COVID-19 stimuli to influence future mental health concerns, insights from the related literature are valuable. For example, research on anxiety disorders suggests that attentional capture by threatening stimuli facilitates extensive information processing [54,55]. Similarly, studies on depression suggest that heightened attention to sad stimuli may sustain chronic depressive symptoms through rumination [56]. Building on these frameworks, we propose that increased attention to COVID-19-specific terms may activate negative emotions and thoughts associated with pandemic-related losses, potentially impairing conflict resolution abilities by taxing executive functions [57]. Given the extensive media coverage of the COVID-19 pandemic, biased attention towards pandemic-related content could reinforce negative associations with loss, potentially disrupting the grieving process. It is possible that repeated exposure to COVID-19 stimuli may cumulatively sustain elevated distress levels, increasing the risk of subsequent mental health issues. This cognitive response may be associated with continual exposure to distressing information, which may lead to cognitive overload, impaired decision-making, reduced flexibility, and increased susceptibility to misinformation [58,59,60,61,62,63]. The phenomenon of “Doomscrolling”, i.e., the compulsive consumption of negative news, has become prevalent during the pandemic. Garfin, Silver, and Holman [64] discussed how constant exposure to alarming news creates a feedback loop of negative emotions, reinforcing feelings of dread and helplessness. This behavior exacerbates existing mental health issues and contributes to the development of new psychological problems. Liu [65] found a direct correlation between the frequency of COVID-19 news consumption and increased psychological distress among young adults, indicating that higher engagement with pandemic-related news predicts symptoms of anxiety, depression, and distress related to COVID-19 (e.g., PTSS, PTSD).
Although these new insights into the role of the pandemic in individual functioning are important, it is important to acknowledge some limitations of this research. Firstly, the cross-sectional nature of this study precludes the possibility of drawing definitive conclusions regarding the direct influence of the COVID-19 pandemic on attentional processes, despite the use of retrospective inquiries. Additionally, the use of the emotional Stroop task in this study may influence the interpretations of attentional patterns due to the large debate regarding the optimal interpretation of its outcomes. Specifically, different perspectives have suggested different interpretations that consider the outcome of the Stroop task as indices of attentional control, cognitive inhibition, or other attentive processes (e.g., [66,67]).
Another limitation of this study is the administration of two different versions of the COVID-19-PTSD: (i) retrospective self-reported traumatic experience with the COVID-19 pandemic and (ii) current distress associated with COVID-19. The retrospective version may be subjected to memory biases and should be carefully considered as an index of the real PTSS perceived during the emergency phase. However, considering previous studies the indication emerging from our study may provide reliable insight about the role of past self-reported distress. Finally, it is important to extend these studies to multiple populations and contexts (what about healthcare professionals who were the most burdened by the pandemic experience?).

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, despite these limitations, our findings indicate that attentional biases may be a significant consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic, warranting further investigation in the literature. These findings provide a rationale for future research into attentional bias modification strategies that may alleviate mental health concerns and eventually influence the behaviors of those who experienced severe psychological consequences after the emergency period. Together, these findings suggest that the association between COVID-19 stimuli and attentional biases extends beyond emotional valence, being retrospectively influenced by mental health during the pandemic, pointing towards pathways to future mental health challenges. It may be beneficial to assess additional aspects of attentional control and their interactions with other attentional networks (e.g., [68]).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.C., G.F., F.F., E.D.P., V.L., and R.T.; methodology, M.C., G.F., F.F., E.D.P., and I.C.; validation, G.F., F.F. formal analysis, G.F., F.F., I.C., G.T., B.B., M.C., and G.M.; investigation, I.C., G.T., B.B., R.P., and G.M.; resources, M.C.; data curation, G.F., F.F.; writing—original draft preparation, G.F., F.F., and M.C.; writing—review and editing, G.F., F.F., I.C., and M.C.; visualization, M.C., G.F., F.F., E.D.P., V.L., R.T., I.C., G.T., B.B., R.P., and G.M.; supervision, M.C.; project administration, I.C., G.T., B.B., R.P., and G.M. funding acquisition, M.C., E.D.P., V.L., F.F., and G.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the UNIVERSITY OF ROME SAPIENZA (Fondi di Ateneo medi), grant number RM12117A7E46990A.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the entire study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee for Transdisciplinary Research of the Sapienza University of Rome (Resolution No. 87/2023; 26 August 2023).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author due to privacy restrictions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Alizadeh, H.; Sharifi, A.; Damanbagh, S.; Nazarnia, H.; Nazarnia, M. Impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on the social sphere and lessons for crisis management: A literature review. Nat. Hazards 2023, 117, 2139–2164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Ng, E.; Stanton, P. The great resignation: Managing people in a post COVID-19 pandemic world. Pers. Rev. 2023, 52, 401–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Kauhanen, L.; Wan Mohd Yunus, W.M.A.; Lempinen, L.; Peltonen, K.; Gyllenberg, D.; Mishina, K.; Gilbert, S.; Bastola, K.; Brown, J.S.L.; Sourander, A. A systematic review of the mental health changes of children and young people before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2023, 32, 995–1013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Sun, Y.; Wu, Y.; Fan, S.; Santo, T.D.; Li, L.; Jiang, X.; Li, K.; Wang, Y.; Tasleem, A.; Krishnan, A.; et al. Comparison of mental health symptoms before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: Evidence from a systematic review and meta-analysis of 134 cohorts. BMJ 2023, 380, e074224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Sauer, K.S.; Jungmann, S.M.; Witthöft, M. Emotional and behavioral consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic: The role of health anxiety, intolerance of uncertainty, and distress (in) tolerance. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Peng, S.; Yang, X.Y.; Yang, T.; Zhang, W.; Cottrell, R.R. Uncertainty stress, and its impact on disease fear and prevention behavior during the COVID-19 epidemic in China: A panel study. Am. J. Health Behav. 2021, 45, 334–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Porcelli, P. Fear, anxiety and health-related consequences after the COVID-19 epidemic. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 2020, 17, 103–111. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  8. Korte, C.; Friedberg, R.D.; Wilgenbusch, T.; Paternostro, J.K.; Brown, K.; Kakolu, A.; Tiller-Ormord, J.; Baweja, R.; Cassar, M.; Barnowski, A.; et al. Intolerance of uncertainty and health-related anxiety in youth amid the COVID-19 pandemic: Understanding and weathering the continuing storm. J. Clin. Psychol. Med. Settings 2021, 29, 645–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Favieri, F.; Forte, G.; Agostini, F.; Giovannoli, J.; Di Pace, E.; Langher, V.; Tambelli, R.; Pazzaglia, M.; Giannini, A.M.; Casagrande, M. The cognitive consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic on members of the general population in Italy: A preliminary study on executive inhibition. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 11, 170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Wang, S.; Quan, L.; Chavarro, J.E.; Slopen, N.; Kubzansky, L.D.; Koenen, K.C.; Kang, J.H.; Weisskopf, M.G.; Branch-Elliman, W.; Roberts, A.L. Associations of depression, anxiety, worry, perceived stress, and loneliness prior to infection with risk of post–COVID-19 conditions. JAMA Psychiatry 2022, 79, 1081–1091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Qi, T.; Hu, T.; Ge, Q.-Q.; Zhou, X.-N.; Li, J.-M.; Jiang, C.-L. COVID-19 pandemic related long-term chronic stress on the prevalence of depression and anxiety in the general population. BMC Psychiatry 2021, 21, 380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Adamis, A.M.; Cole, D.A.; Olatunji, B.O. Intolerance of Uncertainty and Worry Prospectively Predict COVID-19 Anxiety and Distress: A 4-Year Longitudinal Study. Behav. Ther. 2024, 55, 320–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Mullins, R.J.; Meeker, T.J.; Vinch, P.M.; Tulloch, I.K.; Saffer, M.I.; Chien, J.-H.; Bienvenu, O.J.; Lenz, F.A. A cross-sectional time course of COVID-19 related worry, perceived stress, and general anxiety in the context of post-traumatic stress disorder-like symptomatology. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 7178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Folkman, S. Personal control and stress and coping processes: A theoretical analysis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1984, 46, 839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Matthews, G.; Schwean, V.L.; Campbell, S.E.; Saklofske, D.H.; Mohamed, A.A.R. Personality, self-regulation, and adaptation: A cognitive-social framework. In Handbook of Self-Regulation; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2000; pp. 171–207. [Google Scholar]
  16. Faye, C.; McGowan, J.C.; Denny, C.A.; David, D.J. Neurobiological mechanisms of stress resilience and implications for the aged population. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 2018, 16, 234–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Favieri, F.; Forte, G.; Tambelli, R.; Casagrande, M. The Italians in the time of coronavirus: Psychosocial aspects of the unexpected COVID-19 pandemic. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 551924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Chiesa, V.; Antony, G.; Wismar, M.; Rechel, B. COVID-19 pandemic: Health impact of staying at home, social distancing and ‘lockdown’measures—A systematic review of systematic reviews. J. Public Health 2021, 43, e462–e481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Benke, C.; Autenrieth, L.K.; Asselmann, E.; Pané-Farré, C.A. Lockdown, quarantine measures, and social distancing: Associations with depression, anxiety and distress at the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic among adults from Germany. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 293, 113462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Forte, G.; Favieri, F.; Tambelli, R.; Casagrande, M. The enemy which sealed the world: Effects of COVID-19 diffusion on the psychological state of the Italian population. J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 1802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Forte, G.; Favieri, F.; Tambelli, R.; Casagrande, M. COVID-19 pandemic in the Italian population: Validation of a post-traumatic stress disorder questionnaire and prevalence of PTSD symptomatology. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Tambelli, R.; Forte, G.; Favieri, F.; Casagrande, M. Effects of the coronavirus pandemic on mental health: A possible model of the direct and indirect impact of the pandemic on PTSD symptomatology COVID-19 related. Psychol. Hub 2021, 38, 23–30. [Google Scholar]
  23. Kimble, M.O.; Fleming, K.; Bandy, C.; Zambetti, A. Attention to novel and target stimuli in trauma survivors. Psychiatry Res. 2010, 178, 501–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Favieri, F.; Forte, G.; Marotta, A.; Casagrande, M. Food-related attentional bias in individuals with normal weight and overweight: A study with a flicker task. Nutrients 2020, 12, 492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Veerapa, E.; Grandgenevre, P.; El Fayoumi, M.; Vinnac, B.; Haelewyn, O.; Szaffarczyk, S.; Vaiva, G.; D’hondt, F. Attentional bias towards negative stimuli in healthy individuals and the effects of trait anxiety. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 11826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Abado, E.; Richter, T.; Okon-Singer, H. Attention bias toward negative stimuli. In Cognitive Biases in Health and Psychiatric Disorders; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2020; pp. 19–40. [Google Scholar]
  27. Sorokowski, P.; Groyecka, A.; Kowal, M.; Sorokowska, A.; Białek, M.; Lebuda, I.; Dobrowolska, M.; Zdybek, P.; Karwowski, M. Can information about pandemics increase negative attitudes toward foreign groups? A case of COVID-19 outbreak. Sustainability 2020, 12, 4912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lee, Y.E.; Seo, J.H.; Kim, S.T.; Jeon, S.; Park, C.I.; Kim, S.J.; Kang, J.I. Psychological factors associated with COVID-19 related anxiety and depression in young adults during the COVID-19 pandemic. PLoS ONE 2023, 18, e0286636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Cannito, L.; Di Crosta, A.; Palumbo, R.; Ceccato, I.; Anzani, S.; La Malva, P.; Palumbo, R.; Di Domenico, A. Health anxiety and attentional bias toward virus-related stimuli during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 16476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Rubin, M.; Evans, T. COVID-19 Related Loss is Reliably Associated with Attentional Capture and Facilitation by COVID Related Stimuli: Evidence from the Emotional Stroop Dilution Task. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2023, 48, 435–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Li, S.; Li, X. The relationship between attentional bias, anxiety sensitivity, and depression and anxiety symptoms: Evidence from the COVID-19 Pandemic in China. Front. Public Health 2022, 10, 832819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Spielberger, C.D. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults; American Psychological Association (APA): Washington, DC, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  33. Pedrabissi, L.; Santinello, M. Verifica della validità dello STAI forma Y di Spielberger. Giunti Organ. Spec. 1989, 191–192, 11–14. [Google Scholar]
  34. Casagrande, M.; Favieri, F.; Tambelli, R.; Forte, G. The enemy who sealed the world: Effects quarantine due to the COVID-19 on sleep quality, anxiety, and psychological distress in the Italian population. Sleep Med. 2020, 75, 12–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Casagrande, M.; Forte, G.; Tambelli, R.; Favieri, F. The coronavirus pandemic: A possible model of the direct and indirect impact of the pandemic on sleep quality in Italians. Nat. Sci. Sleep 2021, 13, 191–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Calbi, M.; Langiulli, N.; Ferroni, F.; Montalti, M.; Kolesnikov, A.; Gallese, V.; Umiltà, M.A. The consequences of COVID-19 on social interactions: An online study on face covering. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 2601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Curelaru, A.; Marzolf, S.J.; Provost, J.-C.K.G.; Zeon, H.H.H. Social isolation in dementia: The effects of COVID-19. J. Nurse Pract. 2021, 17, 950–953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Luecken, L.J.; Tartaro, J.; Appelhans, B. Strategic coping responses and attentional biases. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2004, 28, 23–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Prochwicz, K.; Kłosowska, J.; Dembińska, A. The mediating role of stress in the relationship between attention to threat bias and psychotic-like experiences depends on coping strategies. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 520033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Leung, C.M.C.; Ho, M.K.; Bharwani, A.A.; Cogo-Moreira, H.; Wang, Y.; Chow, M.S.C.; Fan, X.; Galea, S.; Leung, G.M.; Ni, M.Y. Mental disorders following COVID-19 and other epidemics: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Transl. Psychiatry 2022, 12, 205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Manchia, M.; Gathier, A.W.; Yapici-Eser, H.; Schmidt, M.V.; de Quervain, D.; van Amelsvoort, T.; Bisson, J.I.; Cryan, J.F.; Howes, O.D.; Pinto, L.; et al. The impact of the prolonged COVID-19 pandemic on stress resilience and mental health: A critical review across waves. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2022, 55, 22–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Menzies, R.E.; Neimeyer, R.A.; Menzies, R.G. Death anxiety, loss, and grief in the time of COVID-19. Behav. Chang. 2020, 37, 111–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Gokseven, Y.; Ozturk, G.Z.; Karadeniz, E.; Sarı, E.; Tas, B.G.; Ozdemir, H.M. The fear of COVID-19 infection in older people. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 2022, 35, 460–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Firouzkouhi, M.; Alimohammadi, N.; Abdollahimohammad, A.; Bagheri, G.; Farzi, J. Bereaved families views on the death of loved ones due to COVID 19: An integrative review. OMEGA-J. Death Dying 2023, 88, 4–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Gupta, R.S.; Kujawa, A.; Vago, D.R. The neural chronometry of threat-related attentional bias: Event-related potential (ERP) evidence for early and late stages of selective attentional processing. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2019, 146, 20–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Öhman, A.; Flykt, A.; Esteves, F. Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2001, 130, 466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Lahav, Y. Psychological distress related to COVID-19–the contribution of continuous traumatic stress. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 277, 129–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Holmes, E.A.; O’Connor, R.C.; Perry, V.H.; Tracey, I.; Wessely, S.; Arseneault, L.; Ballard, C.; Christensen, H.; Silver, R.C.; Everall, I.; et al. Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: A call for action for mental health science. Lancet Psychiatry 2020, 7, 547–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. van Stralen, D.; Mercer, T.A. During Pandemic COVID-19, the High-Reliability Organization (HRO) Identifies Maladaptive Stress Behaviors: The Stress-Fear-Threat Cascade. Neonatol. Today 2020, 15, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Coelho, C.M.; Suttiwan, P.; Arato, N.; Zsido, A.N. On the nature of fear and anxiety triggered by COVID-19. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 581314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Sanchez-Gomez, M.; Giorgi, G.; Finstad, G.L.; Urbini, F.; Foti, G.; Mucci, N.; Zaffina, S.; León-Perez, J.M. COVID-19 pandemic as a traumatic event and its associations with fear and mental health: A cognitive-activation approach. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 7422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Cannito, L.; Ceccato, I.; Annunzi, E.; Bortolotti, A.; D’intino, E.; Palumbo, R.; D’addario, C.; Di Domenico, A.; Palumbo, R. Bored with boredom? Trait boredom predicts internet addiction through the mediating role of attentional bias toward social networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2023, 17, 1179142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Lee, S.-H.; Lee, K.-T. The impact of pandemic-related stress on attentional bias and anxiety in alexithymia during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci. Rep. 2023, 13, 6327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Cisler, J.M.; Koster, E.H.W. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 30, 203–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Taylor, C.T.; Cross, K.; Amir, N. Attentional control moderates the relationship between social anxiety symptoms and attentional disengagement from threatening information. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2016, 50, 68–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Koster, E.H.W.; De Lissnyder, E.; Derakshan, N.; De Raedt, R. Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2011, 31, 138–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Herzog, P.; Barth, C.; Rief, W.; Brakemeier, E.-L.; Kube, T. How expectations shape the formation of intrusive memories: An experimental study using the trauma film paradigm. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2022, 46, 809–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Bong, C.L.; Fraser, K.; Oriot, D. Cognitive load and stress in simulation. In Comprehensive Healthcare Simulation; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 3–17. [Google Scholar]
  59. Starcke, K.; Wolf, O.T.; Markowitsch, H.J.; Brand, M. Anticipatory stress influences decision making under explicit risk conditions. Behav. Neurosci. 2008, 122, 1352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Starcke, K.; Brand, M. Decision making under stress: A selective review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012, 36, 1228–1248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Goldfarb, E.V.; Froböse, M.I.; Cools, R.; Phelps, E.A. Stress and cognitive flexibility: Cortisol increases are associated with enhanced updating but impaired switching. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 29, 14–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Greene, C.M.; Murphy, G. Individual differences in susceptibility to false memories for COVID-19 fake news. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 2020, 5, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Phillips-Wren, G.; Adya, M. Decision making under stress: The role of information overload, time pressure, complexity, and uncertainty. J. Decis. Syst. 2020, 29, 213–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Garfin, D.R.; Silver, R.C.; Holman, E.A. The novel coronavirus (COVID-2019) outbreak: Amplification of public health consequences by media exposure. Health Psychol. 2020, 39, 355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Liu, P.L. COVID-19 information seeking on digital media and preventive behaviors: The mediation role of worry. Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw. 2020, 23, 677–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Wylie, G.; Allport, A. Task switching and the measurement of ‘switch costs’. Psychol. Res. 2000, 63, 212–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Forte, G.; Troisi, G.; Favieri, F.; Casagrande, M. Inhibition changes across the lifespan: Experimental evidence from the Stroop task. BMC Psychol. 2024, 12, 336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Casagrande, M.; Agostini, F.; Favieri, F.; Forte, G.; Giovannoli, J.; Guarino, A.; Marotta, A.; Doricchi, F.; Martella, D. Age-related changes in hemispherical specialization for attentional networks. Brain Sci. 2022, 54, 1115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Main characteristics of the sample.
Table 1. Main characteristics of the sample.
Mean (SD)CI 95%
Age24.1 (3.02)23.4–24.7
Years of education17.6 (2.10)17.1–18.0
Trait anxiety (STAI)47.5 (11.00)45.1–49.9
Retrospective COVID-19-PTSS *22.3 (13.60)19.3–25.3
Current COVID-19-PTSD6.4 (10.00)4.19–8.61
* % Percentage of critical score at the COVID-19-PTSD: 28 (34.1%). CI: confidence interval; PTSD: post-traumatic stress disorder; SD: standard deviation.
Table 2. Mean and standard deviation (SD) of the stimuli adopted in the emotional Stroop task.
Table 2. Mean and standard deviation (SD) of the stimuli adopted in the emotional Stroop task.
COVID-19-Related StimuliNon-COVID-19 Stimuli
NegativeNeutralNegativeNeutral
COVID-19
Association
8.60 (1.34)9.40 (0.90)1.0 (0.71)0.60 (0.55)
Valence8.60 (0.55)2.80 (0.84)9.80 (0.45)1.40 (0.55)
WordsVirus,
Pandemia,
Morti,
Reclusione,
Polmonite,
Disinfettante,
Mascherina,
Decreto,
Tampone,
Vaccino,
Guerra,
Bombardamento,
Cancro,
Terremoto,
Omicidio,
Raccoglitore,
Termosifone,
Triangolo,
Etichetta,
Muro,
English translationVirus,
Pandemic, Deaths,
Reclusion,
Pneumonia
Disinfectant,
Mask,
Decree,
Swab,
Vaccine
War,
Bombing,
Cancer,
Earthquake,
Murder
Binder,
Radiator,
Triangle,
Label,
Wall
Table 3. Descriptive statistics of COVID-19 and non-COVID-19-related stimuli.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics of COVID-19 and non-COVID-19-related stimuli.
COVID-19-Related StimuliNon-COVID-19 Stimuli
NegativeNeutralNegativeNeutral
M (SD)CI 95%M (SD)CI 95%M (SD)CI 95%M (SD)CI 95%
Reaction Time626.0
(68.4)
612–640625 (68.9)611–639632 (65.6)618–645626 (63.4)613–619
Proportion of
accuracy
0.97 (0.03)0.96–0.970.97 (0.03)0.96–0.980.96 (0.04)0.95–0.970.96 (0.04)0.95–0.97
Emotional effects0.78 (34.4) [−6.31–7.11]5.40 (37.2) [−2.19–3.13]
CI: confidence interval; SD: standard deviation.
Table 4. Summary of regression analyses.
Table 4. Summary of regression analyses.
COVID-19-Related StimuliNon-COVID-19 Stimuli
NegativeNeutralNegativeNeutral
R0.370.350.290.30
R20.140.130.090.10
F3.142.791.852.39
p0.020.030.130.06
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Forte, G.; Favieri, F.; Corbo, I.; Troisi, G.; Marselli, G.; Blasutto, B.; Ponce, R.; Di Pace, E.; Langher, V.; Tambelli, R.; et al. Adapting Minds: Exploring Cognition to Threatened Stimuli in the Post-COVID-19 Landscape Comparing Old and New Concerns about Pandemic. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 711. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070711

AMA Style

Forte G, Favieri F, Corbo I, Troisi G, Marselli G, Blasutto B, Ponce R, Di Pace E, Langher V, Tambelli R, et al. Adapting Minds: Exploring Cognition to Threatened Stimuli in the Post-COVID-19 Landscape Comparing Old and New Concerns about Pandemic. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(7):711. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070711

Chicago/Turabian Style

Forte, Giuseppe, Francesca Favieri, Ilaria Corbo, Giovanna Troisi, Giulia Marselli, Barbara Blasutto, Renato Ponce, Enrico Di Pace, Viviana Langher, Renata Tambelli, and et al. 2024. "Adapting Minds: Exploring Cognition to Threatened Stimuli in the Post-COVID-19 Landscape Comparing Old and New Concerns about Pandemic" Brain Sciences 14, no. 7: 711. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070711

APA Style

Forte, G., Favieri, F., Corbo, I., Troisi, G., Marselli, G., Blasutto, B., Ponce, R., Di Pace, E., Langher, V., Tambelli, R., & Casagrande, M. (2024). Adapting Minds: Exploring Cognition to Threatened Stimuli in the Post-COVID-19 Landscape Comparing Old and New Concerns about Pandemic. Brain Sciences, 14(7), 711. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070711

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop