An Unavoidable Mind-Set Reversal: Consciousness in Vision Science
Abstract
:1. Introduction: Seeing
- Second, the relationship between explanans (the necessary and sufficient conditions ruled by the laws of organization) and explanandum (the appearance within perception) simultaneously bond together in the phenomenon, making the task of detaching them extremely difficult.
- The difficulty of analyzing and testing the object of research (i.e., the percept and the varieties of its modes of appearance) in the brief experience of actual perception is separate from the unavoidable and simultaneous dimension of the subject’s awareness (the act, for example of “seeing”).
- The fact is that the unity and simultaneity of the momentary experience are only sub-whole in a more comprehensive entirety of experience (the psychic flow).
- Finally, a continued lack of knowledge regarding the nature of psychic energy. However, as Volkelt observed a long time ago, this is precisely the task of a psychology of consciousness [54].
- A brief explanation of the phenomenological standpoint in perception is provided (Section 2).
- The meaning of subjective in seeing and how it is experimentally addressed is specified (Section 3).
- The temporal scanning of awareness is presented and discussed (Section 7).
- The role of place in awareness is discussed (Section 8).
- Preliminary steps on the resulting geometry of seeing are presented (Section 9).
- The complexity of the phenomenal continuum in awareness is pointed out (Section 10).
- Conclusions and examples for a possibly fruitful collaboration between experimental phenomenology and neuroscience are provided (Section 11).
2. A Brief Exposition of the Phenomenological Standpoint in Perception
Regarding the Task of Experimentally Developing Subjective Perception
3. What Is Subjective in Seeing or “Give to the Subject What Belongs to the Subject”
- The presence and role of both empirical and internal factors of awareness.
- The phases of an actual seeing experience.
- The subjective space–time continuum is a condition for the existence of phenomena in awareness.
4. How to Identify the Factors of Awareness
5. Empirical Factors: Field Organization
- A flat rotating ellipse with a constant shape on a single plane.
- An elastic ring or self-distorting figure on the plane that is stably oriented in space.
- A rigid circular disk or constant figure that tilts back and forth in space while rotating.
- The stereokinetic phenomenon is perceived in awareness as a glowing and spacious object if compared with two-dimensional surfaces such as a drawn ellipse.
- All the parts are perceived as moving together.
- Information is given on the location (and direction) of the phenomena deploying a change during their deployment.
- The presence of empirical factors is wired into the phenomenal field [112].
6. Internal Factors: Subjective Timing
7. Scanning Awareness
8. Subjective Localizing
9. What and Where in the Geometry of Seeing
- Reasonably precise locations in space (where).
- (Usually) 3D objects (what), seen at first sight (immediately, globally).
- Other “parts” of former 3D objects.
- Surfaces, lines, and points are perceived equally as visual objects.
- Visual points are real perceptual objects.
- Visual points can be perceived in two different modalities: as objects and as locations in space.
- The boundary between a visual point and something else, such as a disc or a sphere, can be identified and measured (in the three tests, the thresholds ranged between 0.2 and 0.35 deg, depending on the test design).
9.1. Lines
- The what and the where.
- When the line ceases to appear as a line and becomes a surface or hole.
- If the line has a specific orientation.
- If color and texture influence its appearance.
- If the spatial attitude (curvature, shape position) is changed to a more complex visual configuration.
- If and how it is remote.
- If it is cross-modal (the cross-modality of lines is evident when complex musical pieces are transcribed into lines of different width, similar to Klee’s transcription of Bach’s Adagio. See [249])
- Finally, if it has an expressive value.
- Type of line (straight or curved).
- Average thickness (five different levels of 0.5, 1.25, 3.1, 7.8, and 19.5 mm) to verify if and what was the visible boundary between the line and the surface; orientation (horizontal, vertical, or two 45-degree diagonals).
- Color (black RGB 0, 0, 0 and white RGB 255, 255, 255 in the first and third experiments; light blue RGB 133, 226, 236, dark blue RGB 10, 36, 82, brown RGB 67, 26, 6 and yellow RGB 255, 206, 14 in the second and fourth experiments). Isoluminant blue and brown were chosen from a perceptual viewpoint.
- Achromatic stimuli are more consistent than chromatic ones, and the thickness and type variables are the most important.
- The color variable is less important, and the orientation variable is generally not important at all.
- Straight lines are mainly evaluated as surfaces and perceived as cold, masculine, sharp, bound, flat, passive, static, acid, and geometrical.
- Curved lines are mainly evaluated as lines and categorized as warm, feminine, sweet, fluffy, dynamic, sensual, rounded, soft, and agitated.
- The boundary between visual lines and surfaces can be measured (in the experiment, it was between 0.5 and 19.5 mm).
9.2. Surfaces
- The metrical bi-dimensionality of shapes does not necessarily coincide with the perceived bi-dimensionality.
- Depth (remoteness) is a mental product that the observer can expand/contract idiosyncratically.
- Seeing and touching surfaces are not totally unrelated perceptions in extended subjective space [253].
9.3. Qualitative Notation
10. Phenomenal Continuum in Awareness
- The demonstration of the different kinds of continuity/discontinuity in (Newtonian) physics and awareness.
- The different qualitative types of phenomenal movements (partial, pure, hindered, etc.).
- The invalidity in the phenomenal field of the strict before-after relationship generally holds for physical facts.
- A series of identifiable specific durations and
- The optimal intervals are required to perceive a fluid continuous movement.
- Avoid starting from abstract definitions.
- Put the immediate stimuli quantification in brackets and enter (and remain) within the domain of qualities and their internal relationships.
- Consider how to ground the structure of the phenomenal continuum in an interwoven series of durations and spatial localizations, where one sequential chain of physical factors can give rise to two or more simultaneous chains of perceptual events.
- Recognize that the phenomenal continuum has different modes, which are strictly dependent on the subjective space–time relationships shaping the phenomena.
11. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Durations Very short (90 ms to 234–252 ms) Short (234–252 ms to 585–630 ms) |
Indeterminate (the most vivid) (585–630 ms) to (1080–1170 ms) (time of contemporaneity) Long (1080–1170 ms to 2070 ms) Very long → 2070 ms. |
Durations 50 ms (successive events cannot be separated, i.e., there is simultaneity) very short (90 to 107 ms) short (234 to 229 ms) (successive events can be separated with difficulty). Subjective attitude: Analysis intermediate (585 to 490 ms) (successive events can be separated with difficulty) |
long (1080 to 1048, 6). Subjective attitude of Focus (major vividness) (time of contemporaneity) very long (2070 to 2244 ms) (successive events can be integrated with difficulty). Subjective attitude: Synthesis extended (4500 to 4802, 2 ms) (successive events are always perceived as separate) 4500 (specious present or STM memory) (James) <12 s. |
Window effect (from Vicario, 2005, 59–62 [129]).
|
Stopping effect (a variant of the Renard effect) (from Vicario, 2005, 57–59 [129]) A small white rectangle starts from a position on the right, moves to pass under a screen, and then manages to return to its initial position. To see the moving rectangle stop, the stop must physically last for at least 200 msec. Stops between 200 and 65 ms offer a vision of the movement in two stages. Stops between 65 and 30 ms offer a vision of uninterrupted but non-linear movement. Stops between 30 and 0 ms are not seen. |
References
- Gibson, J.J. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception; Houghton Mifflin: Boston, MA, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Lappin, J.S. Inferential and ecological theories of perception. In The Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology. Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance; Albertazzi, L., Ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 39–69. [Google Scholar]
- Rock, I. Inference in Perception. In Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association, Towson, MD, USA, 12 July 1982; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1982; pp. 524–540. [Google Scholar]
- Maturana, H.R.; Varela, F.J. Autopoiesis and Cognition. The Realization of the Living; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maturana, H.R.; Varela, F.J. Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding; Shambhala Publications Inc.: Boulder, CO, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Gregory, R. Mind in Science: A History of Explanations in Psychology and Physics; Penguin: London, UK, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Marr, D. Vision; Freeman Press: New York, NY, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Ullmann, S. Tacit assumptions in the computational theory of vision. In Representations of Vision; Gorea, A., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1991; pp. 305–317. [Google Scholar]
- Fodor, J.A. The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Tononi, G.; Boly, M.; Koch, C. Integrated information theory: From consciousness to its physical substrate. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016, 17, 450–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baars, B.J.; Geld, N.; Kozma, R. Global Workspace Theory (GWT) and pre-frontal cortex: Recent developments. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 749868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de-Wit, L.; Thompson, B.; Putzeys, T. Predictive coding and the neural response to predictable stimuli. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 8702–8703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Noë, A. Action in Perception; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- O’Regan, J.K.; Noë, A. A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behav. Brain. Sci. 2001, 24, 939–973, discussion 973–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thompson, E. Sensorimotor subjectivity and the enactive approach to experience. Phenom. Cogn. Sci. 2005, 4, 407–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Northoff, G.; Zilio, F. Temporo-spatial theory of consciousness (TTC)—Bridging the gap of neuronal activity and phenomenal states. Behav. Brain. Res. 2022, 424, 113788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Northoff, G.; Wainio-Theberge, S.; Evers, K. Is temporo-spatial dynamics the “common currency” of brain and mind? In quest of “Spatiotemporal Neuroscience”. Phys. Life Rev. 2020, 33, 34–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Northoff, G.; Wainio-Theberge, S.; Evers, K. Spatiotemporal neuroscience—What is it and why we need it. Phys. Life Rev. 2020, 33, 78–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Libet, B. Mind Time: The Temporal Factor in Consciousness (Perspectives in Cognitive Neuroscience); Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Brentano, F. Psychology from an Empirical Standpoint; Rancurello, A.C.; Terrell, D.B.; McAlister, L.L., Translators; Routledge: London, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Brentano, F. Descriptive Psychology; Müller, B., Ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Husserl, E. Logical Investigations; Moran, D., Ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Brentano, F. The Theory of Categories; Chisholm, R.M., Guterman, N., Eds.; Nijhoff: Den Haag, The Netherlands, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Meinong, A. Über Gegenstände höherer Ordnung und deren Verhältnis zur inneren Wahrnehmung [About objects of a higher order and their relationships to inner perception]. Z. Psychol. Physiol. Sinnesorgane 1899, 21, 182–272. [Google Scholar]
- Twardowski, K. Zur Lehre vom Inhalt and Gegenstand der Vorstellungen: Eine Psychologische Untersuchung [On the Theory of Content and Object of Presentation: A Psychological Research]; Hölder: Wien, Austria, 1894. [Google Scholar]
- Ivry, R.B.; Schlerf, J.E. Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008, 12, 273–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petitot, J.; Varela, F.J.; Pachoud, B.; Roy, J.-M. Beyond the gap: An introduction to naturalizing phenomenology. In Naturalising Phenomenology; Petitot, J., Varela, F.J., Pachoud, B., Roy, J.-M., Eds.; Stanford University Press: Stanford, CA, USA, 1999; pp. 266–329. [Google Scholar]
- Spillmann, L. Phenomenology and neurophysiological correlations: Two approaches to perception research. Vision Res. 2009, 49, 1507–1521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spillmann, L.; Ehrenstein, W. Gestalt factors in the visual neurosciences? In The Visual Neurosciences; Chalupa, L., Werner, J.S., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2004; pp. 428–434. [Google Scholar]
- Albertazzi, L. Naturalizing phenomenology. A must have? Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Albertazzi, L. Experimental phenomenology: What is and what is not. Synthese 2021, 198, 2191–2212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kleinert, A. Paul Weyland, der Berliner Einstein-Töter. In Naturwissenschaft und Technik in der Geschichte. 25 Jahre Lehrstuhl für Geschichte der Naturwissenschaft und Technik am Historischen Institut der Universität Stuttgart; Albrecht, H., Ed.; Verlag für Geschichte der Naturwissenschaften und Technik: Stuttgart, Germany, 1993; pp. 198–232. Available online: https://websrv.physik.uni-halle.de/Fachgruppen/history/weyland.htm (accessed on 2 April 2024).
- Albertazzi, L. Picking up the gauntlet. A reply to Casper and Haueis. Phenom. Cogn. Sci. 2024, 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, S.; Knoll, R. The perception of temporal order: Fundamental issues and a general model. In Attention and Performance; Kornblum, S., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1973; pp. 629–685. [Google Scholar]
- McTaggart, J.M.E. The Nature of Existence; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1927. [Google Scholar]
- Heider, F.; Simmel, M. An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am. J. Psychol. 1944, 57, 243–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kanizsa, G.; Vicario, G.B. La percezione della relazione intenzionale [The perception of the intentional relation]. In Ricerche Sperimentali sulla Percezione [Experimental Researches in Perception]; Kanizsa, G., Vicario, G.B., Eds.; University of Trieste: Trieste, Intaly, 1968; pp. 69–126. [Google Scholar]
- Michotte, A. The Perception of Causality; Methuen: Andover, MA, USA, 1962. [Google Scholar]
- Koffka, K. Zur Grundlegung der Wahrnehmungspsychologie. Eine Auseinandersetzung mit V. Benussi [On the basis of the psychology of perception. An argument with V. Benussi]. Z. Psychol. 1915, 73, 11–90. [Google Scholar]
- Burke, L. On the Tunnell effect. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1952, 4, 121–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerbino, W. Amodal completion revisited. I-Perception 2020, 11, 2041669520937323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kanizsa, G. Linee virtuali e margini fenomenici in assenza di discontinuità di stimolazione [Virtual lines and phenomenal margins in the absence of stimulation discontinuities]. In Atti del X Convegno degli Psicologi Italiani; Lazzeroni, V., Ed.; Florence University Publishing: Florence, Italy, 1954. [Google Scholar]
- Kanizsa, G. Organization in Vision: Essays on Gestalt Perception; Praeger: New York, NY, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Zavagno, D. The perception of light from a phenomenological perspective. Psychol. Conscious. 2021, 8, 346–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Massironi, M. L’osteria Dei Dadi Truccati [The Loaded Dice Tavern]. Bologna, Mimesis. 2013. Massironi, M. Art Works. Available online: https://www.artnet.com/artists/manfredo-massironi/ (accessed on 2 April 2024).
- Kanizsa, G. Vedere e Pensare [Seeing and Thinking]; Il Mulino: Bologna, Italy, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Pinna, B.; Reeves, A. Lighting, backlighting, and watercolor Illusions and the laws of figurality. Spat. Vis. 2006, 19, 341–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- da Pos, O. Phenomenology of perceptual illusions. Psych. Conscious. 2021, 8, 143–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pinna, B. Illusion, and illusoriness: New perceptual issues and new phenomena. In The Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology. Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance; Albertazzi, L., Ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 317–341. [Google Scholar]
- Zavagno, D. lllusion as a cognitive clash rooted in perception. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenderink, J.J. Visual art and visual perception. Perception 2015, 44, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vicario, G.B. Some observations in the auditory field. In Organization and Representation in Perception; Beck, J., Ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1982; pp. 269–283. [Google Scholar]
- Nagel, T. What is it like to be a bat? Philos. Rev. 1974, 83, 435–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Volkelt, H. Grundbegriffe der Ganzheitspsychologie [Basic Concepts of Holistic Psychology]; Beck’sche Verlagsbuchhandlung: Berlin/München, Germany, 1934. [Google Scholar]
- Owen, I.; Morris, N. The Husserlian phenomenology of consciousness and cognitive science: We can see the path, but nobody is on it. J. Consc. Stud. 1999, 6, 269–272. [Google Scholar]
- Varela, F. The Husserlian phenomenology of consciousness and cognitive science: We can see the path, but nobody is on it: Reply to Owen and Morris. J. Conscious. Stud. 1999, 6, 272–273. [Google Scholar]
- Albertazzi, L. The need of an objective phenomenology. Psychol. Conscious 2021, 8, 81–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenderink, J.J. The all-seeing eye? Perception 2014, 43, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoffman, D.D. Public objects and private qualia: The scope and limits of psychophysics. In The Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology. Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance; Albertazzi, L., Ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 71–89. [Google Scholar]
- Panagiotaropoulos, T.I.; Logotetis, N.K. Multistable visual perception as a gateway to neuronal correlates of phenomenal consciousness: The scope and limits of neuroscientific analysis. In The Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology. Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance; Albertazzi, L., Ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 119–143. [Google Scholar]
- Lutz, A.; Thompson, E. Neurophenomenology integrating subjective experience and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. J. Conscious. Stud. 2003, 10, 31–52. [Google Scholar]
- Bitbol, M.; Petitmengin, C. A defense of introspection from within. Constr. Found. 2013, 8, 269–279. [Google Scholar]
- Petitmengin, C. Describing one’s subjective experience in the second person: An interview method for the science of consciousness. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2006, 5, 229–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petitmengin, C.; Remillieux, A.; Valenzuela-Moguillansky, C. Discovering the structures of lived experience: Towards a micro-phenomenological analysis method. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2019, 18, 691–730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallagher, S. Neurophenomenological research on embodied experience. In Essays in Celebration of the Founding of the Organization of Phenomenological Organizations; Cheung, C.-F., Chvatik, I., Copoeru, I., Embree, L., Iribarne, J., Sepp, H.R., Eds.; 2003; Available online: https://www.academia.edu/2827294/Gallagher_S_2002_Neurophenomenological_research_on_embodied_experience (accessed on 2 April 2024).
- Gallagher, S.; Sørensen, J.B. Experimenting with phenomenology. Conscious. Cogn. 2006, 15, 119–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Berkovich-Ohana, A.; Dor-Ziderman, Y.; Trautwein, F.-M.; Schweizer, Y.; Nave, O.; Fulder, S.; Ataria, Y. The Hitchhiker guide to neurophenomenology—The case of studying self-boundaries with meditators. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brown, J.W. The Self-Embodying Mind. Process, Brain Dynamics, and the Conscious Present; Station Hill Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, J.W. Reflections on Mind and the Image of Reality; Wipf and Stock Publishers: Eugene, OR, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Nave, O.; Ataria, Y.; Trautwein, M.; Dor-Ziderman, Y. Self-boundary dissolution in meditation: A phenomenological investigation. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Albertazzi, L.A. Science of qualities. Biol. Theory 2015, 10, 188–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wertheimer, M. Gestalt theory. Soc. Res. 1944, 11, 78–99. [Google Scholar]
- Dietrich, E.; Hardcastel, V.G. Sisyphus’s Boulder: Consciousness and the Limits of the Knowable; Benjamins Publishing House: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Ameseder, R. Über Vorstellungsproduktion [About production of presentations]. In Untersuchungen zur Gegenstandstheorie und Psychologie; Meinong, A., Ed.; Bart: Leipzig, Germany, 1904; pp. 51–120. [Google Scholar]
- Brentano, F. Von der Natur der Vorstellung [On the nature of presentation]. Conceptus 1987, 53/54, 25–33. [Google Scholar]
- Koffka, K. Zur Analyse der Vorstellungen und Ihrer Gesetze. Eine Experimentelle Untersuchung [On the Analysis of Presentations and Their Laws. An Experimental Study]; Quellen & Meyer: Leipzig, Germany, 1912. [Google Scholar]
- Ebbinghaus, H. Grundzüge der Psychologie [Fundamentals of Psychology] I, Bd.2; Veit & Comp.: Leipzig, Germany, 1905. [Google Scholar]
- Maloney, L.T. Statistical decision theory and biological vision. In Perception and the Physical World; Heyer, D.R., Mausfeld, R., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 2002; pp. 145–189. [Google Scholar]
- Albertazzi, L.; van Tonder, G.; Viswanath, D. Perception Beyond Inference. In The Information Content of Visual Processes; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Kersten, D.; Mamassian, P.; Yuille, A. Object perception as Bayesian inference. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2004, 55, 271–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenderink, J.J. Vision and information. In Perception beyond Inference: The Information Content of Visual Processes; Albertazzi, L., van Tonder, G.J., Vishwanath, D., Eds.; Mit Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2010; pp. 27–57. [Google Scholar]
- Koenderink, J.J. Visual Awareness; De Clootcrans Press: Utrecht, The Netherlands, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Bozzi, P. Le condizioni del movimento “naturale” lungo i piani inclinati [The conditions for “natural” motion along inclined planes]. Riv. Psicol. 1959, 53, 337–352. [Google Scholar]
- McCloskey, M. Intuitive physics. Sci. Am. 1983, 248, 122–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hering, E. Outlines of a Theory of the Light Sense; Hurvich, L.M.; Jameson, D., Translators; Harvard University Press: Cambridge MA, USA, 1964. [Google Scholar]
- da Pos, O. Psychophysics and phenomenology of perceptual transparency. Psychol. Conscious. 2023, 10, 17–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Metelli, F. Zur Analyse der phänomenalen Durchsictighkeiterscheinungen [On the analysis of phenomenal transparency phenomena]. In Gestalt und Wirchlichkeit; Mühler, R., Fischl, J., Eds.; Duncker & Humblot: Berlin, Germany, 1967; pp. 50–71. [Google Scholar]
- Metelli, F. The perception of transparency. Sci. Am. 1974, 230, 90–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Metelli, F. Achromatic color conditions in the perception of transparency. In Perception: Essays in Honor of James, J. Gibson; MacLeod, R.B., Pick, H.L., Eds.; Cornell University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Ehrenstein, W. Über Abwandlungen der L. Hermannschen Helligkeitserscheinung [On modifications of L. Hermann’s brightness phenomenon]. Z. Psychol. Organ Dtsch. Ges. Psychol. 1941, 150, 83–91. [Google Scholar]
- Purghé, F.; Coren, S. Amodal completion, depth stratification, and illusory figures: A test of Kanizsa’s explanation. Perception 1992, 21, 325–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Redies, C.; Spillmann, L. The neon color effect in the Ehrenstein illusion. Perception 1982, 10, 667–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- van Tuijl, H.F.J.; Leeuwenberg, E.L.J. Neon color spreading and structural information measures. Percept. Psychophys. 1979, 25, 269–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Varin, D. Fenomeni di contrasto e diffusione cromatica nell’organizzazione spaziale del campo percettivo [Phenomena of contrast and chromatic diffusion in the spatial organization of the perceptual field]. Riv. Psicol. 1971, 65, 102–119. [Google Scholar]
- Pinna, B.; Brelstaff, G.J.; Spillmann, L. Surface color from boundaries: A new ‘watercolor’ illusion. Vision Res. 2001, 41, 2669–2676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gregory, R. Odd Perceptions; Methuen: London, UK, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Grossberg, S.; Mingolla, E. Neural dynamics of perceptual grouping: Textures, boundaries, and emergent segmentations. Percept Psychophys. 1985, 38, 141–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grossberg, S.; Mingolla, E. Neural dynamics of surface perception: Boundary webs, illuminants, and shape from shading. Comput. Gr. Image Process 1987, 37, 116–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pinna, B.; Grossberg, S. The watercolor illusion and neon color spreading: A unified analysis of new cases and neural mechanisms. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2005, 22, 2207–2221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- von der Heydt, R.; Zhou, H.; Friedman, H.S. Neural coding of border ownership: Implications for the theory of figure-ground perception. In Perceptual Organization in Vision: Behavioral and Neural Perspectives; Kimchi, R., Behrmann, M., Olson, C.R., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2003; pp. 281–304. [Google Scholar]
- Koffka, K. Principles of Gestalt Psychology; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 1935. [Google Scholar]
- Kopfermann, H. Psychologische Untersuchungen über die Wirkung zweidimensionaler Darstellungen körperlicher Gebilde [Psychological studies on the effect of two-dimensional representations of physical structures]. Psychol. Forsch. 1930, 13, 293–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wertheimer, M. Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt II. Psychol. Forsch. 1923, 4, 301–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wertheimer, M. Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt, I. Psychol. Forsch. 1922, 4, 47–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mausfeld, R. The attribute of realness and the internal organization. In The Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology. Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance; Albertazzi, L., Ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 91–118. [Google Scholar]
- Michotte, A. Michotte’s Experimental Phenomenology of Perception; Thinès, G., Costall, A., Butterworth, G., Eds.; Psychology Press: London, UK, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Necker, L.A. Observations on some remarkable optical phaenomena seen in Switzerland; and on an optical phaenomenon which occurs on viewing a figure of a crystal or geometrical solid. Lond. Edinb. Philos. Mag. J. Sci. 1832, 1, 329–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Escher, M.C. The Graphic Work of M. C. Escher; Taschen: Cologne, Germany, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Teuber, M.L. Sources of ambiguity in the prints of Maurits, C. Escher. Sci. Am. 1974, 231, 90–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sterzer, P.; Russ, M.O.; Preibisch, C.; Kleinschmidt, A. Neural correlates of spontaneous direction reversals in ambiguous apparent visual motion. Neuroimage 2002, 15, 908–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerschman, S.J.; Vul, E.; Tenenaum, J. Multistability and perceptual inference. Neural Comput. 2012, 24, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kanizsa, G.; Luccio, R. Multistability, in experimental phenomenology as a research tool. In Ambiguity in Mind and Nature; Stadler, M., Kruse, P., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1995; pp. 47–78. [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzkopf, D.; Song, C.; Rees, G. The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size. Nat. Neurosci. 2011, 14, 28–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heelan, P.A. The phenomenological role of consciousness in measurement. Mind Matter 2004, 2, 61–84. [Google Scholar]
- Walsh, R. The methods of reflexivity. Humanist. Psychol. 2003, 31, 51–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bozzi, P. Fenomenologia Sperimentale [Experimental Phenomenology]; Il Mulino: Bologna, Italy, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Goodale, M.A.; Milner, A.D. Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci. 1992, 15, 20–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kanizsa, G. Grammatica del Vedere (Grammar of Seeing); Il Mulino: Bologna, Italy, 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Metzger, W. Laws of Seeing; Spillmann, L.; Lehar, S.; Stromeyer, M.; Wertheimer, M., Translators; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Bergson, H. Time, and Free Will: An Essay on the Immediate Data of Consciousness; Pogson, F.L., Translator; Dover: New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- James, W. Principles of Psychology; Dover: New York, NY, USA, 1950. [Google Scholar]
- Le Poidevin, R. The Images of Time: An Essay on Temporal Representations; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Kitaoka, A. Configurational coincidence among six phenomena: A comment on van Lier and Csathó (2006). Perception 2006, 35, 799–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ouchi, H. Japanese Optical and Geometrical Art; Dover: New York, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Pinna, B.; Spillmann, L. New illusions of sliding motion in depth. Perception 2005, 34, 1441–1458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pinna, B. The illusion of art. In Art and Perception: Towards a Visual Science of Art; Pinna, B., Ed.; Brill: Leiden, The Netherlands, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Metzger, W. Psychologie: Die Entwicklung Ihrer Grundannahmen seit der Einführung des Experiments; Wolfgang Krammer Verlag: Dresden, Germany, 1941. [Google Scholar]
- Musatti, C.L. Sui fenomeni stereocinetici [On stereokinetic phenomena]. Arch. Ital. Psicol. 1924, 9, 61–156. [Google Scholar]
- Vicario, G.B. Il Tempo [Time]; Il Mulino: Bologna, Italy, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Anstis, S.M. The perception of apparent movement. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1980, 290, 153–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wertheimer, M. Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung [Experimental studies on the vision of movement]. Z. Psychol. 1912, 61, 161–265. [Google Scholar]
- Musatti, C.L. Forma e assimilazione [Shape and assimilation]. Arch. Ital. Psicol. 1931, 9, 61–156. [Google Scholar]
- Wertheimer, M. Über das Sehen von Scheinbewegungen und Scheinkorporen [On seeing apparent movements and apparent bodies]. Z. Psychol. 1912, 61, 463–485. [Google Scholar]
- Ternus, J. Experimentelle Untersuchungen über phänomenale Identität [Experimental Researches on phenomenal identity]. Psychol. Forsch. 1926, 7, 81–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Helson, H.; King, S.M. The Tau effect: An example of psychological relativity. J. Exp Psychol. 1931, 14, 202–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abe, S. Experimental study on the co-relation between time, and space. Tohoku Psychol. Folia 1935, 3, 53–68. [Google Scholar]
- Benussi, V. Versuche zur Bestimmung der Gestaltzeit [Experiments to determine gestalt time]. In Bericht über den VI Kongress für Experimentelle Psychologie in Göttingen; Schumann, F., Ed.; Barth: Leipzig, Germany, 1914; pp. 31–35. [Google Scholar]
- Helson, H. The Tau effect. An example of psychological relativity. Science 1930, 71, 536–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Indino, M. Die Subjektive Interferenz von Zeit und Raum. Entwicklungspsychologische Studien zum Tau- und Kappa-Effekt; [The Subjective Interference of Space and Time. Developmental Psychological Studies on the Tau-and Kappa Effect]; University of Zurich, Faculty of Arts: Zurich, Switzerland, 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- von Helmholtz, H. Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik; Voss: Wien, Austria, 1867. [Google Scholar]
- Rock, I. The Logic of Perception; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Gerbino, W. Perception and past experience 50 years after Kanizsa’s (im)possible experiment. Perception 2020, 49, 247–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kanizsa, G. Gestalt theory has been misinterpreted but has also had some real conceptual difficulties. Philos. Psychol. 1994, 7, 149–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albertazzi, L. From Kanizsa back to Benussi: Varieties of intentional reference. Axiomathes 2003, 13, 239–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feldman, J. Probabilistic models of perceptual features. In The Oxford Handbook of Perceptual Organization; Wagemans, J., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015; pp. 934–947. [Google Scholar]
- Rubin, E. Experimenta Psychologica. Collected Scientitific Papers in German, English & French; Ejnar Munksgaard: Copenhagen, Denmark, 1949. [Google Scholar]
- Adelson, E.H. Perceptual organization and the judgment of brightness. Science 1993, 262, 2042–2044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albertazzi, L. Spatial elements in visual awareness. Challenges for an intrinsic “geometry” of the visible. Philos. Sci. 2015, 19, 95–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shapiro, A.G.; Todorović, D. The Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bühler, C. Die Gestalt-Wahrnehmungen [Gestalt Perceptions]; Verlag von W. Spemann: Stuttgart, Germany, 1913. [Google Scholar]
- Koffka, K. Some problems of space perception. In Psychologies of the 1930; Murchinson, C., Ed.; Clark University Press: Worcester, MA, USA, 1930; pp. 531–585. [Google Scholar]
- Rubin, E. Zur Psychophysik der Geradeheit [Psychophysics of the straight line]. Z. Psychol. 1922, 90, 67–105. [Google Scholar]
- Albertazzi, L. The time of presentness. A chapter in positivistic and descriptive psychology. Axiomathes 1999, 10, 49–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benussi, V. Die Psychologie der Zeitauffassung [Psychology of Time Apprehension]; Hölder: Leipzig, Germany, 1913. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, J.W. Microgenesis and Buddhism: The concept of momentariness. Philos. East West 1999, 49, 261–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicario, G.B.; Zambianchi, E. Some experimental observations on instantaneousness and durableness in visual field. Teor. E Model. 1998, 3, 39–57. [Google Scholar]
- Mayzner, M.S. Visual information processing of alphabetic inputs. Psychol. Monogr. Suppl. 1972, 4, 239–243. [Google Scholar]
- Hartmann, L. Beiträge zur Psychologie der Gestalt [Contributions to the psychology of gestalt]. Psychol. Forsch. 1923, 3, 319–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Musatti, C.L. La stereocinesi e il problema della struttura dello spazio visibile [Stereokinesis and the structure of visible space]. Riv. Psicol. 1955, 49, 3–57. [Google Scholar]
- Benussi, V. Introduzione alla Psicologia Sperimentale. Lezioni Tenute nell’anno 1922–23 dal Prof. V. Benussi e Raccolte dal Dott. C.L. Musatti [Introduction to Experimental Psychology. Lectures of the Year 1922–23 Held by Prof. V. Benussi and Recorded by Dr. L.C. Musatti] Typescript; Benussi Educational Fund: Padova, Italy, 1924. [Google Scholar]
- Benussi, V. La Suggestione e L’ipnosi Come Mezzi di Analisi Psichica Reale [Suggestion and Hypnosis as Tools of Real Psychic Analysis]; Zanichelli: Bologna, Italy, 1925. [Google Scholar]
- Albertazzi, L. Stereokinetic shapes and their shadows. Perception 2004, 33, 1437–1452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vezzani, S.; Kramer, P.; Bressan, P. Stereokinetic effect, kinetic depth effect, and structure from motion. In Oxford Handbook of Perceptual; Wagemans, J., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015; pp. 521–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zanforlin, M. The various appearances of a rotating ellipse and the minimum principle: A review and an experimental test with non-ambiguous percepts. Gestalt. Theory 2000, 22, 157–184. [Google Scholar]
- Kanizsa, G.; Luccio, R. Die Doppeldeutigkeiten der Pragnänz [The ambiguity of Pragnänz]. Gestalt. Theory 1986, 8, 99–135. [Google Scholar]
- Ullmann, S. The Interpretation of Visual Motion; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Gregory, R. Perception as hypotheses. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. 1980, 290, 181–197. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, J.F. The visual perception of velocity. Psychol. Forsch. 1931, 14, 199–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, J.F. On time perception in visual movement fields. Psychol. Forsch. 1931, 14, 233–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartmann, N. Zur Grundlegung der Ontologie [On the Foundation of Ontology]; De Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 1935. [Google Scholar]
- Neisser, U. Cognitive Psychology; Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, NY, USA, 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Maruya, A.; Zaidei, Q. Perceptual transitions between object rigidity and non-rigidity: Competition and cooperation among motion energy, feature tracking, and shape-based priors. J. Vis. 2024, 24, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Welchman, A.E. The human brain in depth: How we see in 3D. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 2016, 2, 345–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vasileios, K.; Wang, R.; Shen, Y.; Tino, P.; Williams, G.; Welchmann, A.; Kourtzi, Z. White-matter pathways for statistical learning of temporal structures. ENeuro 2018, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Michon, J.A. The making of the present: A tutorial review. In Attention and Performance VII; Requin, J., Ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1978; pp. 89–111. [Google Scholar]
- Michon, J.A.; Jackson, J.L. Time, Mind, and Behavior; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Rensink, R.A. The dynamic representation of scenes. Vis. Cogn. 2000, 7, 17–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vatakis, A. The role of stimulus properties and cognitive processes in the quality of the multisensory perception of synchrony. In The Wiley- Blackwell Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology. Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance; Albertazzi, L., Ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 243–263. [Google Scholar]
- Vatakis, A.; Papadelis, G. The research on audiovisual perception of temporal order and the processing of musical temporal patterns: Associations, pitfalls, and future directions. In Subjective Time; Arstila, V., Lloyd, D., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014; pp. 409–430. [Google Scholar]
- Pöppel, E. Temporal mechanisms in perception. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 1994, 37, 185–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nani, A.; Manuello, J.; Liloia, D.; Duca, S.; Costa, T.; Cauda, F. The neural correlates of time: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2024, 31, 1796–1826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cheng, R.-K.; MacDonald, C.J.; Meck, W.H. Differential effects of cocaine and ketamine on time estimation: Implications for neurobiological models of interval timing. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2006, 85, 114–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Drew, M.R.; Fairhurst, S.; Malapani, C.; Horvitz, J.C.; Balsam, P.D. Effects of dopamine antagonists on the timing of two intervals. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2003, 75, 9–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holcombe, A. The temporal organization of perception. In The Oxford Handbook of Perceptual Organization; Wagemans, J., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015; pp. 821–839. [Google Scholar]
- Libet, B.; Wright, E.W.; Gleason, C.A. Readiness-potentials preceding unrestricted “spontaneous” vs. pre-planned voluntary acts. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1982, 54, 322–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Libet, B.; Gleason, C.A.; Wright, E.W.; Pearl, D.K. Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 1983, 106, 623–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zeki, S.; Bartels, A. The autonomy of the visual systems and the modularity of conscious vision. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1998, 29, 1911–1914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wittmann, M. Embodied time: The experience of time, the body, and the self. In Subjective Time: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Temporality; Arstila, V., Lloyd, D., Eds.; Boston Review: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014; pp. 507–523. [Google Scholar]
- Callender, C. What Makes Time Special? Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Benussi, V. Versuche zur Analyse taktil erweckter Scheinbewegungen (kinematohaptische Erscheinungen) nach ihren äußeren Bedingungen und ihren Beziehungen zu den parallelen optischen Phänomenen [Attempts to analyze tactilely awakened apparent movements (kinematohaptic phenomena) according to their external conditions and their relationships to the parallel optical phenomena]. Am. J. Psychol. 1917, 36, 59–135. [Google Scholar]
- Bonaventura, E. La percezione del tempo [Time perception]. Cult. Filos. 1913, 7, 518–528. [Google Scholar]
- Calabresi, R. L’analisi del Presente Psichico [The Analysis of the Psychic Present]; Bemporad: Florence, Italy, 1930. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, W. Über psychische Präsenzzeit [On time of presentness]. Z. Psychol. Physiol. S 1897, 13, 325–349. [Google Scholar]
- Fraisse, P. Perception of duration as organization of successive stimuli; experimental demonstration. L’annee Psychol. 1952, 52, 39–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fraisse, P. Processing of successive data in the recognition of identity of names and drawings. L’année Psychol. 1974, 74, 403–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicario, G.B. La dislocazione temporale nella percezione di successione di stimoli discreti [Temporal dislocation in a perception of discrete stimuli]. Riv. Psicol. 1963, 57, 17–87. [Google Scholar]
- Arstila, V.; Lloyd, D. Subjective time: From past to future. In Subjective Time; Arstila, V., Lloyd, D., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014; pp. 309–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dainton, B. The phenomenal continuum. In Subjective Time: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Temporality; Arstila, V., Llyod, D., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pöppel, E.; Bao, Y. Temporal windows as a bridge from objective to subjective time. In Subjective Time: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Temporality; Arstila, V., Lloyd, D., Eds.; Boston Review: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014; pp. 241–261. [Google Scholar]
- Benussi, V. Stroboskopische Scheinbewegungen und geometrisch-optische Gestalttäuschungen [Stroboscopic apparent movements and geometrical-optical gestalt illusions]. Am. J. Psychol. 1912, 24, 31–62. [Google Scholar]
- Vicario, G.B. Valutazione di durate interlacciate [Evaluation of interlaced durations]. Contrib. Dipartimenti Ist. Ital. Psicol. 1992, 5, 97–122. [Google Scholar]
- Benussi, V. Review of Koffka-Kenkel, Beiträge zur Psychologie der Gestalt-und Bewegungserlebnisse. Johann Ambrosius Barth 1912, 32, 50. [Google Scholar]
- Kenkel, F. Untersuchungen über den Zusammenhang zwischen Erscheinungsgrösse und Ercheinungsbewegung bei einigen sogennanten optischen Tӓuschungen [Investigations into the connection between appearence size and apperance movement in some so-called optical illusions]. J. Psychol. 1913, 67, 358–449. [Google Scholar]
- Albertazzi, L. Vittorio Benussi. In The School of Alexius Meinong; Albertazzi, L., Jacquette, D., Poli, R., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2001; pp. 95–133. [Google Scholar]
- Morinaga, S. Some considerations on the explanations of the Mueller-Lyer illusion. J. Psychol. 1941, 16, 26–39. [Google Scholar]
- Rock, I.; Ebenholtz, S. Stroboscopic movement based on change of phenomenal rather than retinal location. Am. J. Psychol. 1962, 75, 193–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hikosaka, O.; Miyauchi, S.; Shimojo, S. Focal visual attention produces illusory temporal order and motion sensation. Vision Res. 1993, 33, 1219–1240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nakayama, K.; Shimojo, S. Experiencing and perceiving visual surfaces. Science 1992, 257, 1357–1363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newman, E.B. Versuche über das Gamma Phӓnomen [Experiments on the Gamma phenomenon]. Psychol. Forsch. 1934, 19, 102–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicario, G.B.; Kiritani, V. Slow-motion Tunnel effect: An inquiry into vertical organization of perceptual events. Gestalt. Theory 1999, 21, 100–121. [Google Scholar]
- Vicario, G.B.; Vidotto, G.; Tomat, L. Valutazione della lunghezza di linee esposte stabilmente o in evoluzione: Dati e problemi. Ric. Psicol. 1974, 18, 81–100. [Google Scholar]
- Vicario, G.B.; Vidotto, G.; Tomat, L. L’errore spaziale e l’errore temporale in eventi stazionari e non stazionari. Ric. Psicol. 1974, 18, 101–118. [Google Scholar]
- Butterworth, G. Phenomenal permanence. In Michotte’s Experimental Phenomenology of Perception; Thinés, G., Costall, A., Butterworth, G., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, Australia, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Michotte, A. A propos de la permanence phénoménale: Faits and théories. Acta Psychol. 1950, 7, 298–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lashley, K. The problem of serial order in behavior. In Cereb Mech Behav; Jeffress, L.A., Ed.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1951; pp. 112–136. [Google Scholar]
- Dresp-Langley, B. The experience-dependent dynamics of human consciousness. Open J. Philos. 2018, 8, 116–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Husserl, E. On the Phenomenology of the Consciousness of Internal Time; Brough, J.B., Ed.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Heron, J.; Aaen-Stockdale, C.; Hotchkiss, J.; Roach, N.W.; McGraw, P.V.; Whitaker, D. Duration channels mediate human time perception. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2012, 279, 690–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Merchant, H.; Harrington, D.L.; Meck, W.H. Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2013, 36, 313–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Härd, A.; Sivik, L. A theory of colors in combination—A descriptive model related to the NCS color-order system. Color Res. Appl. 2000, 26, 4–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Angiulli, A.; Reeves, A. Experimental Phenomenology meets brain information processing: Vividness of voluntary imagery, consciousness of the present, and priming. Psychol. Conscious. 2021, 8, 397–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haustein, S.; Vellino, A.; D’Angiulli, A. Insights from a bibliometric analysis of vividness and its links with consciousness and mental imagery. Brain Sci. 2019, 10, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kant, I. Critique of Pure Reason; Guyer, P., Wood, A.W., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Sutherland, D. Extensive and intensive magnitudes and continuity. In Kant’s Mathematical World. Mathematics, Cognition, and Experience; Sutherland, D., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1991; pp. 87–120. [Google Scholar]
- Meinong, A. Über Begriff und Eigenschaften der Empfindung [About the concept and properties of sensations]. Vierteljahr. Wiss. Philos. 1888, 12, 324–354. [Google Scholar]
- Titchener, E.B. The psychology of feeling and attention. Psychol. Bull. 1908, 5, 404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scharlau, I. Perceptual latency priming: A measure of attentional facilitation. Psychol. Res. 2007, 71, 678–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Santangelo, V.; Spence, C. Crossmodal exogenous orienting improves the accuracy of temporal order judgments. Exp. Brain Res. 2009, 194, 577–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stroud, J.M. The fine structure of psychological time. In Information Theory in Psychology: Problems and Methods; Quastler, H., Ed.; Free Press: Florence, MA, USA, 1956; pp. 174–207. [Google Scholar]
- Wegner, D.M. The Illusion of Conscious Will; MIT Press: Boston, MA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Crick, F.; Koch, C. Consciousness and neuroscience. Cereb. Cortex 1998, 8, 97–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pöppel, E. Pre-semantically defined temporal windows for cognitive processing. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 2009, 364, 1887–1896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nobre, A.C.; van Ede, F. Anticipated moments: Temporal structure in attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2018, 19, 34–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gazzaniga, M.S.; Mangun, G.R. The Cognitive Neurosciences, 5th ed.; Boston Review: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panksepp, J. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Werner, H. On physiognomic perception. In The New Landscape in Art and Science; Kepes, G., Ed.; Paul Theobald and Co: Chicago, IL, USA, 1956. [Google Scholar]
- Gottschaldt, K. Über den Influss der Erfahrung auf die Wahrnehmung von Figuren II [On the influence of experience on the perception of shapes]. Psychol. Forsch. 1929, 12, 1–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Werner, H. Studies on contour: I. Qualitative analyses. Am. J. Psychol. 1935, 47, 40–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kolers, P.A.; Pomerantz, J.R. Figural change in apparent motion. J. Exp. Psychol. 1971, 87, 99–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kolers, P.A. Aspects of Motion Perception; The Pergamon Press: Oxford, UK, 1972. [Google Scholar]
- Barry, S.R. Fixing My Gaze: A Scientist’s Journey into Seeing in Three Dimensions; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Metzger, W. Optische Untersuchungen am Ganzfeld. II. Zur Phänomenologie des homogenen Ganzfelds [Optical research on Ganzfeld. II. On the phenomenology of homogeneous Ganzfeld]. Psychol. Forsch. 1929, 13, 6–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biederman, I. Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychol. Rev. 1987, 94, 115–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pentland, A.P. Perceptual organization and the representation of natural form. Artif. Intelli. 1986, 28, 293–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Singh, M.; Hoffman, D.D. Constructing and representing visual objects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1997, 1, 98–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenderink, J.J. The Way of the Eye; De Clootcrans Press: Utrecht, The Netherlands, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- From Points and Lines, to Surfaces and Volumes. Presented at SEQS 2012, CIMEC, Trento, Italy, 20–22 February 2012. Unpublished Work.
- Kandinsky, V. Punkt und Linie zu Fläche; Albert Lang Verlag: München, Germany, 1926. [Google Scholar]
- Hachen, I.; Albertazzi, L. Anticipation on the boundaries of musical and pictorial continua. In Handbook of Anticipation: Theoretical and Applied Aspects of the Use of Future in Decision Making; Poli, R., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2019; pp. 875–898. [Google Scholar]
- Albertazzi, L.; Canal, L.; Micciolo, R.; Hachen, I. Perceptual organization of visual elements: Lines. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nankoo, J.F.; Madan, C.R.; Spetch, M.L.; Wylie, D.R. Perception of dynamic glass patterns. Vis. Res. 2012, 72, 55–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Roncato, S.; Roncato, F. The veiled statuary: A lesson from sculpture to vision psychology. Art Percept 2019, 7, 1–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenderink, J.J.; Pont, S. The secret of velvety skin. Mach. Vision Appl. 2003, 14, 260–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenderink, J.J. Surface shape, the science, and the look. In The Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology. Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance; Albertazzi, L., Ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 165–180. [Google Scholar]
- Bergström, S.S. The Ambeguias phenomenon and colour constancy. Perception 2004, 33, 831–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenderink, J.J.; Van Doorn, A.J.; Kappers, A.M.L. Pictorial surface attitude and local depth comparisons. Percept. Psychophys. 1996, 58, 163–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- von Hildebrand, A. Das Problem der Form in der Bildenden Kunst [The problem of form in the visual arts]. In Gesammelte Schriften zur Kunst; Bock, H., Ed.; Westdeutscher Verlag: Köln & Opladen, Germany, 1969. [Google Scholar]
- Tononi, G.; Koch, C. Consciousness: Here, there, and everywhere? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2015, 370, 20140167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Renard, L. Le movement de va-et-vien. Travail du Laboratoire des Psychologie expérimentale de Louvain; Unpublished. Summary in Michotte, A., Thinés, G., Crabbé, G., Les complements amodaux des structures perceptives. Beatrice-Nauwelaerts: Paris, France, 1964; 38–42.
- Vicario, G.B. Analisi sperimentale di un caso di movimento apparente [Experimental analysis of a case of apparent movement]. Riv. Psicol. 1964, 58, 133–189. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, L.W. Psychologie der Veränderungsauffassung [Psychology of Change Apprehension]; Preuss und Junger: Breslau, Poland, 1906. [Google Scholar]
- Vicario, G.B.; Zambianchi, E. On continuity perception in brightness change. Perception 1999, 28, 70–71. [Google Scholar]
- Brentano, F. Philosophical Lectures on Space, Time, and the Continuum; Körner, S., Chisholm, R.M., Eds.; Croom Helm: London, UK, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Arnheim, R. Art and Visual Perception: A Psychology of the Creative Eye; University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA, 1954. [Google Scholar]
- Arnheim, R. The Power of the Center: A Study of Composition in the Visual Arts; University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Koffka, K. Problems in the psychology of art. In Bryn Mawr Notes and Monographs, Berneimer, R., Carpenter, R., Koffka, K., Nahm, M.C., Eds.; Sentry Press: New York, NY, USA, 1940; pp. 180–273. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider, A. “Verschmelzung”, tonal fusion, and consonance: Carl Stumpf revisited. In Joint International Conference on Cognitive and Systematic Musicology; Leman, M., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stumpf, C. Tonpsychologie; Book I; S. Hirzel: Leipzig, Germany, 1883. [Google Scholar]
- Stumpf, C. Tonpsychologie; Book II; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Koenderink, J.J.; van Doorn, A.; Pinna, B.; Wagemans, J. Boundaries, transitions and passages. Art. Percept. 2016, 4, 185–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Albertazzi, L. An Unavoidable Mind-Set Reversal: Consciousness in Vision Science. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 735. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070735
Albertazzi L. An Unavoidable Mind-Set Reversal: Consciousness in Vision Science. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(7):735. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070735
Chicago/Turabian StyleAlbertazzi, Liliana. 2024. "An Unavoidable Mind-Set Reversal: Consciousness in Vision Science" Brain Sciences 14, no. 7: 735. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070735
APA StyleAlbertazzi, L. (2024). An Unavoidable Mind-Set Reversal: Consciousness in Vision Science. Brain Sciences, 14(7), 735. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070735