The Sound of Vision Project: On the Feasibility of an Audio-Haptic Representation of the Environment, for the Visually Impaired
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Evidence for Structural and Functional Differences in Auditory and Haptic Perception between Visually Impaired and Sighted People
3. Neural Adaptation
4. Sensory Substitution
5. Training Sound Localization
5.1. Visual Feedback
5.2. Proprioceptive Feedback
5.3. Haptic Feedback
5.4. Auditory Stimulation (i): Free-Field
5.5. Auditory Stimulation (ii): Virtual Auditory Environments
5.6. Head Dynamics
5.7. Auditory Stimuli
5.8. The Effects of the Training Methods
6. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
ABM | Auditory backward masking |
BCI | Brain-computer interfacing |
BOLD | Blood oxygen level-dependent |
CB | Congenitally blind people |
EB | People that became blind in their first 2 years of life |
ECoG | EEG with implanted electrodes |
EEG | Electroencephalogram |
FEF | Frontal eye field |
fMRI | Functional magnetic resonance imaging |
HRTF | Head Related Transfer Function |
ISI | Inter stimuli interval |
JND | Just noticeable difference |
LB | Late blind people |
LED | Light-emitting diode |
LOC | Lateral occipital complex |
N1 | Negative-going evoked potential appearing about 100 ms after stimulus presentation |
MRI | Magnetic resonance imaging |
PET | Positron emission tomography |
RT | Response time |
rTMS | Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation |
SB | Sighted blindfolded |
SC | Sighted comparison group |
SOA | Stimulus onset asynchrony |
TOJ | Temporal order judgement |
VIP | Visually impaired people |
References
- Sound of Vision. Available online: http://www.soundofvision.net/ (accessed on 24 June 2016).
- Cole, M.; Cole, S.; Lightfoot, C. The Development of Children, 5th ed.; Worth Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Croteau-Chonka, E.C.; Dean, D.C., III; Remer, J.; Dirks, H.; O’Muircheartaigh, J.; Deoni, S.C.L. Examining the relationships between cortical maturation and white matter myelination throughout early childhood. NeuroImage 2016, 125, 413–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Holland, D.; Chang, L.; Ernst, T.M.; Curran, M.; Buchthal, S.D.; Alicata, D.; Skranes, J.; Johansen, H.; Hernandez, A.; Yamakawa, R.; et al. Structural growth trajectories and rates of change in the first 3 months of infant brain development. JAMA Neurol. 2014, 71, 1266–1269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kolb, B.; Mychasiuk, R.; Muhammad, A.; Li, Y.; Frost, D.O.; Gibb, R. Experience and the developing prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 17186–17193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Petanjek, Z.; Judaš, M.; Šimić, G.; Rašin, M.R.; Uylings, H.B.M.; Rakic, P.; Kostović, I. Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 13281–13286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blakemore, C.; Cooper, G.F. Development of the brain depends on the visual environment. Nature 1970, 228, 477–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wiesel, T.N.; Hubel, D.H. Single-cell responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye. J. Neurophysiol. 1963, 26, 1003–1017. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- May, A. Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the adult human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 475–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ostrovsky, Y.; Andalman, A.; Sinha, P. Vision following extended congenital blindness. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 1009–1014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sinha, P.; Held, R. Sight restoration. F1000 Med. Rep. 2012, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Elli, G.V.; Benetti, S.; Collignon, O. Is there a future for sensory substitution outside academic laboratories? Multisens. Res. 2014, 27, 271–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Park, H.-J.; Lee, J.D.; Kim, E.Y.; Park, B.; Oh, M.-K.; Lee, S.; Kim, J.-J. Morphological alterations in the congenital blind based on the analysis of cortical thickness and surface area. Neuro Image 2009, 47, 98–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stevens, A.A.; Weaver, K. Auditory perceptual consolidation in early-onset blindness. Neuropsychologia 2005, 43, 1901–1910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Després, O.; Candas, V.; Dufour, A. Spatial auditory compensation in early-blind humans: Involvement of eye movements and/or attention orienting? Neuropsychologia 2005, 43, 1955–1962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Röder, B.; Teder-Sälejärvi, W.; Sterr, A.; Rösler, F.; Hillyard, S.A.; Neville, H.J. Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans. Nature 1999, 400, 162–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Voss, P.; Tabry, V.; Tabry, V.; Zatorre, R.J. Trade-off in the sound localization abilities of early blind individuals between the horizontal and vertical planes. J. Neurosci. 2015, 35, 6051–6056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Finocchietti, S.; Cappagli, G.; Gori, M. Encoding audio motion: Spatial impairment in early blind individuals. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1309–1306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fieger, A.; Röder, B.; Teder-Sälejärvi, W.; Hillyard, S.A.; Neville, H.J. Auditory spatial tuning in late-onset blindness in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006, 18, 149–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lerens, E.; Renier, L. Does visual experience influence the spatial distribution of auditory attention? Acta Psychol. 2014, 146, 58–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lessard, N.; Pare, M.; Lepore, F.; Lassonde, M. Early-Blind human subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects. Nature 1998, 395, 278–280. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Voss, P.; Lassonde, M.; Gougoux, F.; Fortin, M.; Guillemot, J.P.; Lepore, F. Early- and late-onset blind individuals show supra-normal auditory abilities in far-space. Curr. Biol. 2004, 14, 1734–1738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Voss, P.; Gougoux, F.; Zatorre, R.J.; Lassonde, M.; Lepore, F. Differential occipital responses in early- and late-blind individuals during a sound-source discrimination task. Neuroimage 2008, 40, 746–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gori, M.; Sandini, G.; Martinoli, C.; Burr, D.C. Impairment of auditory spatial localization in congenitally blind human subjects. Brain 2014, 137, 288–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Röder, B.; Rösler, F.; Neville, H.J. Effects of interstimulus interval on auditory event-related potentials in congenitally blind and normally sighted humans. Neurosci. Lett. 1999, 264, 53–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Occelli, V.; Spence, C.; Zampini, M. Audiotactile temporal order judgments in sighted and blind individuals. Neuropsychologia 2008, 46, 2845–2850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Röder, B.; Rösler, F.; Spence, C. Early vision impairs tactile perception in the blind. Curr. Biol. 2004, 14, 121–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hötting, K.; Röder, B. Hearing cheats touch, but less in congenitally blind than in sighted individuals. Psychol. Sci. 2004, 15, 60–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hötting, K.; Rösler, F.; Röder, B. Crossmodal and intermodal attention modulate event-related brain potentials to tactile and auditory stimuli. Exp. Brain Res. 2003, 148, 26–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hötting, K.; Rösler, F.; Röder, B. Altered auditory-tactile interactions in congenitally blind humans: An event-related potential study. Exp. Brain Res. 2004, 159, 370–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wan, C.Y.; Wood, A.G.; Reutens, D.C.; Wilson, S.J. Early but not late-blindness leads to enhanced auditory perception. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 344–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Postma, A.; Zuidhoek, S.; Noordzij, M.L.; Kappers, A.M.L. Differences between early-blind, late-blind, and blindfolded-sighted people in haptic spatial-configuration learning and resulting memory traces. Perception 2007, 36, 1253–1265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Withagen, A.; Kappers, A.M.L.; Vervloed, M.P.J.; Knoors, H.; Verhoeven, L. Haptic object matching by blind and sighted adults and children. Acta Psychol. 2012, 139, 261–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goldreich, D.; Kanics, I.M. Tactile acuity is enhanced in blindness. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 3439–3445. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Maljkovic, V.; Nakayama, K. Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features. Mem. Cogn. 1994, 22, 657–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kristjansson, A.; Jóhannesson, Ó.I. How priming in visual search affects response time distributions: Analyses with ex-Gaussian fits. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2014, 76, 2199–2211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ásgeirsson, Á.G.; Kristjánsson, Á.; Bundesen, C. Independent priming of location and color in identification of briefly presented letters. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2014, 76, 40–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dehaene, S.; Naccache, L.; Cohen, L.; Le Bihan, D.; Mangin, J.F.; Poline, J.B.; Rivière, D. Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nat. Neurosci. 2001, 4, 752–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kristjansson, A.; Vuilleumier, P.; Schwartz, S.; Macaluso, E.; Driver, J. Neural basis for priming of pop-out during visual search revealed with fMRI. Cere. Cortex 2007, 17, 1612–1624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grill-Spector, K.; Malach, R. fMR-adaptation: A tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. Acta Psychol. 2001, 107, 293–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kourtzi, Z.; Kanwisher, N. Representation of perceived object shape by the human lateral occipital complex. Science 2001, 293, 1506–1509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Larsson, J.; Smith, A.T. fMRI repetition suppression: Neuronal adaptation or stimulus expectation? Cereb. Cortex 2012, 22, 567–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Krekelberg, B.; Boynton, G.M.; van Wezel, R.J. Adaptation: From single cells to BOLD signals. Trends Neurosci. 2006, 29, 250–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kovács, G.; Zimmer, M.; Harza, I.; Vidnyánszky, Z. Adaptation duration affects the spatial selectivity of facial aftereffects. Vis. Res. 2007, 47, 3141–3149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vizioli, L.; Rousselet, G.A.; Caldara, R.; Hari, R. Neural repetition suppression to identity is abolished by other-race faces. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2010, 107, 20081–20086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rentzeperis, I.; Nikolaev, A.R.; Kiper, D.C.; van Leeuwen, C. Relationship between neural response and adaptation selectivity to form and color: An ERP study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rossion, B.; Boremanse, A. Robust sensitivity to facial identity in the right human occipito-temporal cortex as revealed by steady-state visual-evoked potentials. J. Vis. 2011, 11, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gerlicher, A.M.V.; van Loon, A.M.; Scholte, H.S.; Lamme, V.A.F.; van der Leij, A.R. Emotional facial expressions reduce neural adaptation to face identity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013, 9, 610–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dobbins, I.G.; Schnyer, D.M.; Verfaellie, M.; Schacter, D.L. Cortical activity reductions during repetition priming can result from rapid response learning. Nature 2004, 428, 316–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wander, J.D.; Blakely, T.; Miller, K.J.; Weaver, K.E.; Johnson, L.A.; Olson, J.D.; Ojemann, J.G. Distributed cortical adaptation during learning of a brain–computer interface task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 10818–10823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bichot, N.P.; Schall, J.D. Effects of similarity and history on neural mechanisms of visual selection. Nat. Neurosci. 1999, 2, 549–554. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Bichot, N.P.; Schall, J.D. Priming in Macaque frontal cortex during popout visual search: Feature-based facilitation and location-based inhibition of return. J. Neurosci. 2002, 22, 4675–4685. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- De Volder, A.G.; Bol, A.; Blin, J.; Robert, A.; Arno, P.; Grandin, C.; Michel, C.; Veraart, C. Brain energy metabolism in early blind subjects: Neural activity in the visual cortex. Brain Res. 1997, 750, 235–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bach-y-Rita, P.; Kercel, S.W. Sensory substitution and the human-machine interface. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 541–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sadato, N.; Pascual-Leone, A.; Grafmani, J.; Ibañez, V.; Deiber, M.-P.; Dold, G.; Hallett, M. Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects. Nature 1996, 380, 526–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Volder, A.G.; Catalan-Ahumada, M.; Robert, A.; Bol, A.; Labar, D.; Coppens, A.; Michel, C.; Veraart, C. Changes in occipital cortex activity in early blind humans using a sensory substitution device. Brain Res. 1999, 826, 128–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Collignon, O.; Lassonde, M.; Lepore, F.; Bastien, D.; Veraart, C. Functional cerebral reorganization for auditory spatial processing and auditory substitution of vision in early blind subjects. Cere. Cortex 2006, 17, 457–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amedi, A.; Raz, N.; Azulay, H.; Malach, R.; Zohary, E. Cortical activity during tactile exploration of objects in blind and sighted humans. Restor. Neurolo. Neurosci. 2010, 28, 143–156. [Google Scholar]
- Striem-Amit, E.; Amedi, A. Visual cortex extrastriate body-selective area activation in congenitally blind people “seeing” by using sounds. Curr. Biol. 2014, 24, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maidenbaum, S.; Abboud, S.; Amedi, A. Sensory substitution: Closing the gap between basic research and widespread practical visual rehabilitation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 41, 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bauer, R.W.; Matuzsa, J.L.; Blackmer, R.F.; Glucksberg, S. Noise localization after unilateral attenuation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1966, 40, 441–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Florentine, M. Relation between lateralization and loudness in asymmetrical hearing losses. J. Am. Audiol. Soc. 1976, 1, 243–251. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Musicant, A.D.; Butler, R.A. Monaural localization: An analysis of practice effects. Percept. Psychophys. 1980, 28, 236–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Slattery, W.H.; Middlebrooks, J.C. Monaural sound localization: Acute verses chronic unilateral impairment. Hear. Res. 1994, 75, 38–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kumpik, D.P.; Kacelnik, O.; King, A.J. Adaptive reweighting of auditory localization cues in response to chronic unilateral ear plugging in humans. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 4883–4894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Held, R. Shifts in binaural localization after prolonged exposures to a typical combinations of stimuli. Am. J. Psychol. 1955, 68, 526–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hofman, P.M.; van Riswick, J.G.A.; van Opstal, A.J. Relearning sound localization with new ears. Nat. Neurosci. 1998, 1, 417–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carlile, S.; Blackman, T. Relearning auditory spectral cues for locations inside and outside the visual field. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 2014, 15, 249–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carlile, S. The plastic ear and perceptual relearning in auditory spatial perception. Front Neurosci 2014, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zahorik, P.; Bangayan, P.; Sundareswaran, V.; Wang, K.; Tam, C. Perceptual recalibration in human sound localization: Learning to remediate front-back reversals. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2006, 120, 343–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strelnikov, K.; Rosito, M.; Barone, P. Effect of audiovisual training on monaural spatial hearing in horizontal plane. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e18344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Honda, A.; Shibata, H.; Hidaka, S.; Gyoba, J.; Iwaya, Y.; Suzuki, Y. Effects of head movement and proprioceptive feedback in training of sound localization. Iperception 2013, 4, 253–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bălan, O.; Moldoveanu, A.; Nagy, H.; Wersényi, G.; Botezatu, N.; Stan, A.; Lupu, R.G. Haptic-auditory perceptual feedback based training for improving the spatial acoustic resolution of the visually impaired people. In Proceedings of the 21st International Conference on Auditory Display (ICAD-2015), Graz, Austria, 8–10 July 2015.
- Parseihian, G.; Katz, B.F.G. Rapid head-related transfer function adaptation using a virtual auditory environment. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2012, 131, 2948–2957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mendonça, C.; Campos, G.; Dias, P.; Santos, J.A. Learning auditory space: Generalization and long-term effects. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e77900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Muchnik, C.; Efrati, M.; Nemeth, E.; Malin, M.; Hildesheimer, M. Central auditory skills in blind and sighted subjects. Scand. Audiol. 1991, 20, 19–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- King, A.J. Crossmodal plasticity and hearing capabilities following blindness. Cell Tissue Res. 2015, 361, 295–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- King, A.J. What happens to your hearing if you are born blind? Brain 2014, 137, 6–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kristjánsson, T.; Thorvaldsson, T.P.; Kristjánsson, Á. Divided multimodal attention: Sensory trace and context coding strategies in spatially congruent auditory and visual presentation. Multisens. Res. 2014, 27, 91–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spence, C.; Driver, J. On measuring selective attention to an expected sensory modality. Percept. Psychophys. 1997, 59, 389–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boven, R.W.V.; Hamilton, R.H.; Kauffman, T.; Keenan, J.P.; Pascual-Leone, A. Tactile spatial resolution in blind Braille readers. Neurology 2000, 54, 2230–2236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kohn, A. Visual adaptation: Physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. J. Neurophysiol. 2007, 97, 3155–3164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grill-Spector, K.; Henson, R.; Martin, A. Repetition and the brain: Neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 14–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Geng, J.J.; Eger, E.; Ruff, C.C.; Kristjansson, A.; Rotshtein, P.; Driver, J. On-line attentional selection from competing stimuli in opposite visual fields: Effects on human visual cortex and control processes. J. Neurol. Neurophysiol. 2006, 96, 2601–2612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rorden, C.; Kristjánsson, Á.; Revill, K.P.; Saevarsson, S. Neural correlates of inter-trial priming and role-reversal in visual search. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Clayson, P.E.; Larson, M.J. Psychometric properties of conflict monitoring and conflict adaptation indices: Response time and conflict N2 event-related potentials. Psychophysiology 2013, 50, 1209–1219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McGraw, P.V.; Badcock, D.R.; Khuu, S. Steady viewing dissipates global structure. Perception 2004, 33, 121–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sigurjónsdóttir, Ó.; Sigurðardóttir, S.; Björnsson, A.S.; Kristjánsson, Á. Barking up the wrong tree in attentional bias modification? Comparing the sensitivity of four tasks to attentional biases. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry. 2015, 48, 9–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hauptmann, B.; Karni, A. From primed to learn: The saturation of repetition priming and the induction of long-term memory. Cogn. Brain Res. 2002, 13, 313–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horner, A.J.; Henson, R.N. Priming, response learning and repetition suppression. Neuropsychologia 2008, 46, 1979–1991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vuilleumier, P.; Henson, R.N.; Driver, J.; Dolan, R.J. Multiple levels of visual object constancy revealed by event-related fMRI of repetition priming. Nat. Neurosci. 2002, 5, 491–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bach-y-Rita, P.; Collins, C.C.; Saunders, F.A.; White, B.; Scadden, L. Vision substitution by tactile image projection. Nature 1969, 221, 963–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bach-y-Rita, P. Brain Mechanisms in Sensory Substitution; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1972. [Google Scholar]
- Amedi, A.; Malach, R.; Hendler, T.; Peled, S.; Zohary, E. Visuo-haptic object-related activation in the ventral visual pathway. Nat. Neurosci. 2001, 4, 324–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amedi, A.; Jacobson, G.; Hendler, T.; Malach, R.; Zohary, E. Convergence of visual and tactile shape processing in the human lateral occipital complex. Cere. Cortex 2002, 12, 1202–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kujala, T.; Huotilainen, M.; Sinkkonen, J.; Ahonen, A.I.; Alho, K.; Hämäläinen, M.S.H.; Ilmoniemi, R.J.; Kajola, M.; Knuutila, J.E.T.; Lavikainen, J.; et al. Visual cortex activation in blind humans during sound discrimination. Neurosci. Lett. 1995, 183, 143–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weeks, R.; Horwitz, B.; Aziz-Sultan, A.; Tian, B.; Wessinger, C.M.; Cohen, L.G.; Hallett, M.; Rauschecker, J.P. A positron emission tomographic study of auditory localization in the congenitally blind. J. Neurosci. 2000, 20, 2664–2672. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Kupers, R.; Ptito, M. Compensatory plasticity and cross-modal reorganization following early visual deprivation. Neurosci. Biobehav. R. 2014, 41, 36–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Loomis, J.M.; Klatzky, R.L.; Giudice, N.A. Sensory Substitution of Vision: Importance of Perceptual and Cognitive Processing; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2012; pp. 162–191. [Google Scholar]
- Dellepiane, M.; Pietroni, N.; Tsingos, N.; Asselot, M.; Scopigno, R. Reconstructing head models from photographs for individualized 3D-audio processing. Comput. Graph. Forum 2008, 27, 1719–1727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meshram, A.; Mehra, R.; Yang, H.; Dunn, E.; Frahm, J.M.; Manocha, D. P-HRTF: Efficient Personalized HRTF Computation for High-Fidelity Spatial Sound. In Proceeding of International Symposium on Mixed and Augmented Reality (ISMAR), Munich, Germany, 10–12 September 2014.
- Mendonça, C. A review on auditory space adaptations to altered head-related cues. Front. Neurosci 2014, 8, 219. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Van Wanrooij, M.M.; van Opstal, A.J. Relearning sound localization with a new ear. J. Neurosci. 2005, 25, 5413–5424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Wanrooij, M.M.; van Opstal, A.J. Sound localization under perturbed binaural hearing. J. Neurophysiol. 2007, 97, 715–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bălan, O.; Moldoveanu, A.; Moldoveanu, F.; Negoi, I. The Role of Perceptual Feedback Training on Sound Localization Accuracy in Audio Experiments. In Proceedings of the 11th International Scientific Conference eLearning and Software for Education, Bucharest, Romania, 23–24 April 2015.
- MIT HRTF Database. Available online: http://sound.media.mit.edu/resources/KEMAR.html (accessed on 7 October 2015).
- Majdak, P.; Goupell, M.J.; Laback, B. 3-D localization of virtual sound sources: Effects of visual environment, pointing method, and training. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2010, 72, 454–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blum, A.; Katz, B.F.G.; Warusfel, O. Eliciting Adaptation to Non-Individual HRTF Spectral Cues with Multi-Modal Training. In Proceedings of the Joint Meeting of the German and the French Acoustical Societies (CFA/DAGA 04), Strasboug, France, 22–25 March 2004.
- Shinn-Cunningham, B.; Streeter, T.; Gyss, J.F. Perceptual Plasticity in Spatial Auditory Displays. In Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Audio display, CD-Rom, Espoo, Finland, 29 July–1 August 2001.
- Woodworth, R.S.; Schlosberg, H. Experimental Psychology; Rinehart and Winston: New York, NY, USA, 1954. [Google Scholar]
- Wightman, F.L.; Kistler, D.J. Resolution of front-back ambiguity in spatial hearing by listener and source movement. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1999, 105, 2841–2853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Paper | Main Results | Method |
---|---|---|
Park et al. [13] | Differences in cortical thickness, volume and area between VIPs and SCs. | MRI |
Stevens and Weaver [14] | CBs show better TOJ and ABM performance than SCs. | Behavioral |
Després et al. [15] | CBs are faster than SCs at locating a sound source from behind them in the vertical plain but not straight ahead. No significant differences in the horizontal plane. | Behavioral |
Röder et al. [16] | In the peripheral condition N1 amplitude decreased faster for CBs than SCs indicating that CBs process peripheral auditory stimuli more efficiently than sighted people, but sighted people respond more accurately to central auditory stimuli. | Behavioral and EEG |
Voss et al. [17] | No differnece between the performance of EBs and SCs when binaurally locating sound sources in the horizontal plane but a EB-subgroup performed worse in the vertical plane. | Behavioral |
Finocdhietti et al. [18] | VIPs and SCs show similar performance in detecting the direction of a moving sound, but the performance of EBs in locating the end point of the moving sound is worse than for both LBs and SCs. | Behavioral |
Fieger et al. [19] | SCs were slower in responding to peripheral than to central auditory stimuli. LBs were slower than the SCs but equally fast in responding to peripheral and central stimuli. | Behavioral |
Fieger et al. [19] | CBs and LBs process peripheral auditory stimuli more efficiently than sighted people. | EEG |
Lerens and Renier [20] | EBs respond faster to both central and peripheral auditory stimuli than Scs | Behavioral |
Lessard et al. [21] | Found no differences in sound localization performance between congenital blind participants and sighted participants but the performance of their participants that had some residual sight was inferior compared to the other groups | Behavioral |
Voss et al. [22,23] | No differences found between VIPs and SC, neither in locating sound sorces or comparing sound soureces locations | Behavioral |
Gori et al. [24] | When judging whether the left or the right sound were closer to a central sound the CBs performed at chance level while the SCs solved the task adequately. | Behavioral |
Gori et al. [24] | When comparing the distance of the first and third sound to the middle sound and when judging whether the sound was to the left or right of the base sound the performance of the CBs and SCs did not differ. | Behavioral |
Röder et al. [25] | CBs responded significantly faster to acoustic stimuli than SCs. | Behavioral and EEG |
Occelli et al. [26] | CBs showed lower JNDs than SCs in judging whether an auditory or tactile stimulus appeared first when stimuli appeared at different locations. | Behavioral |
Röder et al. [27] | Better TOJ for CBs than LBs and SCs when comparing tactile stimuli. | Behavioral |
Hötting and Röder [28] | When 3 or 4 tones are presented along with haptic stimuli the tones influence the performance less for CBs than SCs. | Behavioral |
Hötting et al. [29,30] | CBs responded significantly faster to tactile stimuli than SCs but the difference was not significant for auditory stimuli. | Behavioral and EEG |
Hötting et al. [29,30] | The initial selection process of simultaneously presented haptic and auditory stimuli may involve similar brain areas in CBs and SCs. The selection mechanism might however differ when stimuli have to be filtered by two factors (location and modality). | Behavioral and EEG |
Wan et al. [31] | CBs and EBs, but not LBs, show better pitch and timbre discrimination than matched SCs. | Behavioral |
Postma et al. [32] | Both EBs and LBs were faster than sighted blindfolded people (SB) in putting geometric shapes in their correct places on a wooden board. After rotating the board 90° the VIPs were still faster than the Sbs | Behavioral |
Withagen et al. [33] | VIPs faster than SCs in haptically comparing artificial objects and telling whether they were the same or not. | Behavioral |
Goldreich and Kanics [34] | VIPs show better tactile acuity than SCs. | Behavioral |
Paper | Main Results | Method |
---|---|---|
Maljkovic and Nakayama [35]; Kristjánsson and Jóhannesson [36]. | Repeated presentation of stimulus properties, e.g., color and shape reduces RTs. | Behavioral |
Maljkovic and Nakayama [35]; Ásgeirssson et al. [37]. | Repeated presentation of the stimuli at the same location reduces RTs. | Behavioral |
Dehane et al. [38]; Kristjansson et al. [39], Grill-Spector and Malach [40]; Kourtzi and Kanwisher [41]; Larsson and Smith [42]. | When particular stimuli repeat, this results in suppression of the BOLD signal as measured with fMRI. | fMRI |
Krekelberg, Boynton and van Wezel [43]. | Repetition suppression may cause selective “pruning” of the neuronal population response in a given situation. | Review on fMRI |
Kovács et al. [44]; Vizioli et al. [45] | Adaptation, in general, reduces EEG signals | EEG |
Rentzeperis et al. [46] | When stimulus’ color was repeated the amplitude of the N1 component became significantly lower than when color did not repeat. | EEG |
Rossion and Boremanse [47] | When the same face was repeatedly presented, amplitude measured over the occipito-temporal scalp decreased significantly compared to when the face on the current trial differed from the previous one. | EEG |
Gerlicher et al. [48] | Adaptation occurs for repeated faces with neutral emotional expressions but not threatening expressions. | EEG |
Dobbins et al. [49] | RTs are faster for previously presented, versus novel stimuli, in conjunction with BOLD repetition suppression in a number of brain regions. | Behavioral and fMRI |
Wander et al. [50] | When moving a cursor over the screen through BCI, activity as measured with ECoG (implanted electrodes) in a number of brain areas decreased as proficiency on the task increased | BCI ana ECoG |
Bichot and Schall [51,52] | Single cell recording in frontal eyefields of Macaque monkeys showed decreased neuronal activity when stimulus properties were repeated | Single cell recording |
Paper | Main Results | Method |
---|---|---|
De Volder et al. [53] | The metabolic rate of the visual cortex in the congenitally blind is as high as in sighted people indicating that it’s neurons are still active. | PET and MRI |
Bach-y-Rita and Karcel [54] | Sensory substitution may offer the possibility of regaining lost perceptual abilities through the process of plasticity. | Review on sensory substitution |
Sadato et al. [55] | Braille reading activated the primary and secondary visual cortex in blind people. | PET |
De Volder et al. [56] | When an ultrasonic echolocation device was used to locate obstacles PET studies revealed that the metabolic rate in the primary and associate visual cortex of EBs was higher than for SCs. | PET |
Collignon et al. [57] | rTMS of visual cortex interfered with auditory tasks performed by blind subjects but not for blindfolded sighted participants. Shows that the visual cortex of EBs is involved in auditory discrimination. | rTMS |
Amedi et al. [58] | LOC of blind individuals was activated in the same way as the sighted controls during an object-localization task. | fMRI |
Striem-Amit and Amedi [59] | When using a visual-to-auditory sensory-substitution device to recognize body shapes and body posture fMRI revealed that the right extrastriate visual cortex was highly activated during the task in both CBs and SCs. | fMRI |
Maidenbaum et al. [60] | The brain is task-oriented rather than sensory-specific, supporting the “task-machine” brain hypothesis. | Review on sensory substituion |
Paper | Main Results |
---|---|
Bauer et al. [61]; Florentine [62]; Musicant and Butler [63]; Slattery and Middlebrooks [64]; Kumpik et al. [65]. | Low initial sound localization accuracy, but significant improvements after a period of several days. |
Held [66] | Displacement of auditory representation halfway in the direction of the sound source’s azimuth shift. Localization accuracy improved after a few days. |
Hofman et al. [67]; Carlile and Blackman [68] | Immediately after mold insertion, localization was severely impaired, but after several days performance improved gradually, reaching pre-training accuracy levels when the molds were removed. |
Carlile [69]; Zahorik et al. [70]; Strelnikov et al. [71]. | Improvements in sound localization, especially decreased front-back confusion. |
Honda et al. [72]. | Decreases in sound localization errors in the horizontal and in the vertical plane, regardless of head movement |
Bălan et al. [73] | Decreases in sound localization error and front-back confusion rate for broadband and narrowband sounds. |
Parseihian and Katz [74] | Reduced angular precision errors and front-back confusion |
Mendonça et al. [75] | Sound localization performance improved for all participants |
© 2016 by the authors; licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Jóhannesson, Ó.I.; Balan, O.; Unnthorsson, R.; Moldoveanu, A.; Kristjánsson, Á. The Sound of Vision Project: On the Feasibility of an Audio-Haptic Representation of the Environment, for the Visually Impaired. Brain Sci. 2016, 6, 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci6030020
Jóhannesson ÓI, Balan O, Unnthorsson R, Moldoveanu A, Kristjánsson Á. The Sound of Vision Project: On the Feasibility of an Audio-Haptic Representation of the Environment, for the Visually Impaired. Brain Sciences. 2016; 6(3):20. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci6030020
Chicago/Turabian StyleJóhannesson, Ómar I., Oana Balan, Runar Unnthorsson, Alin Moldoveanu, and Árni Kristjánsson. 2016. "The Sound of Vision Project: On the Feasibility of an Audio-Haptic Representation of the Environment, for the Visually Impaired" Brain Sciences 6, no. 3: 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci6030020
APA StyleJóhannesson, Ó. I., Balan, O., Unnthorsson, R., Moldoveanu, A., & Kristjánsson, Á. (2016). The Sound of Vision Project: On the Feasibility of an Audio-Haptic Representation of the Environment, for the Visually Impaired. Brain Sciences, 6(3), 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci6030020