Next Article in Journal
Neurodegeneration and Sensorimotor Deficits in the Mouse Model of Traumatic Brain Injury
Next Article in Special Issue
The GABAA Receptor α2 Subunit Activates a Neuronal TLR4 Signal in the Ventral Tegmental Area that Regulates Alcohol and Nicotine Abuse
Previous Article in Journal
Local Choices: Rationality and the Contextuality of Decision-Making
Previous Article in Special Issue
Preliminary Findings that a Targeted Intervention Leads to Altered Brain Function in Children with Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Behavioral and Brain Activity Indices of Cognitive Control Deficits in Binge Drinkers

1
Spatio-Temporal Brain Imaging Lab, Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA 92120, USA
2
Department of Radiology, University of California, San Diego, CA 92039, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2018, 8(1), 9; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8010009
Submission received: 6 November 2017 / Revised: 16 December 2017 / Accepted: 4 January 2018 / Published: 4 January 2018
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Alcohol Induced Central Nervous System Damage)

Abstract

:
Heavy episodic drinking is prevalent among young adults and is a public issue of increasing importance. Its initiation and maintenance are associated with deficits in the capacity to inhibit automatic processing in favor of non-habitual responses. This study used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine behavioral and brain activity indices of cognitive control during the Stroop task as a function of binge drinking. Heavy episodic drinkers (HED) reported consuming 5+/6+ drinks in two hours at least five times in the past six months and were compared to light drinkers (LED) who reported two or fewer binge episodes but were matched on demographics, intelligence and family history of alcoholism. Greater conflict-induced activity in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) and thalamus was observed in HED participants and it was positively correlated with alcohol intake and alcohol-related harmful consequences. HEDs maintained intact accuracy but at a cost of prolonged reaction times to high-conflict trials and increased ratings of task difficulty. Greater activation of the areas implicated in cognitive control is consistent with compensatory network expansion to meet higher cognitive demands. These results provide further insight into degradation of cognitive control in HEDs which may benefit development of detection and prevention strategies.

1. Introduction

Cognitive control is a facet of executive functioning that underlies optimization of behavior by integrating appropriate response selection with previous experiences and current goals [1,2,3,4,5]. These top-down influences have been explored with tasks that probe decision making under conditions that induce response interference and selective inhibition [6,7,8]. Extensive neuroimaging literature has characterized the predominantly frontal cortical network comprised of the anterior cingulate, ventrolateral (VLPFC) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (DLPFC), insula, as well as the parietal cortex and basal ganglia [6,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16]. The neurofunctional system subserving cognitive control is particularly vulnerable to the effects of both acute alcohol intoxication [17,18,19] and long-term excessive alcohol use [20,21,22,23]. Structural imaging studies indicate that alcohol use disorder (AUD) is associated with compromised white matter tracts, reductions in hippocampal volume, and decreases in cortical thickness [24,25,26,27,28,29]. These measures are predictive of clinical outcomes such as the duration of abstinence after treatment [30]. Results of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies using executive tasks are less consistent with some studies showing decreased [31,32,33], and others reporting increased activation in participants with AUD compared with controls [34,35,36,37,38,39,40]. Activation increase in AUD participants is commonly observed in the absence of executive task performance deficits suggesting that compensatory mechanisms are engaged to maintain normative behavioral performance especially on tasks imposing higher cognitive demands.
Heavy episodic drinking (HED), also termed binge drinking, is a variation of alcohol use characterized by high volume drinking episodes interspersed with periods of low or no intake. It has been on the rise especially among young and emerging adults, raising serious concerns about the neurotoxic effects of alcohol on a large scale [41]. Neurobiologically based accounts of addiction conceptualize HED as a stage in an addiction cycle comprised of withdrawal periods followed by active alcohol seeking/craving behaviors [42,43,44]. While acute alcohol-induced pharmacological effects result in enhanced neural inhibition overall [45,46], protracted periods of heavy episodic drinking elicit neuroadaptive changes to compensate for alcohol’s effects on the brain [42,47]. This is reflected in neural hyperexcitability and associated with symptoms of withdrawal, dysphoria and increased risk of relapse and dependence [48,49,50,51]. Evidence is accumulating in support of the “continuum hypothesis” where HED may serve as a precursor to AUD [52,53,54]. Structural imaging studies have shown that the effects of HED are particularly deleterious during critical stages of brain development and frontal lobe maturation in adolescence and emerging young adulthood [54,55,56,57]. Furthermore, HED is associated with impaired executive functions [58,59,60,61]. Neuroimaging studies of cognitive control functions, however, have reported mixed results with HED participants showing decreased fMRI activation on response inhibition [33,62]. Conversely, increased activation during response inhibition was observed in heavy drinking adolescents [63]. Increased activation in frontal regions was reported with greater reliability during more complex tasks engaging executive functions such as spatial interference [64] and working memory [65]. These findings have been interpreted as reflecting compensatory engagement of cortical areas needed to maintain performance accuracy during cognitively demanding tasks. They primarily encompass frontal regions suggesting an underlying dysregulation of cognitive control which is an important factor in the development of AUD. Indeed, deficient self-regulation can contribute to the formation of habitual, compulsive alcohol consumption resulting in impaired capacity to refrain from drinking [66,67,68,69,70,71,72].
Despite the prevalence of HED and its importance for public health, there is a paucity of studies on the associated harmful consequences in the neurocognitive domain. Most studies report no deficits in intelligence or on task performance in HED indicating that more sensitive neuroimaging measures are needed to characterize deficits associated with binge drinking patterns [54,73,74]. Given the importance of cognitive control for the development of AUD, the aim of the present study was to examine its neural underpinnings and possible emerging signs of excessive drinking habits in HED. Cognitive control is commonly probed with tasks that involve inhibition of automatic responses in favor of those that are task-relevant but non-habitual [75,76]. Participants performed a version of the color-Stroop response conflict task, which has been shown to elicit conflict interference with high sensitivity to alcohol intoxication effects [17,19], during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). HED and matched light episodic drinkers (LED) were compared on behavioral and brain indices of response conflict in the context of a comprehensive set of questionnaires on alcohol-related behaviors, dispositional traits, personality, and intelligence measures.

2. Materials and Method

2.1. Research Participants

Thirty-one healthy, right-handed young adults (18 female, age 24.7 ± 3.9) participated in the study. They had no history of seizures, traumatic brain injury or concussions, no neurological or neuropsychiatric disorders, vision or hearing problems, and they all complied with MRI safety criteria. Participants were medication-free, they reported using no drugs or tobacco products for at least one month prior to the study and none had previously sought or been enrolled in alcohol abuse treatment. Based on the screening information on alcohol consumption rate, frequency and pattern, participants were assigned to heavy episodic drinking group (HED, n = 14) if they reported engaging in ≥5 binge episodes in the previous six months. Light episodic drinking (LED) group comprised individuals who had ≤2 binge episodes in that interval. A binge episode was defined as consuming 5+/6+ drinks for women/men within a two-hour time frame based on research evidence indicating that it is likely to reach a legal level of intoxication (0.08%) [77,78]. The two groups were matched on age, gender, education and family history of alcoholism (see Table 1 for group characteristics). The HED group scored higher on a wide range of alcohol-related variables, higher motor impulsivity [79], and disinhibition and boredom symptoms [80] (Table 1). Young adults were recruited from the San Diego area with flyers and on-line postings. All subjects gave written informed consent to experimental procedures approved by the relevant Institutional Review Boards. Participants were provided monetary compensation for their participation.

2.2. Experimental Protocol

All participants provided information on multiple dimensions of their alcohol use, such as the severity of their alcohol habit (Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test, AUDIT) [81]; the prevalence of particular signs of alcohol misuse (Short Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test, SMAST) [82]; the number and characteristics of drinking occasions that had occurred over the past thirty days (The Time Line Follow Back, TLFB) [83]; the degree to which they crave alcohol (The Penn Alcohol Craving Scale, PACS) [84]; the reasons for engaging in drinking episodes (Drinking Motive Questionnaire Revised Short Form, DMQ-R SF) [85]; quantifying the occurrence of consequences from drinking (Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire, B-YAACQ) [86]; and personality traits (Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, EPQ) [87]. In addition, we collected information regarding the presence of depressive symptomology (Patient Health Questionnaire, EPQ) [88]; anxiety (Generalized Anxiety Disorder, GAD7) [89]; the degree of impulsive behavior for motor, attention and non-planning dimensions (Abbreviated Impulsiveness Scale, ABIS) [79]; attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptomology (Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale, ASRS) [90]; and finally the desire for novel situations and risk-taking behavior (Brief Measure of Sensation Seeking Scale, BSSS) [80]. Intelligence was assessed with the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI-II) [91]. Family history of alcoholism was assessed with a modified version of the Family History Assessment Module (FHAM) [92]. A positive family history for alcoholism (FH+) was defined as having at least one first-degree and one first- or second-degree relative, or at least three second-degree relatives diagnosed with AUD. Statistical analysis between the alcohol use measures, personality scores, and behavioral results were conducted via SPSS 24 [93]. Prior to scanning, subjects were screened for illicit substances via urinary analysis and women were tested for pregnancy and all tested negative.

2.3. Task

Cognitive control processes were probed with a modified Stroop color naming task [17,19,94]. The subjects were instructed to identify the color of the font (red, green, blue or yellow) and respond as quickly and accurately as possible with index and middle fingers of both hands mapped to four buttons (Figure 1). In the congruent (CONG) condition, the color of the font matched the meaning of the color word, whereas in the incongruent (INCONG) condition the color of the font was different from the color word, inducing interference. To maintain reading dominance and automaticity, additional color words were presented in gray font and the subject responded to the meaning of the word (READ) (Figure 1). The Stroop task was presented as a randomized event-related design and consisted of 540 stimuli across four runs comprising 20% (108) CONG, 20% (108) INCONG and 60% (324) READ trials. Words were presented for a stim duration of 300 ms followed by a 1700 ms fixation (XXXX) period for a total stimulus onset asynchrony of 2 s. In addition, 108 null fixation trials were optimally interleaved with Optseq2 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/optseq/) randomization algorithm to allow for proper finite impulse response (FIR) deconvolution modeling during fMRI analysis [95]. The task was programmed with Presentation V.19.0 (Neurobehavioral Systems) to sync with the scanner through transistor–transistor (TTL) pulses.

2.4. Image Acquisition and Analysis

Structural and functional imaging data were collected with a GE Discovery MR750 3.0T whole body scanner equipped with an 8-channel head coil. The head was secured with a pillow and foam padding to minimize movement and maximize subject comfort. The subject was provided M3 earplugs (EAR Soft FX) to dampen scanner noise and protect hearing. A mirror was attached to the head coil to allow for comfortable viewing of the front-projected display. A high-resolution SPGR (spoiled gradient recalled echo) T1-weighted structural image sequence was acquired for each subject with the following parameters: TR = 7.38 ms, TE = 2.984 ms, flip angle = 8°, field of view (FOV) = 240, matrix 256 × 256, 170 slices, 1.2 mm slice thickness with a 94 × 94 mm in-plane resolution. During task administration, 4 runs of functional whole-brain blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) volumes (648 total) were collected with a T2*-weighted echo planar imaging sequence of 35 interleaved bottom-up 4 mm slices in AC-PC orientation (TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 220 mm, matrix 64 × 64, generating a 3.437 × 3.437 mm in-plane resolution). Anatomical and functional volumes were analyzed with AFNI (Analysis of Functional Neuroimages) 17.1.12 [96,97]. AFNI’s Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI; TT_avg152T1) template was used to warp the anatomical and functional datasets to standardized space. Volume registration of the echo planar imaging (EPI) runs was completed by setting the volume with the least number of outlier voxels as the base. A three-dimensional Gaussian kernel (FWHM 8.0) was used to blur the data within each volume and each voxel was scaled to percent signal change before deconvolution was performed. Motion correction was performed by removing TRs exceeding 0.25 mm rotational and translational motion, removing trials in which 25 percent or more of the voxels are considered outliers, and regressing out motion derivatives in deconvolution through six motion parameters and a third-order polynomial for drift. Hemodynamic response function (HRF) was modeled for each trial through AFNI’s version of finite impulse response model (also termed a “tent” function) within a time window from −4 to 12 s with respect to stimulus onset. The contrast matrix was generated by 3dDeconvolve and used for residual maximum likelihood (REML) and generalized least squares (GLSQ) statistical analysis to identify voxels with significant changes [98]. For group level statistics, the coefficients and corresponding t-values generated from individual REML analysis were used for mixed-effects meta-analysis (MEMA). Cluster simulations were performed via 3dClustSim at the group level to adjust for multiple comparisons and to keep p-values below 0.05. Region-of-interest (ROI) analysis was performed to identify the pattern of BOLD activation at anatomical locations associated with the task paradigm in order to contrast activity between heavy and light episodic drinkers. ROIs were chosen from an uncorrelated orthogonal general linear model (GLM) contrast (all conditions v. fixation periods) activation map across all subjects [99,100]. The selected anatomical locations contained active voxels clusters at a p < 0.0001 threshold corrected for multiple testing and family-wise error (FWE) via 3dClustSim. Beta weights representing percent signal change at each point in the time series were extracted from the ROIs for each subject. Time courses were analyzed using a mixed model ANOVA with drinking Group as a between-group factor and Condition as a within-subject factor.

3. Results

3.1. Drinking and Personality Variables

Group characteristics are displayed in Table 1. HED and LED groups were equated on age, education, GPA, gender, family history of alcoholism, and intelligence. HED participants had higher scores on all alcohol-related variables compared to LEDs. They reported higher numbers of drinking days, more drinks per occasion, binge episodes and blackouts in the previous six months. HEDs also reported more alcoholism-related symptoms, higher alcohol cravings and more harmful consequences from drinking. They reported using alcohol as a coping strategy, to enhance their experience in social situations, and because they enjoyed the euphoria it caused. On average, HED participants started drinking around the age of sixteen, two years earlier than LEDs. In contrast, no differences between groups were detected in anxiety, depression, ADHD-like symptoms or personality measures of psychoticism, neuroticism and extraversion. However, HED individuals reported higher motor impulsivity, boredom susceptibility, and disinhibition. In addition, they rated the Stroop task as being more difficult than LEDs.

3.2. Task Performance

Accuracy and reaction times were analyzed with a mixed-model ANOVA with the factors of Group and Condition. As shown in Figure 2, there was a main effect of Condition on task accuracy (F2,58 = 27.04, p < 0.001) and reaction time (F2,58 = 269, p < 0.0001), confirming that the task successfully elicited the Stroop interference effect. The groups did not differ on accuracy (F1,29 = 0.22, p = 0.61) but overall accuracy was the lowest on the INCONG trials compared to both the CONG (F1,30 = 33.0, p < 0.0001) and READ (F1,30 = 27.38, p < 0.0001) trials. Similarly, reaction times (RT) were the longest on the INCONG trials (M = 923 ms), followed by READ (M = 753 ms) and CONG (M = 721 ms), with all conditions differing from each other (p’s < 0.0001). A significant Condition x Group interaction for RT (F2,58 = 5.44, p < 0.01) was driven by the HED individuals responding with significantly slower response times to INCONG stimuli (F1,29 = 4.22, p < 0.05) relative to LEDs (Figure 2). There were no significant task performance differences between men and women. Altogether, this indicates that regardless of gender HEDs were particularly sensitive to response conflict. Indeed, the interference effect, calculated as the RT difference between INCONG and CONG conditions, was positively correlated with all drinking measures (p’s < 0.05). Stroop task difficulty was positively correlated with binge frequency (rs = 0.55, p = 0.002), average drinks per occasion (rs = 0.43, p = 0.02, and the AUDIT (rs = 0.46, p = 0.013).

3.3. Neuroimaging Results

As shown in Figure 3, voxel-wise analysis of the peak activation showed a distributed activation pattern which is broadly consistent with previous studies. The task conditions activated inferior precentral, anterior ventrolateral prefrontal, sensorimotor, parietal, occipital cortices and the insula laterally, and the supplementary/presupplementary cortex and the thalamus medially. A robust main effect of Condition with stronger activation to INCONG compared to CONG and READ trials was observed across most association fronto-parietal activated areas but not in the precentral, sensorimotor, and visual cortices (Figure 4, Table 2). Extracted ROI time series were statistically assessed via mixed-model ANOVA and presented in percent signal change values (Figure 4, Table 2). The largest effects of Group were observed in the VLPFC and the thalamus bilaterally. INCONG trials elicited stronger activity in HED in both the VLPFC (F1,29 = 13.4, p < 0.001) and left thalamus (F1,29 = 5.3, p < 0.05) relative to LED (Figure 3 and Figure 4). Similar to the behavioral results there were no significant gender differences in the ROI analysis.
Nonparametric correlations of Spearman’s Rho were calculated between alcohol measures and extracted time courses for ROI peak activations across groups. Bilateral peak activation of the VLPFC to INCONG trials was positively correlated with binge episodes, blackouts, and the SMAST and AUDIT alcohol severity measures (p’s < 0.02). Peak activation of the left thalamus elicited by INCONG trials was positively correlated with binge episodes (rs = 0.38, p = 0.035). There was a significant positive relationship between the rVLPFC and Stroop difficulty during both CONG (rs = 0.43, p = 0.023) and INCONG (rs = 0.466, p = 0.013) trials. Finally, when parsing apart Groups, HED individuals have a positive correlation between the rVLPFC with non-planning impulsivity (rs = 0.601, p = 0.03) during INCONG trials.

4. Discussion

This study used fMRI to investigate behavioral and brain activity indices of cognitive control in heavy and light episodic drinkers during response conflict evoked by the Stroop task. As expected, the high conflict (INCONG) condition evoked greater activity in the fronto-parietal association cortices. Importantly, this activation was greater for HEDs in the VLPFC and thalamus bilaterally relative to LEDs. Conflict-induced activity in the bilateral VLPFC was positively correlated with levels of alcohol intake including binge episodes and the number of drinks consumed per occasion, as well as measures of alcoholism-related symptoms (AUDIT, SMAST) and blackouts. Reported binge episodes were further correlated with activity in the left thalamus. In the absence of accuracy deficits, HEDs had significantly slower RTs to INCONG stimuli, which presumably allowed them to maintain performance levels on par with LEDs. HEDs were particularly affected by response conflict as both the Stroop interference effect and task difficulty ratings were associated with alcohol intake measures. Taken together, these results suggest that heavy episodic drinkers benefit from engaging an expanded cognitive control network and responding more slowly to resolve stimulus conflict. These effects are especially pronounced in those with heavier drinking patterns and more binge episodes which may be indicative of the deleterious effects of excessive alcohol use.
Binge drinking participants in the present study were young, healthy individuals who showed no cognitive deficits on a standardized intelligence scale. Nonetheless, the observed group differences in brain activity are consistent with those found in AUD samples. Increased activity of the VLPFC has been reported on tasks probing working memory [38,39,101], response inhibition [35], and delay discounting [36]. Our results are most in line with the findings by Wilcox and colleagues [37] who used a multisensory Stroop and observed bilateral VLPFC hyperactivation and an overall increase in RTs in AUD participants compared to the control group. Despite a paucity of neuroimaging studies investigating cognitive control in binge drinkers, the results that have emerged are also similar to those found in AUD. Increased activity of the VLPFC has been reported on a task probing spatial interference [64] and declarative memory in young adult heavy drinkers [40]. A study of spatial working memory in adolescents reported greater activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus in male but not female adolescent binge drinkers [102]. Moreover, on the Eriksen flanker task, increased activity was induced by response conflict in the VLPFC under acute intoxication in social drinkers [103]. Inconsistent reports notwithstanding [33,62,104], accruing neuroimaging evidence indicates frontal hyperactivation in AUD and binge drinkers compared to low-drinking control groups. More studies are needed to corroborate these findings, but it appears that greater inferior frontal activation is elicited by tasks that impose higher demands on cognitive control by relying on deliberative functions and multidimensional contingencies [105].
This converging evidence is consistent with compensatory accounts of increased engagement across cognitive and emotional neurofunctional systems in alcoholism [21,23,106,107,108]. Extensive functional imaging evidence suggests that the VLPFC is activated by tasks probing cognitive control [8]. It has been proposed as a key area for inhibitory control of inappropriate motor responses [16,109] but it is also activated during attentional capture [110] and domain-general tasks that are cognitively demanding but that do not rely on inhibition [111,112]. Meta analyses have provided further insights into functional specificity within the VLPFC subregions [113]. However, it is increasingly clear that the VLPFC is an integral part of a network that is engaged by a range of tasks imposing attentional demands in the context of processing novelty, contingency monitoring and conflict resolution [110,111,114]. Functional connectivity studies have revealed extensive connections of the VLPFC with other parts of the lateral frontal cortex, the anterior cingulate, parietal, and temporal cortices [115]. Therefore, in response to increased demands imposed by multi-rule tasks, the VLPFC is likely to be recruited in a process of flexible network reconfiguration [114]. On that view, the brain functions as a dynamic, interactive system that handles changing environmental demands via targeted, yet flexible and integrated engagement of the relevant neurofunctional networks in order to optimize responding [116]. These networks interact across spatiotemporal scales, they are synchronized from local, specialized, to global-level networks and reflect typical [117] and pathological changes [118]. In the present study, greater activation of the VLPFC was elicited by response conflict selectively in HEDs and correlated with Stroop difficulty ratings. Furthermore, both of these variables were associated with various measures of alcohol intake and harmful drinking consequences. Therefore, the converging evidence suggests that in HEDs the task demands exceeded the normative network capacity, necessitating compensatory engagement of the VLPFC and adjustment of response strategy. The observed activation increase is associated with heavy alcohol intake and may reflect adaptation of the brain’s functional networks to the sequelae of heavy drinking. This interpretation is consistent with evidence indicating an expansion of primarily frontal networks in AUD. Muller-Oehring and colleagues [119] examined functional connectivity in sober alcoholics compared to matched controls during wakeful rest and reported expanded attention/salience network which comprised the inferior frontal cortex. The compensatory interpretation is supported by the correlation between better task performance and network enlargement [119]. Not all studies, however, show increased activity in brain regions implicated in cognitive control. Decreased activation has been reported in studies with simpler contingencies that rely on response inhibition such as the go/no go and stop signal tasks [31,32,33]. These tasks may not be challenging enough to generate high conflict and may not engage cognitive control at the level that would necessitate activation of additional areas [120]. The present results could serve as an indirect indication of impaired cognitive control in HED individuals.
Overwhelming evidence indicates that cognitive control is subserved by a distributed, but predominantly frontal, cortical network [4,6,13,121]. It has been established that AUD is associated with a range of impairments in the cognitive domain and neurophysiological changes in the frontal lobes [21,22,23,122]. This degradation of prefrontal functions results in impaired decision making and self-control which further contribute to the development of alcohol dependence [67,123,124,125]. The Stroop task probes top-down regulation by necessitating the suppression of prepotent responses in favor of controlled processing. The sensitivity of prefrontal regions to the effects of acute alcohol on response interference has been shown in previous imaging studies [17,19]. Results of the present study additionally indicate cognitive control deficits in young adult HEDs. This supports existing evidence that self-control impairments can contribute to excessive drinking [66,69,72]. Indeed, automatic modes of processing are associated with the increased risk of heavy alcohol use [126], its maintenance across time [127], and relapse in abstinent alcoholics [128].
Our study revealed group differences in thalamic activity with HEDs showing greater sensitivity to response interference. The increased activation was positively correlated with the number of binge episodes. Similar results were reported by Campanella and colleagues [65], with increased thalamic activity in HEDs during working memory in association with drinking levels. In alcohol-dependent participants, increased thalamic activity was observed during an auditory go/nogo [35] and multisensory Stroop task [37], but lower activity during working memory [129]. Indeed, there is growing evidence that the thalamus plays a modulatory role in integrating activity across different levels of the neuraxis and that it contributes to cognitive control and flexible action selection [130,131,132,133,134]. In the present study, HED participants not only exhibited greater thalamic activity, but they also rated the task as being significantly more difficult and responded with longer RTs than the LED subjects indicating that the task imposed increased cognitive demands. It is likely that the increased difficulty was accompanied by heightened arousal [135,136] which is in part subserved by the thalamus [137,138]. Another possibility is that the increased activation of the thalamus reflects neuroadaptive changes resulting from frequent bouts of heavy drinking [42,45]. The thalamus is sensitive to long-term excessive alcohol use and is implicated in a range of deficits across distributed neural circuits [139]. Smaller thalamic volumes are predictive of subsequent relapse and alcohol intake in chronic alcoholics [140]. Thalamic hyperactivity could, therefore, reflect its sensitivity to protracted heavy use and its compensatory engagement during increased response conflict difficulty [106].
Measures of impulsivity and disinhibition were positively correlated with alcohol intake variables in the present study, confirming well-established associations between AUD and traits of impulsivity, hyperactivity, and sensation/novelty seeking [141,142,143,144]. Dysregulation of impulse control underlies the inability to maintain inhibitory control over drinking, which has been considered fundamental to addiction [67,123,145,146,147]. Findings suggest that the vulnerability to alcoholism shares a common genetic component with a cluster of impulsivity traits that may predispose individuals to AUD [144,148,149]. Recent evidence converges on dopaminergic modulation of impulsive behavior [150,151], suggesting that the same genetic pathways may mediate both addiction and impulsivity [152,153]. Overall, these findings are strongly suggestive of shared genetic vulnerability to AUD and externalizing traits. The present study cannot speak to the issue of possible pre-existing characteristics of the HED sample unrelated to drinking levels that may have resulted in the observed group differences. However, the behavioral and brain activity findings are correlated with a range of alcohol intake measures.
Moreover, findings across different studies indicate that the compensatory activity increase may reflect deficits as a function of alcohol-induced neurotoxicity. In a working memory task, Campanella and colleagues [65] reported positive correlation between alcohol consumption and activity in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in binge drinkers. Wetherill and colleagues [154] tracked the development of alcohol habits in young adults and similarly reported a positive relationship between alcohol-induced blackouts and increased prefrontal on a response inhibition task. In a study of alcohol-dependent participants, lifetime alcohol consumption was predictive of activity in the posterior cingulate cortex and midbrain during a multisensory Stroop task [155]. Taken together, the evidence suggests that the disinhibitory traits are implicated in drinking initiation and maintenance via impaired self-control, but that the group differences in neural function may at least in part reflect alcohol-induced neurotoxicity.
The development of addiction is an exceedingly complex process mediated by environmental risk factors, and interindividual variability [156] due in part to a person’s genetic makeup [52,157,158,159,160]. However, heavy episodic drinking is associated with deficits in neural functioning in response to conflict-inducing situations with increased vulnerability in adolescents and emerging adults [48,57,161]. These neural compromises are often not detectable by behavioral measures but can be revealed by measures of neural function [74,162,163] and may signify early trajectory toward compulsive intake characterizing AUD [42,44,164]. Given that individuals who engage in most hazardous binge drinking are young and vulnerable to neurotoxicity, it is of paramount importance to better understand the neural indices associated with excessive drinking. Such insights may inform development of therapeutic, personally tailored approaches and prevention strategies.

Acknowledgments

We thank members of the Spatio-Temporal Brain Imaging Lab at SDSU for their assistance. This study was supported by start-up funds from the College of Science at San Diego State University and the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism R01 AA 016624 and T32 AA013525

Author Contributions

K.M., S.M.M. and L.A.H. conceived and designed the experiments; L.E.B., J.P.H., L.A.H. and S.M.M. performed the experiments; S.M.M. and D.A. analyzed the data; L.E.B., L.A.H., J.P.H. and S.H. contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools; S.M.M. and K.M. wrote the paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Posner, M.; Snyder, C.R.R. Attention and cognitive control. In Information Processing and Cognition; Solso, R.L., Ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1975; pp. 55–85. [Google Scholar]
  2. Miller, E.K. The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2000, 1, 59–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Badre, D. Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro-caudal organization of the frontal lobes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008, 12, 193–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Niendam, T.A.; Laird, A.R.; Ray, K.L.; Dean, Y.M.; Glahn, D.C.; Carter, C.S. Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2012, 12, 241–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kennerley, S.W.; Walton, M.E. Decision making and reward in frontal cortex: Complementary evidence from neurophysiological and neuropsychological studies. Behav. Neurosci. 2011, 125, 297–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Laird, A.R.; McMillan, K.M.; Lancaster, J.L.; Kochunov, P.; Turkeltaub, P.E.; Pardo, J.V.; Fox, P.T. A comparison of label-based review and ALE meta-analysis in the Stroop task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005, 25, 6–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Neumann, J.; Lohmann, G.; Derrfuss, J.; von Cramon, D.Y. Meta-analysis of functional imaging data using replicator dynamics. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005, 25, 165–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Nee, D.E.; Wager, T.D.; Jonides, J. Interference resolution: Insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2007, 7, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Roberts, K.L.; Hall, D.A. Examining a supramodal network for conflict processing: A systematic review and novel functional magnetic resonance imaging data for related visual and auditory Stroop tasks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008, 20, 1063–1078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Summerfield, C.; Koechlin, E. Decision making and prefrontal executive function. In The Cognitive Neurosciences; Gazzaniga, M., Ed.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2009; pp. 1019–1030. [Google Scholar]
  11. Ridderinkhof, K.R.; Ullsperger, M.; Crone, E.A.; Nieuwenhuis, S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 2004, 306, 443–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Posner, M.I.; Rothbart, M.K.; Sheese, B.E.; Voelker, P. Control networks and neuromodulators of early development. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 48, 827–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Brass, M.; Derrfuss, J.; Forstmann, B.; von Cramon, D.Y. The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 314–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Botvinick, M.M. Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2007, 7, 356–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Carter, C.S.; van Veen, V. Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: An update of theory and data. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2007, 7, 367–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Aron, A.R.; Robbins, T.W.; Poldrack, R.A. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade on. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014, 18, 177–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Marinkovic, K.; Rickenbacher, E.; Azma, S.; Artsy, E. Acute alcohol intoxication impairs top-down regulation of Stroop incongruity as revealed by blood oxygen level-dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012, 33, 319–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Marinkovic, K.; Rickenbacher, E.; Azma, S.; Artsy, E.; Lee, A.K. Effects of acute alcohol intoxication on saccadic conflict and error processing. Psychopharmacology 2013, 230, 487–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Kovacevic, S.; Azma, S.; Irimia, A.; Sherfey, J.; Halgren, E.; Marinkovic, K. Theta oscillations are sensitive to both early and late conflict processing stages: Effects of alcohol intoxication. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e43957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Moselhy, H.F.; Georgiou, G.; Kahn, A. Frontal lobe changes in alcoholism: A review of the literature. Alcohol Alcohol. 2001, 36, 357–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Oscar-Berman, M.; Marinković, K. Alcohol: Effects on neurobehavioral functions and the brain. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2007, 17, 239–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Le Berre, A.P.; Fama, R.; Sullivan, E.V. Executive functions, memory, and social cognitive deficits and recovery in chronic alcoholism: A critical review to inform future research. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2017, 41, 1432–1443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Sullivan, E.V.; Pfefferbaum, A. Neurocircuitry in alcoholism: A substrate of disruption and repair. Psychopharmacology 2005, 180, 583–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Sullivan, E.V.; Harris, R.A.; Pfefferbaum, A. Alcohol’s effects on brain and behavior. Alcohol Res. Health 2010, 33, 127–143. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  25. Zahr, N.M.; Pfefferbaum, A.; Sullivan, E.V. Perspectives on fronto-fugal circuitry from human imaging of alcohol use disorders. Neuropharmacology 2017, 122, 189–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Harris, G.J.; Jaffin, S.K.; Hodge, S.M.; Kennedy, D.; Caviness, V.S.; Marinkovic, K.; Papadimitriou, G.M.; Makris, N.; Oscar-Berman, M. Frontal white matter and cingulum diffusion tensor imaging deficits in alcoholism. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2008, 32, 1001–1013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Fortier, C.B.; Leritz, E.C.; Salat, D.H.; Venne, J.R.; Maksimovskiy, A.L.; Williams, V.; Milberg, W.P.; McGlinchey, R.E. Reduced cortical thickness in abstinent alcoholics and association with alcoholic behavior. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2011, 35, 2193–2201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Chanraud, S.; Martelli, C.; Delain, F.; Kostogianni, N.; Douaud, G.; Aubin, H.J.; Reynaud, M.; Martinot, J.L. Brain morphometry and cognitive performance in detoxified alcohol-dependents with preserved psychosocial functioning. Neuropsychopharmacology 2007, 32, 429–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Ruiz, S.M.; Oscar-Berman, M.; Sawyer, K.S.; Valmas, M.M.; Urban, T.; Harris, G.J. Drinking history associations with regional white matter volumes in alcoholic men and women. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2013, 37, 110–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Durazzo, T.C.; Mon, A.; Gazdzinski, S.; Meyerhoff, D.J. Regional brain volume changes in alcohol-dependent individuals during early abstinence: Associations with relapse following treatment. Addict. Biol. 2017, 22, 1416–1425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Li, C.S.; Luo, X.; Yan, P.; Bergquist, K.; Sinha, R. Altered impulse control in alcohol dependence: Neural measures of stop signal performance. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2009, 33, 740–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Claus, E.D.; Feldstein Ewing, S.W.; Filbey, F.M.; Hutchison, K.E. Behavioral control in alcohol use disorders: Relationships with severity. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 2013, 74, 141–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Hu, S.; Zhang, S.; Chao, H.H.; Krystal, J.H.; Li, C.S.R. Association of drinking problems and duration of alcohol use to inhibitory control in nondependent young adult social drinkers. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2016, 40, 319–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Czapla, M.; Baeuchl, C.; Simon, J.J.; Richter, B.; Kluge, M.; Friederich, H.-C.; Mann, K.; Herpertz, S.C.; Loeber, S. Do alcohol-dependent patients show different neural activation during response inhibition than healthy controls in an alcohol-related fmri go/no-go-task? Psychopharmacology 2017, 234, 1001–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Karch, S.; Jager, L.; Karamatskos, E.; Graz, C.; Stammel, A.; Flatz, W.; Lutz, J.; Holtschmidt-Taschner, B.; Genius, J.; Leicht, G.; et al. Influence of trait anxiety on inhibitory control in alcohol-dependent patients: Simultaneous acquisition of ERPs and BOLD responses. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2008, 42, 734–745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Claus, E.D.; Kiehl, K.A.; Hutchison, K.E. Neural and behavioral mechanisms of impulsive choice in alcohol use disorder. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2011, 35, 1209–1219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Wilcox, C.E.; Mayer, A.R.; Bogenschutz, M.P.; Ling, J.; Dekonenko, C.; Cumbo, H. Cognitive control network function in alcohol use disorder before and during treatment with lorazepam. Subst. Use Misuse 2015, 50, 40–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Pfefferbaum, A.; Desmond, J.E.; Galloway, C.; Menon, V.; Glover, G.H.; Sullivan, E.V. Reorganization of frontal systems used by alcoholics for spatial working memory: An fMRI study. NeuroImage 2001, 14, 7–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Desmond, J.E.; Chen, S.A.; DeRosa, E.; Pryor, M.R.; Pfefferbaum, A.; Sullivan, E.V. Increased frontocerebellar activation in alcoholics during verbal working memory: An fMRI study. NeuroImage 2003, 19, 1510–1520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Dager, A.D.; Jamadar, S.; Stevens, M.C.; Rosen, R.; Jiantonio-Kelly, R.E.; Sisante, J.-F.; Raskin, S.A.; Tennen, H.; Austad, C.S.; Wood, R.M. FMRI response during figural memory task performance in college drinkers. Psychopharmacology 2014, 231, 167–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Patrick, M.E.; Terry-McElrath, Y.M.; Kloska, D.D.; Schulenberg, J.E. High-intensity drinking among young adults in the United States: Prevalence, frequency, and developmental change. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2016, 40, 1905–1912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Koob, G.F.; Le Moal, M. Neurobiological mechanisms for opponent motivational processes in addiction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 2008, 363, 3113–3123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Koob, G.F. Neurobiology of addiction. Focus 2011, 9, 55–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Koob, G.F. Theoretical frameworks and mechanistic aspects of alcohol addiction: Alcohol addiction as a reward deficit disorder. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 2013, 13, 3–30. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  45. Roberto, M.; Varodayan, F.P. Synaptic targets: Chronic alcohol actions. Neuropharmacology 2017, 122, 85–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Burnett, E.J.; Chandler, L.J.; Trantham-Davidson, H. Glutamatergic plasticity and alcohol dependence-induced alterations in reward, affect and cognition. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2016, 65, 309–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Koob, G.F.; Le Moal, M. Neurobiology of Addiction; Elsevier Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  48. Crabbe, J.C.; Harris, R.A.; Koob, G.F. Preclinical studies of alcohol binge drinking. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2011, 1216, 24–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Becker, H.C.; Mulholland, P.J. Neurochemical mechanisms of alcohol withdrawal. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2014, 125, 133–156. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  50. Duka, T.; Gentry, J.; Malcolm, R.; Ripley, T.L.; Borlikova, G.; Stephens, D.N.; Veatch, L.M.; Becker, H.C.; Crews, F.T. Consequences of multiple withdrawals from alcohol. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2004, 28, 233–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Koob, G.F.; Le Moal, M. Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2008, 59, 29–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Enoch, M.A. Genetic and environmental influences on the development of alcoholism. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2006, 1094, 193–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Lannoy, S.; Heeren, A.; Moyaerts, N.; Bruneau, N.; Evrard, S.; Billieux, J.; Maurage, P. Differential impairments across attentional networks in binge drinking. Psychopharmacology 2017, 234, 1059–1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Petit, G.; Maurage, P.; Kornreich, C.; Verbanck, P.; Campanella, S. Binge drinking in adolescents: A review of neurophysiological and neuroimaging research. Alcohol Alcohol. 2013, 49, 198–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Jacobus, J.; Tapert, S.F. Neurotoxic effects of alcohol in adolescence. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2013, 9, 703–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Silveri, M.M.; Dager, A.D.; Cohen-Gilbert, J.E.; Sneider, J.T. Neurobiological signatures associated with alcohol and drug use in the human adolescent brain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016, 70, 244–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Cservenka, A.; Brumback, T. The burden of binge and heavy drinking on the brain: Effects on adolescent and young adult neural structure and function. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Gil-Hernandez, S.; Garcia-Moreno, L.M. Executive performance and dysexecutive symptoms in binge drinking adolescents. Alcohol 2016, 51, 79–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Parada, M.; Corral, M.; Mota, N.; Crego, A.; Rodriguez Holguin, S.; Cadaveira, F. Executive functioning and alcohol binge drinking in university students. Addict. Behav. 2012, 37, 167–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Scaife, J.C.; Duka, T. Behavioural measures of frontal lobe function in a population of young social drinkers with binge drinking pattern. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2009, 93, 354–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Hermens, D.F.; Lagopoulos, J.; Tobias-Webb, J.; de Regt, T.; Dore, G.; Juckes, L.; Latt, N.; Hickie, I.B. Pathways to alcohol-induced brain impairment in young people: A review. Cortex 2013, 49, 3–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Ahmadi, A.; Pearlson, G.D.; Meda, S.A.; Dager, A.; Potenza, M.N.; Rosen, R.; Austad, C.S.; Raskin, S.A.; Fallahi, C.R.; Tennen, H. Influence of alcohol use on neural response to go/no-go task in college drinkers. Neuropsychopharmacology 2013, 38, 2197–2208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Wetherill, R.R.; Squeglia, L.M.; Yang, T.T.; Tapert, S.F. A longitudinal examination of adolescent response inhibition: Neural differences before and after the initiation of heavy drinking. Psychopharmacology 2013, 230, 663–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Kashfi, K.; Fang, D.; Hou, J.; Al-Khalil, K.; Anderson, R.; Syapin, P.J.; O’Boyle, M.W. Spatial attention in binge-drinking and moderate-drinking college students: An fMRI investigation. Alcohol. Treat. Q. 2017, 35, 260–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Campanella, S.; Peigneux, P.; Petit, G.; Lallemand, F.; Saeremans, M.; Noël, X.; Metens, T.; Nouali, M.; de Tiège, X.; de Witte, P. Increased cortical activity in binge drinkers during working memory task: A preliminary assessment through a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e62260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Field, M.; Wiers, R.W.; Christiansen, P.; Fillmore, M.T.; Verster, J.C. Acute alcohol effects on inhibitory control and implicit cognition: Implications for loss of control over drinking. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2010, 34, 1346–1352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Fillmore, M.T. Drug abuse as a problem of impaired control: Current approaches and findings. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 2003, 2, 179–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Finn, P. Acute effects of alcohol on cognition and impulsive-disinhibited behavior. In Review of NIAAA’s Neuroscience and Behavioral Research Portfolio; Noronha, A., Eckardt, M., Warren, K., Eds.; US Department of Health and Human Services: Bethesda, MD, USA, 2000; Volume 34, pp. 337–356. [Google Scholar]
  69. Lyvers, M. “Loss of control” in alcoholism and drug addiction: A neuroscientific interpretation. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2000, 8, 225–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Rodriguez-Jimenez, R.; Avila, C.; Ponce, G.; Ibanez, M.I.; Rubio, G.; Jimenez-Arriero, M.A.; Ampuero, I.; Ramos, J.A.; Hoenicka, J.; Palomo, T. The TaqIA polymorphism linked to the DRD2 gene is related to lower attention and less inhibitory control in alcoholic patients. Eur. Psychiatry 2006, 21, 66–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Nigg, J.T.; Wong, M.M.; Martel, M.M.; Jester, J.M.; Puttler, L.I.; Glass, J.M.; Adams, K.M.; Fitzgerald, H.E.; Zucker, R.A. Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2006, 45, 468–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Volkow, N.D.; Fowler, J.S.; Wang, G.J.; Swanson, J.M.; Telang, F. Dopamine in drug abuse and addiction: Results of imaging studies and treatment implications. Arch. Neurol. 2007, 64, 1575–1579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Crego, A.; Holguin, S.R.; Parada, M.; Mota, N.; Corral, M.; Cadaveira, F. Binge drinking affects attentional and visual working memory processing in young university students. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2009, 33, 1870–1879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Maurage, P.; Pesenti, M.; Philippot, P.; Joassin, F.; Campanella, S. Latent deleterious effects of binge drinking over a short period of time revealed only by electrophysiological measures. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009, 34, 111–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. MacLeod, C.M.; MacDonald, P.A. Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: Uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 383–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Roelofs, A.; van Turennout, M.; Coles, M.G. Anterior cingulate cortex activity can be independent of response conflict in Stroop-like tasks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 13884–13889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Lange, J.E.; Voas, R.B. Defining binge drinking quantities through resulting blood alcohol concentrations. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2001, 15, 310–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Read, J.P.; Beattie, M.; Chamberlain, R.; Merrill, J.E. Beyond the “binge” threshold: Heavy drinking patterns and their association with alcohol involvement indices in college students. Addict. Behav. 2008, 33, 225–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Coutlee, C.G.; Politzer, C.S.; Hoyle, R.H.; Huettel, S.A. An abbreviated impulsiveness scale constructed through confirmatory factor analysis of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale version 11. Arch. Sci. Psychol. 2014, 2, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Hoyle, R.H.; Stephenson, M.T.; Palmgreen, P.; Lorch, E.P.; Donohew, R.L. Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2002, 32, 401–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Saunders, J.B.; Aasland, O.G.; Babor, T.F.; de la Fuente, J.R.; Grant, M. Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-II. Addiction 1993, 88, 791–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Selzer, M.L.; Vinokur, A.; van Rooijen, L. A self-administered Short Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (SMAST). J. Stud. Alcohol 1975, 36, 117–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Sobell, L.C.; Sobell, M.B. Timeline follow-back. In Measuring Alcohol Consumption; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 41–72. [Google Scholar]
  84. Flannery, B.; Volpicelli, J.; Pettinati, H. Psychometric properties of the Penn Alcohol Craving scale. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 1999, 23, 1289–1295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Kuntsche, E.; Kuntsche, S. Development and validation of the Drinking Motive Questionnaire Revised Short Form (DMQ-R SF). J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2009, 38, 899–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Kahler, C.W.; Strong, D.R.; Read, J.P. Toward efficient and comprehensive measurement of the alcohol problems continuum in college students: The brief young adult alcohol consequences questionnaire. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2005, 29, 1180–1189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Eysenck, H.J.; Eysenck, S.B.G. Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire; Hodder & Staughton: London, UK, 1975. [Google Scholar]
  88. Kroenke, K.; Spitzer, R.L. The PHQ-9: A new depression diagnostic and severity measure. Psychiatr. Ann. 2002, 32, 509–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Spitzer, R.L.; Kroenke, K.; Williams, J.B.; Löwe, B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Arch. Intern. Med. 2006, 166, 1092–1097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Kessler, R.C.; Adler, L.; Ames, M.; Demler, O.; Faraone, S.; Hiripi, E.; Howes, M.J.; Jin, R.; Secnik, K.; Spencer, T. The world health organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): A short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol. Med. 2005, 35, 245–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI-II); The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  92. Rice, J.P.; Reich, T.; Bucholz, K.K.; Neuman, R.J.; Fishman, R.; Rochberg, N.; Hesselbrock, V.M.; Nurnberger, J.I.; Schuckit, M.A.; Begleiter, H. Comparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 1995, 19, 1018–1023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  93. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24.0. Armonk. Available online: http://www-01.ibm.com/support/docview.wss?uid=swg27047057 (accessed on 20 December 2017).
  94. Repovs, G. The mode of response and the stroop effect: A reaction time analysis. Horiz. Psychol. 2004, 13, 105–114. [Google Scholar]
  95. Burock, M.A.; Buckner, R.L.; Woldorff, M.G.; Rosen, B.R.; Dale, A.M. Randomized event-related experimental designs allow for extremely rapid presentation rates using functional MRI. Neuroreport 1998, 9, 3735–3739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Cox, R.W. AFNI: What a long strange trip it’s been. NeuroImage 2012, 62, 743–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Cox, R.W. AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 1996, 29, 162–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Chen, G.; Saad, Z.S.; Nath, A.R.; Beauchamp, M.S.; Cox, R.W. FMRI group analysis combining effect estimates and their variances. Neuroimage 2012, 60, 747–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Friston, K.J.; Rotshtein, P.; Geng, J.J.; Sterzer, P.; Henson, R.N. A critique of functional localisers. NeuroImage 2006, 30, 1077–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Poldrack, R.A. Region of interest analysis for fMRI. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2007, 2, 67–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Caldwell, L.C.; Schweinsburg, A.D.; Nagel, B.J.; Barlett, V.C.; Brown, S.A.; Tapert, S.F. Gender and adolescent alcohol use disorders on BOLD (Blood Oxygen Level Dependent) response to spatial working memory. Alcohol Alcohol. 2005, 40, 194–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Squeglia, L.M.; Schweinsburg, A.D.; Pulido, C.; Tapert, S.F. Adolescent binge drinking linked to abnormal spatial working memory brain activation: Differential gender effects. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2011, 35, 1831–1841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Marinkovic, K.; Rickenbacher, E.; Azma, S. Effects of alcohol intoxication on response conflict in a flanker task. J. Addict. Res. Ther. 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Tapert, S.F.; Pulido, C.; Paulus, M.P.; Schuckit, M.A.; Burke, C. Level of response to alcohol and brain response during visual working memory. J. Stud. Alcohol. 2004, 65, 692–700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Wilcox, C.E.; Dekonenko, C.J.; Mayer, A.R.; Bogenschutz, M.P.; Turner, J.A. Cognitive control in alcohol use disorder: Deficits and clinical relevance. Rev. Neurosci. 2014, 25, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Chanraud, S.; Sullivan, E. Compensatory recruitment of neural resources in chronic alcoholism. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2014, 125, 369–380. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  107. Oscar-Berman, M.; Valmas, M.M.; Sawyer, K.S.; Ruiz, S.M.; Luhar, R.B.; Gravitz, Z.R. Profiles of impaired, spared, and recovered neuropsychological processes in alcoholism. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2014, 125, 183–210. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  108. Marinkovic, K.; Oscar-Berman, M.; Urban, T.; O’Reilly, C.E.; Howard, J.A.; Sawyer, K.; Harris, G.J. Alcoholism and dampened temporal limbic activation to emotional faces. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2009, 33, 1880–1892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Chikazoe, J.; Konishi, S.; Asari, T.; Jimura, K.; Miyashita, Y. Activation of right inferior frontal gyrus during response inhibition across response modalities. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007, 19, 69–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Erika-Florence, M.; Leech, R.; Hampshire, A. A functional network perspective on response inhibition and attentional control. Nat. Commun. 2014, 5, 4073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Chatham, C.H.; Claus, E.D.; Kim, A.; Curran, T.; Banich, M.T.; Munakata, Y. Cognitive control reflects context monitoring, not motoric stopping, in response inhibition. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e31546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Hampshire, A. Putting the brakes on inhibitory models of frontal lobe function. NeuroImage 2015, 113, 340–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Levy, B.J.; Wagner, A.D. Cognitive control and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: Reflexive reorienting, motor inhibition, and action updating. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2011, 1224, 40–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Braun, U.; Schäfer, A.; Walter, H.; Erk, S.; Romanczuk-Seiferth, N.; Haddad, L.; Schweiger, J.I.; Grimm, O.; Heinz, A.; Tost, H. Dynamic reconfiguration of frontal brain networks during executive cognition in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 11678–11683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Goulas, A.; Uylings, H.B.; Stiers, P. Unravelling the intrinsic functional organization of the human lateral frontal cortex: A parcellation scheme based on resting state fMRI. J. Neurosci. 2012, 32, 10238–10252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Bullmore, E.; Sporns, O. The economy of brain network organization. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012, 13, 336–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Breakspear, M.; Stam, C.J. Dynamics of a neural system with a multiscale architecture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 2005, 360, 1051–1074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Fornito, A.; Zalesky, A.; Breakspear, M. The connectomics of brain disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015, 16, 159–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  119. Müller-Oehring, E.M.; Jung, Y.-C.; Pfefferbaum, A.; Sullivan, E.V.; Schulte, T. The resting brain of alcoholics. Cereb. Cortex 2014, 25, 4155–4168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Rosen, B.Q.; Padovan, N.; Marinkovic, K. Alcohol hits you when it is hard: Intoxication, task difficulty, and theta brain oscillations. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2016, 40, 743–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Miller, E.K.; Cohen, J.D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 24, 167–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Oscar-Berman, M.; Marinkovic, K. Alcoholism and the brain: An overview. Alcohol Res. Health 2004, 27, 125–133. [Google Scholar]
  123. Goldstein, R.Z.; Volkow, N.D. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: Neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2011, 12, 652–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Koob, G.F.; Volkow, N.D. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010, 35, 217–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Kwako, L.E.; Momenan, R.; Litten, R.Z.; Koob, G.F.; Goldman, D. Addictions neuroclinical assessment: A neuroscience-based framework for addictive disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 2016, 80, 179–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Weafer, J.; Fillmore, M.T. Acute alcohol effects on attentional bias in heavy and moderate drinkers. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2013, 27, 32–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Paz, A.L.; Keim, C.A.; Rosselli, M. Inhibitory performance predicting drinking behaviours among young adults. Alcohol Alcohol. 2016, 51, 677–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Grüsser, S.M.; Wrase, J.; Klein, S.; Hermann, D.; Smolka, M.N.; Ruf, M.; Weber-Fahr, W.; Flor, H.; Mann, K.; Braus, D.F. Cue-induced activation of the striatum and medial prefrontal cortex is associated with subsequent relapse in abstinent alcoholics. Psychopharmacology 2004, 175, 296–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  129. Tapert, S.F.; Brown, G.G.; Kindermann, S.S.; Cheung, E.H.; Frank, L.R.; Brown, S.A. FMRI measurement of brain dysfunction in alcohol-dependent young women. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2001, 25, 236–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Sherman, S.M. Thalamus plays a central role in ongoing cortical functioning. Nat. Neurosci. 2016, 19, 533–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Peters, S.K.; Dunlop, K.; Downar, J. Cortico-striatal-thalamic loop circuits of the salience network: A central pathway in psychiatric disease and treatment. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  132. Minamimoto, T.; Hori, Y.; Yamanaka, K.; Kimura, M. Neural signal for counteracting pre-action bias in the centromedian thalamic nucleus. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  133. Saalmann, Y.B. Intralaminar and medial thalamic influence on cortical synchrony, information transmission and cognition. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  134. Ridderinkhof, K.R.; van den Wildenberg, W.P.; Segalowitz, S.J.; Carter, C.S. Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain Cogn. 2004, 56, 129–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  135. Verguts, T.; Notebaert, W. Adaptation by binding: A learning account of cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009, 13, 252–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Critchley, H.D. Psychophysiology of neural, cognitive and affective integration: FMRI and autonomic indicants. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2009, 73, 88–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  137. Schiff, N.D. Central thalamic contributions to arousal regulation and neurological disorders of consciousness. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008, 1129, 105–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  138. Kirouac, G.J. Placing the paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus within the brain circuits that control behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015, 56, 315–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  139. Pitel, A.L.; Segobin, S.H.; Ritz, L.; Eustache, F.; Beaunieux, H. Thalamic abnormalities are a cardinal feature of alcohol-related brain dysfunction. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015, 54, 38–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  140. Segobin, S.H.; Chetelat, G.; Le Berre, A.P.; Lannuzel, C.; Boudehent, C.; Vabret, F.; Eustache, F.; Beaunieux, H.; Pitel, A.L. Relationship between brain volumetric changes and interim drinking at six months in alcohol-dependent patients. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2014, 38, 739–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  141. Moeller, F.G.; Dougherty, D.M. Antisocial personality disorder, alcohol, and aggression. Alcohol Res. Health 2001, 25, 5–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  142. Mitchell, M.R.; Potenza, M.N. Addictions and personality traits: Impulsivity and related constructs. Curr. Behav. Neurosci. Rep. 2014, 1, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  143. Sher, K.J.; Trull, T.J. Personality and disinhibitory psychopathology: Alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1994, 103, 92–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  144. Dick, D.M.; Smith, G.; Olausson, P.; Mitchell, S.H.; Leeman, R.F.; O’Malley, S.S.; Sher, K. Understanding the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to alcohol use disorders. Addict. Biol. 2010, 15, 217–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  145. Leeman, R.F.; Patock-Peckham, J.A.; Potenza, M.N. Impaired control over alcohol use: An under-addressed risk factor for problem drinking in young adults? Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2012, 20, 92–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  146. Everitt, B.J.; Belin, D.; Economidou, D.; Pelloux, Y.; Dalley, J.W.; Robbins, T.W. Neural mechanisms underlying the vulnerability to develop compulsive drug-seeking habits and addiction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 2008, 363, 3125–3135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  147. Jentsch, J.D.; Pennington, Z.T. Reward, interrupted: Inhibitory control and its relevance to addictions. Neuropharmacology 2014, 76, 479–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  148. Begleiter, H.; Porjesz, B. What is inherited in the predisposition toward alcoholism? A proposed model. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 1999, 23, 1125–1135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  149. Schuckit, M.A.; Smith, T.L.; Kalmijn, J. The search for genes contributing to the low level of response to alcohol: Patterns of findings across studies. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2004, 28, 1449–1458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  150. Buckholtz, J.W.; Treadway, M.T.; Cowan, R.L.; Woodward, N.D.; Li, R.; Ansari, M.S.; Baldwin, R.M.; Schwartzman, A.N.; Shelby, E.S.; Smith, C.E.; et al. Dopaminergic network differences in human impulsivity. Science 2010, 329, 532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  151. Basar, K.; Sesia, T.; Groenewegen, H.; Steinbusch, H.W.; Visser-vandewalle, V.; Temel, Y. Nucleus accumbens and impulsivity. Prog. Neurobiol. 2010, 92, 533–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  152. Bevilacqua, L.; Doly, S.; Kaprio, J.; Yuan, Q.; Tikkanen, R.; Paunio, T.; Zhou, Z.; Wedenoja, J.; Maroteaux, L.; Diaz, S.; et al. A population-specific HTR2B stop codon predisposes to severe impulsivity. Nature 2010, 468, 1061–1066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  153. Goldman, D.; Oroszi, G.; Ducci, F. The genetics of addictions: Uncovering the genes. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2005, 6, 521–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  154. Wetherill, R.R.; Castro, N.; Squeglia, L.M.; Tapert, S.F. Atypical neural activity during inhibitory processing in substance-naive youth who later experience alcohol-induced blackouts. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013, 128, 243–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  155. Schulte, T.; Müller-Oehring, E.M.; Sullivan, E.V.; Pfefferbaum, A. Synchrony of corticostriatal-midbrain activation enables normal inhibitory control and conflict processing in recovering alcoholic men. Biol. Psychiatry 2012, 71, 269–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  156. Sher, K.J.; Grekin, E.R.; Williams, N.A. The development of alcohol use disorders. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 1, 493–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  157. Goldman, D. Gene × environment interactions in complex behavior: First, build a telescope. Biol. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 295–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  158. Dick, D.M.; Riley, B.; Kendler, K.S. Nature and nurture in neuropsychiatric genetics: Where do we stand? Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2010, 12, 7–23. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  159. Van der Zwaluw, C.S.; Engels, R.C. Gene-environment interactions and alcohol use and dependence: Current status and future challenges. Addiction 2009, 104, 907–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  160. Spanagel, R. Alcoholism: A systems approach from molecular physiology to addictive behavior. Physiol. Rev. 2009, 89, 649–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  161. Crews, F.T.; Vetreno, R.P.; Broadwater, M.A.; Robinson, D.L. Adolescent alcohol exposure persistently impacts adult neurobiology and behavior. Pharmacol. Rev. 2016, 68, 1074–1109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  162. Watson, T.D.; Sweeney, J.F.; Louis, H. Neurocognitive, psychological and behavioral correlates of binge drinking and use of alcohol with caffeinated beverages in college-aged adults. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 2014, 40, 58–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  163. Petit, G.; Kornreich, C.; Maurage, P.; Noel, X.; Letesson, C.; Verbanck, P.; Campanella, S. Early attentional modulation by alcohol-related cues in young binge drinkers: An event-related potentials study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2012, 123, 925–936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  164. Kimbrough, A.; Kim, S.; Cole, M.; Brennan, M.; George, O. Intermittent access to ethanol drinking facilitates the transition to excessive drinking after chronic intermittent ethanol vapor exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2017, 41, 1502–1509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. In a modified version of the Stroop task, congruent (CONG) trials consisted of color words displayed in matching colored font, incongruent (INCONG) stimuli had mismatched color words and font, and (READ) trials displayed the name of a color in a grey font. The task was to press a button corresponding to the color of the font or to the meaning of the words presented in gray. Trials were presented for 300 ms every 2 s in an optimally randomized manner.
Figure 1. In a modified version of the Stroop task, congruent (CONG) trials consisted of color words displayed in matching colored font, incongruent (INCONG) stimuli had mismatched color words and font, and (READ) trials displayed the name of a color in a grey font. The task was to press a button corresponding to the color of the font or to the meaning of the words presented in gray. Trials were presented for 300 ms every 2 s in an optimally randomized manner.
Brainsci 08 00009 g001
Figure 2. Lower accuracy and longer reaction times (RT) on INCONG trials confirm the Stroop interference effect. HED participants show longer RT time only on incongruent trials: * p < 0.05.
Figure 2. Lower accuracy and longer reaction times (RT) on INCONG trials confirm the Stroop interference effect. HED participants show longer RT time only on incongruent trials: * p < 0.05.
Brainsci 08 00009 g002
Figure 3. Lateral and medial views of group averaged activity for heavy (HED) and light episodic drinkers (LED) across task conditions at 4 s post stimulus. Voxel threshold was set at p = 0.001 with a false discovery rate (FDR) value of q = 0.003, t = 3.725. The color scale represents voxels with percent signal change coefficients between −0.3 and 0.3 that survive the threshold obtained from mixed-effects meta-analysis (MEMA).
Figure 3. Lateral and medial views of group averaged activity for heavy (HED) and light episodic drinkers (LED) across task conditions at 4 s post stimulus. Voxel threshold was set at p = 0.001 with a false discovery rate (FDR) value of q = 0.003, t = 3.725. The color scale represents voxels with percent signal change coefficients between −0.3 and 0.3 that survive the threshold obtained from mixed-effects meta-analysis (MEMA).
Brainsci 08 00009 g003
Figure 4. Group average time series expressed as % signal change of the blood oxygenated level dependent (BOLD) signal for the effects of Condition and Group. As shown in Table 2, greater BOLD contrast was observed in HEDs in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) and the thalamus bilaterally. Response conflict evoked greater activity in the frontal and parietal association cortices overall, but only marginally in the thalamus and not in the sensorimotor cortex (*** <0.0001; * <0.05; # <0.07).
Figure 4. Group average time series expressed as % signal change of the blood oxygenated level dependent (BOLD) signal for the effects of Condition and Group. As shown in Table 2, greater BOLD contrast was observed in HEDs in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) and the thalamus bilaterally. Response conflict evoked greater activity in the frontal and parietal association cortices overall, but only marginally in the thalamus and not in the sensorimotor cortex (*** <0.0001; * <0.05; # <0.07).
Brainsci 08 00009 g004
Table 1. Subjects characteristics (mean ± SD or n (%)) for heavy (HED) and light episodic drinking (LED) groups.
Table 1. Subjects characteristics (mean ± SD or n (%)) for heavy (HED) and light episodic drinking (LED) groups.
HED (n = 14)LED (n = 17)Stat. Value (U/chi a)p
% Female64.3%52.9%0.406 ans a
Age23.8 ± 3.425.5 ± 4.190.5ns
Family History Positive50%58.8%0.241 ans a
Education Years15.1 ± 1.915.9 ± 2.381ns
Undergraduate Grade Point Avg.3.40 ± 0.383.59 ± 0.3379ns
Stroop Task Difficulty Ratings3.07 ± 0.622.00 ± 0.78320.001
In the past 6 months
  No. of drinking days/wk2.67 ± 1.21.35 ± 0.96460.003
  No. of drinks/occasion5.25 ± 2.62.2 ± 1.2340.001
  No. of binge episodes15.21 ± 15.20.41 ± 0.70.000<0.001
  No. of alcohol-induced blackouts3.14 ± 2.90.18 ± 0.533<0.001
  Max No. of drinks in 24 h12.57 ± 9.83.41 ± 2.0611<0.001
Age Onset of Alcohol Use15.72 ± 1.4918.13 ± 2.1331.50.007
Severity of Alcoholism (SMAST)2.86 ± 2.40.76 ± 1.0470.003
Alc. Use Disorder Ident. Test (AUDIT)13.43 ± 5.803.82 ± 1.554.5<0.001
Motivation
  Enhancement2.33 ± 0.3331.69 ± 0.4626.5<0.001
  Social2.62 ± 0.4052.04 ± 0.4440.50.002
  Conformity1.41 ± 0.5641.43 ± 0.45104ns
  Coping1.67 ± 0.4721.19 ± 0.2937.50.002
Drinking Consequences (B-YAACQ)8.86 ± 6.112.76 ± 3.09470.004
Alcohol Craving (PACS)7.71 ± 4.532.53 ± 2.27360.001
Anxiety (GAD-7)2.07 ± 1.683.06 ± 2.63100ns
Depression (PHQ-9)3.07 ± 2.703.18 ± 2.96118ns
ADHD Symptomology (ASRS)1.64 ± 1.591.06 ± 1.3494.5ns
Impulsivity
  Attention2.06 ± 0.4781.83 ± 0.3975ns
  Motor2.23 ± 0.6321.78 ± 0.42610.036
  Non-Planning2.09 ± 0.7391.78 ± 0.4884.5ns
Sensation Seeking
  Experience4.23 ± 0.8063.97 ± 0.8690ns
  Boredom4.11 ± 0.6503.59 ± 0.7665.50.05
  Thrill3.69 ± 1.153.41 ± 1.2897.5ns
  Disinhibition3.81 ± 0.7222.94 ± 1.0354.50.018
WASI-II Percentile69.36 ± 21.1172.81 ± 21.4099.5ns
Eysenck Personality
  Neuroticism3.08 ± 2.293.71 ± 3.75110ns
  Psychoticism2.38 ± 2.262.18 ± 1.38110ns
  Extraversion9.46 ± 2.637.71 ± 3.5780.5ns
a Tested with Chi-Square; all other comparisons performed with the nonparametric Mann–Whitney U-test. SMAST: Short Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test; AUDIT: Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test; B-YAACQ: Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire; PACS: The Penn Alcohol Craving Scale; GAD7: Generalized Anxiety Disorder; EPQ: Patient Health Questionnaire; ASRS: Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale; WASI-II: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence.
Table 2. Statistical results for regions of interest (ROI) and their Talairach coordinates, trial condition effects, and INCONG vs. CONG & READ (INC > C + R) results for HEDs and LEDs.
Table 2. Statistical results for regions of interest (ROI) and their Talairach coordinates, trial condition effects, and INCONG vs. CONG & READ (INC > C + R) results for HEDs and LEDs.
AreaTal. Coord (LPI)m.e.Group (F2,58)m.e.Cond (F2,58)Cond x GroupHED: INC > C + R (F1,13)LED: INC > C + R (F1,16)
L. VLPFC−40.1, 25.8, 21.35.3 < 0.0556.3 < 0.0001ns17.6 < 0.001116.2 < 0.001
L. Thalamus−11.6, 20.6, 5.25.9 < 0.0514.6 < 0.001ns11.2 < 0.006ns
L. PreSMA−5.5, 4.7, 49.2ns63.9 < 0.0001ns29.4 < 0.00135.6 < 0.0001
L. PreCent. cx−50.6, 0.6, 31ns48.3 < 0.0001ns17.2 < 0.00136.6 < 0.0001
L. Parietal cx−37.5, −49.3, 45.5ns43.1 < 0.0001ns23.9 < 0.00118.8 < 0.001
L. Insula−28.7, 16.6, 9.9ns30.2 < 0.0001ns9.2 < 0.0126.3 < 0.0001
R. VLPFC42.1, 28.7, 19.417.7 < 0.000332.4 < 0.00014.4 < 0.0517.7 < 0.00112.1 < 0.01
R. Thalamus12.6, −22.2, 6.13.6 < 0.075.9 < 0.05nsnsns
R. Insula40.0, 5.4, 11.2ns6.9 < 0.05nsns5.86 < 0.05
R. PreCent. cx37.3, −26.4, 54.5nsnsnsnsns
R. Motor cx37.1, −21, 56.1nsnsnsnsns

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Molnar, S.M.; Beaton, L.E.; Happer, J.P.; Holcomb, L.A.; Huang, S.; Arienzo, D.; Marinkovic, K. Behavioral and Brain Activity Indices of Cognitive Control Deficits in Binge Drinkers. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8010009

AMA Style

Molnar SM, Beaton LE, Happer JP, Holcomb LA, Huang S, Arienzo D, Marinkovic K. Behavioral and Brain Activity Indices of Cognitive Control Deficits in Binge Drinkers. Brain Sciences. 2018; 8(1):9. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8010009

Chicago/Turabian Style

Molnar, Sean M., Lauren E. Beaton, Joseph P. Happer, Lee A. Holcomb, Siyuan Huang, Donatello Arienzo, and Ksenija Marinkovic. 2018. "Behavioral and Brain Activity Indices of Cognitive Control Deficits in Binge Drinkers" Brain Sciences 8, no. 1: 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8010009

APA Style

Molnar, S. M., Beaton, L. E., Happer, J. P., Holcomb, L. A., Huang, S., Arienzo, D., & Marinkovic, K. (2018). Behavioral and Brain Activity Indices of Cognitive Control Deficits in Binge Drinkers. Brain Sciences, 8(1), 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8010009

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop