Preconceptual Spectral and Temporal Cues as a Source of Meaning in Speech and Music
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Meaning Before Language and Music
3. Affective Sounds and Vocalizations
4. Calls, Vocalizations, and Human Music: Affectively-Based Sound–Meaning Relationships
5. Sound Communication, Emotion, and Affective Speech
6. Sound/Speech Understanding and the Gestural Approach
7. Sound Comprehension in Speech and Music: Spectral and Temporal Cues
8. Conclusions and Perspectives
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Trehub, S.E.; Hannon, E.E. Infant Music Perception: Domain-General or Domain-Specific Mechanisms? Cognition 2006, 100, 73–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zentner, M.R.; Kagan, J. Perception of Music by Infants. Nature 1996, 383, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cooper, R.P.; Aslin, R.N. Preference for Infant-Directed Speech in the First Month after Birth. Child. Dev. 1990, 61, 1584–1595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fernald, A.; Kuhl, P. Acoustic Determinants of Infant Preference for Motherese Speech. Infant Behav. Dev. 1987, 10, 279–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masataka, N. Preference for Infant-Directed Singing in 2-Day-Old Hearing Infants of Deaf Parents. Dev. Psychol. 1999, 35, 1001–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Papoušek, M. Intuitive Parenting: A Hidden Source of Musical Stimulation in Infancy. In Musical Beginnings Origins and Development of Musical Competence; Deliège, I., Sloboda, J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 88–112. [Google Scholar]
- Werker, J.F.; McLeod, P.J. Infant Preference for Both Male and Female Infant-Directed Talk: A Developmental Study of Attentional and Affective Responsiveness. Can. J. Psychol. Can. Psychol. 1989, 43, 230–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trainor, L.J. Infant Preferences for Infant-Directed versus Noninfant-Directed Playsongs and Lullabies. Infant Behav. Dev. 1996, 19, 83–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trainor, L.J.; Clark, E.D.; Huntley, A.; Adams, B.A. The Acoustic Basis of Preferences for Infant-Directed Singing. Infant Behav. Dev. 1997, 20, 383–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gorzelańczyk, E.J.; Podlipniak, P. Human Singing as a Form of Bio-Communication. Bio-Algorithms Med. Syst. 2011, 7, 79–83. [Google Scholar]
- Malloch, S.N. Mothers and Infants and Communicative Musicality. Music. Sci. 1999, 3, 29–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wermke, K.; Mende, W. Musical Elements in Human Infants’ Cries: In the Beginning Is the Melody. Music. Sci. 2009, 13, 151–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aubergé, V.; Cathiard, M. Can We Hear the Prosody of Smile? Speech Commun. 2003, 40, 87–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernald, A.; Mazzie, C. Prosody and Focus in Speech to Infants and Adults. Dev. Psychol. 1991, 27, 209–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panksepp, J.; Bernatzky, G. Emotional Sounds and the Brain: The Neuro-Affective Foundations of Musical Appreciation. Behav. Process. 2002, 60, 133–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fassbender, C. Infants’ Auditory Sensitivity towards Acoustic Parameters of Speech and Music. In Musical Beginnings Origins and Development of Musical Competence; Deliège, I., Sloboda, J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 56–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trehub, S.E. Musical Predispositions in Infancy: An Update. In The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music; Peretz, I., Zatorre, R.J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 2–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trehub, S.E.; Schellenberg, E.; Glenn Hill, D.S. The Origins of Music Perception and Cognition: A Developmental Perspective. In The Origins of Music Perception and Cognition: A Developmental Perspective; Deliège, I., Sloboda, J.A., Eds.; Psychology Press/Erlbaum (UK) Taylor & Francis: Hove, UK, 1997; pp. 103–128. [Google Scholar]
- Papoušek, H. Musicality in Infancy Research: Biological and Cultural Origins of Early Musicality. In Musical Beginnings Origins and Development of Musical Competence; Deliège, I., Sloboda, J.A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 37–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, S. The “Musilanguage” Model of Musical Evolution. In The Origins of Music; Wallin, N.L., Merker, B., Brown, S., Eds.; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 271–300. [Google Scholar]
- Fenk-Oczlon, G.; Fenk, A. Some Parallels between Language and Music from a Cognitive and Evolutionary Perspective. Music. Sci. 2009, 13, 201–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koelsch, S.; Kasper, E.; Sammler, D.; Schulze, K.; Gunter, T.; Friederici, A.D. Music, Language and Meaning: Brain Signatures of Semantic Processing. Nat. Neurosci. 2004, 7, 302–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Steinbeis, N.; Koelsch, S. Comparing the Processing of Music and Language Meaning Using EEG and FMRI Provides Evidence for Similar and Distinct Neural Representations. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, e2226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitch, W.T. The Biology and Evolution of Music: A Comparative Perspective. Cognition 2006, 100, 173–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hauser, M.D.; McDermott, J. The Evolution of the Music Faculty: A Comparative Perspective. Nat. Neurosci. 2003, 6, 663–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cross, I. Music, Cognition, Culture, and Evolution. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2001, 930, 28–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cross, I. Music, Mind and Evolution. Psychol. Music 2001, 29, 95–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cross, I. The Evolutionary Nature of Musical Meaning. Music. Sci. 2009, 13, 179–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huron, D. Is Music an Evolutionary Adaptation? In The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music; Peretz, I., Zatorre, R.J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 57–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDermott, J.; Hauser, M. The Origins of Music: Innateness, Uniqueness, and Evolution. Music Percept. 2005, 23, 29–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reybrouck, M. From Sound to Music: An Evolutionary Approach to Musical Semantics. Biosemiotics 2013, 6, 585–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tolbert, E. Music and Meaning: An Evolutionary Story. Psychol. Music 2001, 29, 84–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wallin, N.L. Biomusicology: Neurophysiological, Neuropsychological, and Evolutionary Perspectives on the Origins and Purposes of Music; Pendragon Press: Stuyvesant, NY, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, S.; Merker, B.; Wallin, N.L. An Introduction to Evolutionary Musicology. In The Origins of Music; Wallin, N.L., Merker, B., Brown, S., Eds.; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 3–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Drake, C.; Bertrand, D. The Quest for Universals in Temporal Processing in Music. In The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music; Peretz, I., Zatorre, R.J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 21–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geissmann, T. Gibbon Songs and Human Music from an Evolutionary Perspective. In The Origins of Music; Wallin, N.L., Merker, B., Brown, S., Eds.; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 103–123. [Google Scholar]
- Huron, D.B. The Other Semiotic Legacy of Charles Sanders Peirce: Ethology and Music-Related Emotion. In Music, Analysis, Experience. New Perspectives in Musical Semiotics; Meader, C., Reybrouck, M., Eds.; Leuven University Press: Leuven/Louvain, Belgium, 2015; pp. 185–208. [Google Scholar]
- Darwin, C. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex, 1st ed.; John Murray: London, UK, 1871. [Google Scholar]
- Ma, W.; Thompson, W.F. Human Emotions Track Changes in the Acoustic Environment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 14563–14568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fitch, W.T. On the Biology and Evolution of Music. Music Percept. 2006, 24, 85–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitch, W.T. Musical Protolanguage: Darwin’s Theory of Language Evolution Revisited. In Birdsong, Speech, and Language: Exploring the Evolution of Mind and Brain; Bolhuis, J.J., Everaert, M., Eds.; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2013; pp. 489–503. [Google Scholar]
- Mithen, S.J. The Singing Neanderthals: The Origins of Music, Language, Mind, and Body; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, W.F.; Marin, M.M.; Stewart, L. Reduced Sensitivity to Emotional Prosody in Congenital Amusia Rekindles the Musical Protolanguage Hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 19027–19032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hurford, J.R. The Origins of Meaning: Language in the Light of Evolution; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Hauser, M.D. The Evolution of Communication; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Fitch, W.T.; Zuberbühler, K. Primate Precursors to Human Language: Beyond Discontinuity. In Evolution of Emotional Communication: From Sounds in Nonhuman Mammals to Speech and Music in Man; Altenmüller, E., Schmidt, S., Zimmermann, E., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013; Volume 16, pp. 27–48. [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp, J. Affective Consciousness: Core Emotional Feelings in Animals and Humans. Conscious. Cogn. 2005, 14, 30–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bickerton, D. Adam’s Tongue: How Humans Made Language, How Language Made Humans; Hill and Wang: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Roederer, J.G. On the Concept of Information and Its Role in Nature. Entropy 2003, 5, 3–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hauser, M.D.; Konishi, M. The Design of Animal Communication; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Merker, B. Is There a Biology of Music? And Why Does It Matter? In Proceedings of the 5th Triennial ESCOM Conference; Kopiez, R., Lehmann, A.C., Wolther, I., Wolf, C., Eds.; Hanover University of Music and Drama: Hanover, Germany, 2003; pp. 402–405. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmermann, E.; Leliveld, L.; Schehka, S. Toward the Evolutionary Roots of Affective Prosody in Human Acoustic Communication: A Comparative Approach to Mammalian Voices. In Evolution of Emotional Communication: From Sounds in Nonhuman Mammals to Speech and Music in Man; Altenmüller, E., Schmidt, S., Zimmermann, E., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 116–132. [Google Scholar]
- Merker, B. Music: The Missing Humboldt System. Music. Sci. 2002, 6, 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, S.K.; Townsend, S.W.; Schel, A.M.; Wilke, C.; Wallace, E.K.; Cheng, L.; West, V.; Slocombe, K.E. Vocal Learning in the Functionally Referential Food Grunts of Chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 2015, 25, 495–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Browman, C.P.; Goldstein, L. Articulatory Phonology: An Overview. Phonetica 1992, 49, 155–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bird, S.; Klein, E. Phonological Events. J. Linguist. 1990, 26, 33–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Studdert-Kennedy, M. The Phoneme as a Perceptuomotor Structure. In Cognitive Science Series Language Perception and Production: Relationships Between Listening, Speaking, Reading and Writing; Allport, A., MacKay, D., Prinz, W., Scheerer, E., Eds.; Academic Press: London, UK, 1987; pp. 67–84. [Google Scholar]
- Frayer, D.W.; Nicolay, C. Fossil Evidence for the Origin of Speech Sounds. In The Origin of Music; Wallin, N.L., Merker, B., Brown, S., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 271–300. [Google Scholar]
- Clynes, M. Sentics: The Touch of Emotions; Anchor Press: Garden City, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Arias, P.; Belin, P.; Aucouturier, J.-J. Auditory Smiles Trigger Unconscious Facial Imitation. Curr. Biol. CB 2018, 28, R782–R783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scherer, K.R. Vocal Communication of Emotion: A Review of Research Paradigms. Speech Commun. 2003, 40, 227–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bänziger, T.; Hosoya, G.; Scherer, K.R. Path Models of Vocal Emotion Communication. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0136675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scherer, K.R.; Clark-Polner, E.; Mortillaro, M. In the Eye of the Beholder? Universality and Cultural Specificity in the Expression and Perception of Emotion. Int. J. Psychol. 2011, 46, 401–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scherer, K.R. Personality Inference from Voice Quality: The Loud Voice of Extroversion. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1978, 8, 467–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trehub, S.E.; Unyk, A.M.; Trainor, L.J. Maternal Singing in Cross-Cultural Perspective. Infant Behav. Dev. 1993, 16, 285–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malloch, S.; Trevarthen, C. The Human Nature of Music. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nadel, J.; Butterworth, G. Imitation in Infancy; Nadel, J., Butterworth, G., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Kyndrup, M. Mediality and Literature: Literature versus Literature. In Why Study Literature; Nielsen, H.S., Kraglund, R., Eds.; Aarhus University Press: Aarhus, Denmark, 2011; pp. 85–96. [Google Scholar]
- Wierød, L.M.L. Where to Draw the Line? In Music, Analysis, Experience; Maeder, C., Reybrouck, M., Eds.; New Perspectives in Musical Semiotics; Leuven University Press: Leuven/Louvain, Belgium, 2015; pp. 135–148. [Google Scholar]
- Ewens, G. Die Klänge Afrikas: Zeitgenössische Musik von Kairo Bis Kapstadt; Marino-Verlag: München, Germany, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Brandt, A.; Gebrian, M.; Slevc, L.R. Music and Early Language Acquisition. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rubin, D.C. Memory in Oral Traditions: The Cognitive Psychology of Epic, Ballads, and Counting-Out Rhymes; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Meyer, J. Typology and Acoustic Strategies of Whistled Languages: Phonetic Comparison and Perceptual Cues of Whistled Vowels. J. Int. Phon. Assoc. 2008, 38, 69–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rendall, D.; Owren, M.J. Vocalizations as Tools for Influencing the Affect and Behavior of Others. Handb. Behav. Neurosci. 2010, 19, 177–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gil-da-Costa, R.; Braun, A.; Lopes, M.; Hauser, M.D.; Carson, R.E.; Herscovitch, P.; Martin, A. Toward an Evolutionary Perspective on Conceptual Representation: Species-Specific Calls Activate Visual and Affective Processing Systems in the Macaque. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2004, 101, 17516–17521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Owings, D.H.; Morton, E.S. Animal Vocal Communication: A New Approach; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Herzog, M.; Hopf, S. Behavioral Responses to Species-Specific Warning Calls in Infant Squirrel Monkeys Reared in Social Isolation. Am. J. Primatol. 1984, 7, 99–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seyfarth, R.M.; Cheney, D.L.; Marler, P. Monkey Responses to Three Different Alarm Calls: Evidence of Predator Classification and Semantic Communication. Science 1980, 210, 801–803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huron, D.B. Voice Leading: The Science Behind a Musical Art; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Owren, M.J.; Dieter, J.A.; Seyfarth, R.M.; Cheney, D.L. Vocalizations of Rhesus (Macaca Mulatta) and Japanese (M. Fuscata) Macaques Cross-Fostered between Species Show Evidence of Only Limited Modification. Dev. Psychobiol. 1993, 26, 389–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hellbernd, N.; Sammler, D. Neural Bases of Social Communicative Intentions in Speech. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2018, 13, 604–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Owings, D.H.; Zeifman, D. Human Infant Crying as an Animal Communication System: Insights from an Assessment/Management Approach. In Evolution of Communication Systems: A Comparative Approach; Oller, D.K., Griebel, U., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2004; pp. 151–170. [Google Scholar]
- Chi, T.; Gao, Y.; Guyton, M.C.; Ru, P.; Shamma, S. Spectro-Temporal Modulation Transfer Functions and Speech Intelligibility. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1999, 106, 2719–2732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Theunissen, F.E.; Elie, J.E. Neural Processing of Natural Sounds. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014, 15, 355–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Arnal, L.H.; Flinker, A.; Kleinschmidt, A.; Giraud, A.-L.; Poeppel, D. Human Screams Occupy a Privileged Niche in the Communication Soundscape. Curr. Biol. CB 2015, 25, 2051–2056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rendall, D.; Owren, M.J.; Rodman, P.S. The Role of Vocal Tract Filtering in Identity Cueing in Rhesus Monkey (Macaca Mulatta) Vocalizations. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1998, 103, 602–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ghazanfar, A.A.; Turesson, H.K.; Maier, J.X.; Van Dinther, R.; Patterson, R.D.; Logothetis, N.K. Vocal-Tract Resonances as Indexical Cues in Rhesus Monkeys. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 425–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fischer, J.; Price, T. Meaning, Intention, and Inference in Primate Vocal Communication. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 82, 22–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fitch, W.T.; De Boer, B.; Mathur, N.; Ghazanfar, A.A. Monkey Vocal Tracts Are Speech-Ready. Sci. Adv. 2016, 2, e1600723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ackermann, H.; Hage, S.R.; Ziegler, W. Brain Mechanisms of Acoustic Communication in Humans and Nonhuman Primates: An Evolutionary Perspective. Behav. Brain Sci. 2014, 37, 529–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Owren, M.J.; Amoss, R.T.; Rendall, D. Two Organizing Principles of Vocal Production: Implications for Nonhuman and Human Primates. Am. J. Primatol. 2011, 73, 530–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scherer, K.R.; Johnstone, T.; Klasmeyer, G. Vocal Expression of Emotion. In Handbook of Affective Sciences; Series in affective science; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 433–456. [Google Scholar]
- Snowdon, C.T.; Teie, D. Affective Responses in Tamarins Elicited by Species-Specific Music. Biol. Lett. 2010, 6, 30–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lewis, P.A.; Critchley, H.D.; Rotshtein, P.; Dolan, R.J. Neural Correlates of Processing Valence and Arousal in Affective Words. Cereb. cortex 2007, 17, 742–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Altenmüller, E.; Schmidt, S.; Zimmermann, E. Evolution of Emotional Communication: From Sounds in Nonhuman Mammals to Speech and Music in Man; Altenmüller, E., Schmidt, S., Zimmermann, E., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, G.; Lewandowski, J.; Galantucci, B. How Communication Changes When We Cannot Mime the World: Experimental Evidence for the Effect of Iconicity on Combinatoriality. Cognition 2015, 141, 52–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Monaghan, P.; Shillcock, R.C.; Christiansen, M.H.; Kirby, S. How Arbitrary Is Language? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2014, 369, 20130299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aryani, A. Affective Iconicity in Language and Poetry, Freie Universität Berlin. Ph.D. Dissertation, 2018. Available online: https://refubium.fu-berlin.de/handle/fub188/22744 (accessed on 1 March 2019).
- Aryani, A.; Conrad, M.; Schmidtke, D.; Jacobs, A. Why “piss” Is Ruder than “Pee”? The Role of Sound in Affective Meaning Making. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0198430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aryani, A.; Hsu, C.-T.; Jacobs, A.M. The Sound of Words Evokes Affective Brain Responses. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ullrich, S.; Kotz, S.A.; Schmidtke, D.S.; Aryani, A.; Conrad, M. Phonological Iconicity Electrifies: An ERP Study on Affective Sound-to-Meaning Correspondences in German. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aryani, A.; Jacobs, A.M. Affective Congruence between Sound and Meaning of Words Facilitates Semantic Decision. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heffner, H.E.; Heffner, R.S. The Evolution of Mammalian Sound Localization. Acoust. Today 2016, 12, 20–27. [Google Scholar]
- Rendall, D.; Notman, H.; Owren, M.J. Asymmetries in the Individual Distinctiveness and Maternal Recognition of Infant Contact Calls and Distress Screams in Baboons. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2009, 125, 1792–1805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Monrad-Krohn, G.H. The Third Element of Speech: Prosody and Its Disorders. In Problems in Dynamic Neurology; Halpern, L., Ed.; Hebrew University Press: Jerusalem, Israel, 1963; pp. 101–117. [Google Scholar]
- Belin, P.; Fillion-Bilodeau, S.; Gosselin, F. The Montreal Affective Voices: A Validated Set of Nonverbal Affect Bursts for Research on Auditory Affective Processing. Behav. Res. Methods 2008, 40, 531–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scherer, K.R. Expression of Emotion in Voice and Music. J. Voice 1995, 9, 235–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schröder, M. Experimental Study of Affect Bursts. Speech Commun. 2003, 40, 99–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paquette, S.; Peretz, I.; Belin, P. The “Musical Emotional Bursts”: A Validated Set of Musical Affect Bursts to Investigate Auditory Affective Processing. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ekman, P.; Friesen, W.V.; Hager, J.C.; A Human Face (Firm). Facial Action Coding System; A Human Face: Salt Lake City, UT, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Hauser, M.D. The Sound and the Fury: Primate Vocalizations as Reflections of Emotion and Thought. In The Origins of Music; Wallin, N.L., Brown, S., Merker, B., Eds.; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 77–102. [Google Scholar]
- Bryant, G.A. Animal Signals and Emotion in Music: Coordinating Affect across Groups. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Seifert, U.; Verschure, P.F.M.J.; Arbib, M.A.; Cohen, A.J.; Fogassi, L.; Fritz, T.; Kuperberg, G.; Manzolli, J.; Rickard, N. Semantics of Internal and External Worlds. In Language, Music, and the Brain; Arbib, M.A., Ed.; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2013; pp. 203–230. [Google Scholar]
- Newman, J.D. Neural Circuits Underlying Crying and Cry Responding in Mammals. Behav. Brain Res. 2007, 182, 155–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Filippi, P.; Congdon, J.V.; Hoang, J.; Bowling, D.L.; Reber, S.A.; Pašukonis, A.; Hoeschele, M.; Ocklenburg, S.; De Boer, B.; Sturdy, C.B.; et al. Humans Recognize Emotional Arousal in Vocalizations across All Classes of Terrestrial Vertebrates: Evidence for Acoustic Universals. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2017, 284, 20170990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Köhler, W. Gestalt Psychology; An Introduction to New Concepts in Modern Psychology, Rev. ed.; Liveright: Oxford, UK, 1947. [Google Scholar]
- Fort, M.; Martin, A.; Peperkamp, S. Consonants Are More Important than Vowels in the Bouba-Kiki Effect. Lang. Speech 2015, 58, 247–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morton, E.S. On the Occurrence and Significance of Motivation-Structural Rules in Some Bird and Mammal Sounds. Am. Nat. 1977, 111, 855–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ohala, J. Signaling with the Eyebrows–Commentary on Huron, Dahl, and Johnson. Empir. Musicol. Rev. 2009, 4, 101–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sievers, B.; Polansky, L.; Casey, M.; Wheatley, T. Music and Movement Share a Dynamic Structure That Supports Universal Expressions of Emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 70–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Trevarthen, C. Musicality and the Intrinsic Motive Pulse: Evidence from Human Psychobiology and Infant Communication. Music. Sci. 1999, 3, 155–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnstone, T.; Scherer, K.R. Vocal Communication of Emotion. In Handbook of Emotions; Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J.M., Eds.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Massaro, D.W. Multimodal Emotion Perception: Analogous to Speech Processes. In Proceedings of the ISCA Workshop on Speech and Emotion, Newcastle, Northern Ireland, UK, 5–7 September 2000; pp. 114–121. [Google Scholar]
- Gallese, V.; Fadiga, L.; Fogassi, L.; Rizzolatti, G. Action Recognition in the Premotor Cortex. Brain 1996, 119, 593–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McGurk, H.; MacDonald, J. Hearing Lips and Seeing Voices. Nature 1976, 264, 746–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Merker, B. Synchronous Chorusing and Human Origins. In The Origins of Music; Wallin, N.L., Merker, B., Brown, S., Eds.; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 315–327. [Google Scholar]
- Stainsby, T.; Cross, I. The Perception of Pitch. In The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology; Hallam, S., Cross, I., Thaut, M., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 2008; Volume 1, pp. 47–58. [Google Scholar]
- Peng, S.-C.; Chatterjee, M.; Lu, N. Acoustic Cue Integration in Speech Intonation Recognition With Cochlear Implants. Trends Amplif. 2012, 16, 67–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Banse, R.; Scherer, K.R. Acoustic Profiles in Vocal Emotion Expression. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 614–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bradley, M.M.; Lang, P.J. Affective Reactions to Acoustic Stimuli. Psychophysiology 2000, 37, 204–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Xu, L.; Thompson, C.S.; Pfingst, B.E. Relative Contributions of Spectral and Temporal Cues for Phoneme Recognition. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2005, 117, 3255–3267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McAdams, S.; Giordano, B.L. The Perception of Musical Timbre. In The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology; Hallam, S., Cross, I., Thaut, M., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, NY, USA, 2008; Volume 1, pp. 72–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fu, Q.-J.; Zeng, F.-G.; Shannon, R.V.; Soli, S.D. Importance of Tonal Envelope Cues in Chinese Speech Recognition. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1998, 104, 505–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lehiste, I. The Function of Quantity in Finnish and Estonian. Language 1965, 41, 447–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suomi, K. Temporal Conspiracies for a Tonal End: Segmental Durations and Accentual F0 Movement in a Quantity Language. J. Phon. 2005, 33, 291–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Järvikivi, J.; Vainio, M.; Aalto, D. Real-Time Correlates of Phonological Quantity Reveal Unity of Tonal and Non-Tonal Languages. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e12603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Reybrouck, M.; Podlipniak, P. Preconceptual Spectral and Temporal Cues as a Source of Meaning in Speech and Music. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 53. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9030053
Reybrouck M, Podlipniak P. Preconceptual Spectral and Temporal Cues as a Source of Meaning in Speech and Music. Brain Sciences. 2019; 9(3):53. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9030053
Chicago/Turabian StyleReybrouck, Mark, and Piotr Podlipniak. 2019. "Preconceptual Spectral and Temporal Cues as a Source of Meaning in Speech and Music" Brain Sciences 9, no. 3: 53. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9030053
APA StyleReybrouck, M., & Podlipniak, P. (2019). Preconceptual Spectral and Temporal Cues as a Source of Meaning in Speech and Music. Brain Sciences, 9(3), 53. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9030053