Next Article in Journal
Corticotropin Releasing Factor Type 1 and 2 Receptor Signaling in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex Modulates Binge-Like Ethanol Consumption in C57BL/6J Mice
Next Article in Special Issue
Using Dual-Site Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation to Probe Connectivity between the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex and Ipsilateral Primary Motor Cortex in Humans
Previous Article in Journal
Domain-Specific Expectations in Music Segmentation
Previous Article in Special Issue
Effect of a Mental Stimulation Program of Computer and Internet Learning on Cognitive Functions and Wellbeing in Older Community-Dwelling Mexicans
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

White Matter Hyperintensity Regression: Comparison of Brain Atrophy and Cognitive Profiles with Progression and Stable Groups

by
Omar M. Al-Janabi
1,2,3,*,
Christopher E. Bauer
4,
Larry B. Goldstein
3,
Richard R. Murphy
3,
Ahmed A. Bahrani
1,5,
Charles D. Smith
1,3,
Donna M. Wilcock
1,6,
Brian T. Gold
1,4 and
Gregory A. Jicha
1,2,3,*
1
Sanders-Brown Center on Aging, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40536, USA
2
Department of Behavioral Science, College of Medicine, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40508, USA
3
Department of Neurology, College of Medicine, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40508, USA
4
Department of Neuroscience, College of Medicine, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40508, USA
5
Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Engineering, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40506, USA
6
Department of Physiology, Colleges of Medicine, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40536, USA
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2019, 9(7), 170; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9070170
Submission received: 28 May 2019 / Revised: 7 July 2019 / Accepted: 16 July 2019 / Published: 19 July 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Collection on Cognitive Neuroscience)

Abstract

:
Subcortical white matter hyperintensities (WMHs) in the aging population frequently represent vascular injury that may lead to cognitive impairment. WMH progression is well described, but the factors underlying WMH regression remain poorly understood. A sample of 351 participants from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative 2 (ADNI2) was explored who had WMH volumetric quantification, structural brain measures, and cognitive measures (memory and executive function) at baseline and after approximately 2 years. Selected participants were categorized into three groups based on WMH change over time, including those that demonstrated regression (n = 96; 25.5%), stability (n = 72; 19.1%), and progression (n = 209; 55.4%). There were no significant differences in age, education, sex, or cognitive status between groups. Analysis of variance demonstrated significant differences in atrophy between the progression and both regression (p = 0.004) and stable groups (p = 0.012). Memory assessments improved over time in the regression and stable groups but declined in the progression group (p = 0.003; p = 0.018). WMH regression is associated with decreased brain atrophy and improvement in memory performance over two years compared to those with WMH progression, in whom memory and brain atrophy worsened. These data suggest that WMHs are dynamic and associated with changes in atrophy and cognition.

1. Introduction

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) cerebral white matter hyperintensities (WMH) are non-specific, subcortical signal regions with prolonged T2 relaxation times compared to normal appearing white matter. They often appear conspicuously bright on T2-weighted images, particularly fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images where nearby bright cerebrospinal fluid intensity is suppressed. Late-life WMH are thought to largely represent cerebrovascular injury resulting from cerebral small vessel disease (cSVD) [1]. Such injury may lead to neuronal circuit dysfunction in affected areas that can be associated with vascular cognitive impairment and dementia. Several previous studies demonstrate an association between progression of WMH lesion volume and worsening cognitive impairment over time [2,3,4,5]. Although some studies have reported cases of WMH volume decrease over time (regression), the relationship of these changes with cognitive outcomes needs to be further evaluated [6,7]. To date, the only study that examined the relationship between the WMH regression and structural brain changes and cognitive function is from the RUN DMC cohort [8]. The latter study concluded that WMH regression group showed similar cognitive decline when compared to the WMH stable group. In addition, WMH progression group showed more cognitive decline when compared to the WMH stable group. Both WMH regression and stable groups showed no brain atrophy, suggesting a relatively benign prognosis of such groups.
It is possible that WMH regression reflects imaging or methodological confounders rather than a true biological phenomenon [9]. The use of standardized imaging sequences, scanners, and head coils across longitudinal visits, in addition to regular scanner calibration and identical processing techniques with uniform intensity corrections, such as that used in the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) study, are required to exclude analytic confounders and imaging artifacts that could be interpreted as representing WMH regression. ADNI was launched in 2003 as a public/private partnership designed to assess biological and clinical markers of Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) progression (http://adni.loni.usc.edu).
Biologically, it is possible that regression of WMH volume represents gliotic contraction and/or microvascular encephalomalacia resulting from static ischemic injury. If WMH regression represents such a biological phenomenon, it could be associated with greater global brain atrophy and unchanged or diminished cognitive function as occurs in many individuals post-stroke. Alternatively, WMH regression might reflect a longitudinal reduction in inflammatory changes and focal edema associated with cSVD, and if so, such changes should be associated with improved cognition despite a reduction in total brain volume. Lastly, it is possible that WMH regression represents healing or regenerative processes, that would be associated with reduced brain atrophy and improvement in cognitive performance. A theoretical framework to summarize the course and interpretation of such longitudinal changes are provided in Table 1.
The present study explored hypothetical mechanisms underlying WMH regression by examining cognitive and structural brain changes (i.e., brain atrophy) that occur in the setting of WMH volume regression through the analysis of longitudinal data collected as part of the ADNI 2 study.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Population

The study cohort was comprised of 351 ADNI 2 participants with normal cognition or mild cognitive impairment who had WMH quantification both at baseline and at 2-years +/− 3 months [10]. Inclusion criteria included the availability of complete demographic information, diagnostic information within 1 year of the T2 FLAIR scan used for WMH quantification at baseline, a T1-weighted (MPRAGE) image and FreeSurfer structural segmentation [11,12] within 1 year of the baseline FLAIR images, atrophy composite scores within 1 year of the baseline FLAIR images, and neurocognitive composite metrics for assessments of both memory [13] and executive function (EF) [14]. Potential ADNI participants not meeting these criteria were excluded. The memory composite included the Rey auditory verbal learning test (RAVLT), the cognitive component of the Alzheimer’s disease assessment scale (ADAS-Cog), the Folstein mini-mental state examination (MMSE), and Wechsler logical memory scale scores, whereas the executive function composite included the clock drawing test, trail making test, category fluency (animal and vegetable), Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised (WAIS-R) digit span and digit symbol tests. Details of ADNI clinical procedures and methodology are available elsewhere [15,16].

2.2. MRI Acquisition

FLAIR images were acquired according to standard ADNI protocols [15]. Scanning parameters were identical within participants between the two-time points. As a result, the reported WMH progression, stability, and regression measures fulfill the criteria needed to control for radiological and methodological confounders [17]. Further information can be found at http://adni.loni.usc.edu.

2.3. White Matter Hyperintensity Calculations

WMH volumes were calculated using the 4-tissue segmentation method [18]. Briefly, FLAIR images were co-registered to the T1 image, inhomogeneity-corrected and non-linearly aligned to a minimal deformation template (MDT) using the T1 transformation and the FMRIB Software Library (FSL) toolbox [19,20]. WMHs were estimated in MDT space using Bayesian probability and prior probability maps [18]. Binary WMH masks were created using a threshold of 3.5 SD above the mean. Volume of WMH were then calculated after back-transformation into native space. Gray, white, and CSF measurements were segmented using an expectation–maximization algorithm. WMH were ultimately subtracted from segmented white matter volume and reported in cubic millimeters.

2.4. Longitudinal Change Calculations

Changes in the various measures (Δ) were calculated by subtracting the baseline value from the value at the 2-year follow-up. Positive values indicate increases whereas negative values indicate decreases between the two time points. For WMH volume change, a negative value greater than 150 mm3 indicates regression (i.e., less WMH volume at follow-up) whereas a positive value greater than 150 mm3 indicates progression (i.e., more WMH volume at follow-up). Ten participants did not have follow-up neurocognitive measures and 1 did not have ventricular volume within the 2 years +/− 3-month range; these values were excluded from further analyses.

2.5. WMH Categorization

WMH net volume change between the baseline and the follow-up visits was used to calculate Δ WMH. Participants were initially grouped based on Δ WMH volume (regression, stable, and progression) using a percentile-based approach in which the percentile for no change was first identified (the 35th percentile). Although definitions based on standard deviations were initially considered, the notable leptokurtic distribution (Figure 1) of the data precluded such classification as only the most extreme values would be defined outside of the stable group. Ultimately, we defined the stable group as representing the +/− 10th percentile from the percentile of no change (25th–45th percentile of the study participant distribution). This corresponded to +/− Δ of 150 mm3 of WMH lesion volume. Participants classified in the regression group had reductions in WMH volume greater than 150 mm3 (more than 10 percentiles below the percentile of no change) and those classified in the progression group had an increase in WMH volume greater than 150 mm3 (more than 10 percentiles above the percentile of no change).

2.6. Atrophy Composite Calculation

To quantitate changes in brain and ventricular volume to estimate changes in global brain atrophy that may be related to WMH changes, gray and white matter segmentation volumes [18] from the four-tissue ADNI classification were combined to produce a total brain volume (cm3). However, this global measure alone does not specifically account for the volumetric changes in the ventricles, which is associated with both WMH changes [21] and AD-related neurodegenerative processes [22]. Therefore, the volume of the lateral ventricles (cm3) was estimated using FreeSurfer (University of California at San-Francisco, San-Francisco, CA, USA). Although these measures were assessed individually, the final measure was a composite that used the z-scores of brain volume subtracted from the z-scores from the lateral ventricles (higher value means less brain volume and/or larger ventricles). This was done as an attempt to account for both periventricular and subcortical atrophy (ventricular volume) as well as more cortical-based whole brain atrophy (brain volume).

2.7. Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (version 24, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). All dependent variables were approximately normally distributed and screened for outlier removal. Outliers were removed if they were 3 standard deviations outside the mean, specifically, three data points in Δ Memory, three in Δ EF, seven in Δ Ventricular Volume, and three in Δ Brain Volume; any outliers removed from the latter two were not entered into Δ Atrophy.
ANOVAs were used to compare age and education between WMH progression, regression, and stable groups. Pearson’s Chi Square was used to examine differences between groups in sex, marital status, and cognitive status (normal, mild cognitive impairment [MCI], and AD). Between-group differences in Δ atrophy composite, Δ ventricular volume, Δ brain volume, Δ memory, and Δ EF between the 3 groups were examined using ANCOVA with both age and sex as covariates. All statistics were considered significant at p < 0.05 and were false discovery rate (FDR) corrected using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure [23].

3. Results

Participants were 72 ± 7.2 years old (48.3% women), with 16.5 ± 2.6 years of education. Clinical cognitive status was normal in 40.2% and MCI in 59.8% (Table 2). There were no significant differences between regression, stable, and progression groups in age, education, sex, marital status, diagnosis, or Δ EF. Participants were classified as having WMH regression in 25.5%, no change in 19.1%, and WMH progression in 55.4%.
ANCOVA revealed that there were differences in Δ atrophy composite between groups (p = 0.005), particularly the progression and regression (p = 0.041) and the progression and stable groups (p = 0.003, Table 2). Longitudinally, memory improved in the regression and stable groups compared to progression (p = 0.036; p = 0.036 respectively, Table 3). There were no differences between any groups in Δ EF (p = 0.492, Table 3).

4. Discussion

These data indicate that WMH regression is associated with decreased brain atrophy and cognitive decline over a period of two years compared to WMH progression, which, consistent with prior reports, is associated with increased brain atrophy [24] and cognitive decline [25,26,27] (Table 2 and Figure 2).
WMH progression and regression have been reported in smaller cohorts [6,7,22,28], and a stable group has also been identified in other studies [7,8,22]. Several studies reported that WMH progression is associated with greater cognitive decline [4,5,8]; however, only one study reported on the potential associations between WMH regression and changes in global atrophy or cognitive test performance over time. The latter study showed that WMH regression was not associated with brain atrophy and was associated with cognitive decline similar to that seen in the WMH stable group, suggesting a relatively benign cognitive outcome [8].
Our data shows that, although there were significant differences in atrophy and cognition between those with WMH progression and regression, and between those with progressive and stable WMHs, there were no differences between those with WMH regression and stability. Both WMH progression and regression groups had larger baseline WMH volume than the stable group. This implies that at least some of those with WMH injury can return toward a normal functioning state.
We used the conventional net change in the WMH between the baseline and the follow-up visits together with the percentile approach to categorize the dynamic changes into three groups (progression, stable, and regression). We found that about 25.5% of study participants showed WMH regression, which is similar to the 11.3% and 21.5% reported by others [8,29]. In addition, 19.1% of the cohort remained stable and 55.4% showed WMH progression over two years. These data add to the existing literature suggesting that WMH volume change is a dynamic process.
We had several possible hypotheses regarding possible explanations for WMH regression. First, it could be due to imaging acquisition or methodological confounders such as the use of different scanners across visits, lack of scanner calibration, lack of standardized acquisition parameters, differences in post-acquisition processing techniques such as registration or segmentation pipelines, all of which may affect the accuracy of the WMH volume change calculation [9]. Use of the ADNI cohort and WMH volumes from the validated UCD four-tissue segmentation method for this study minimized such effects, suggesting instead that the observed dynamic nature of longitudinal WMH volume changes reflects a real biological phenomenon.
Biological causes for WMH regression could include gliotic scarring and microstructural encephalomalacia due to irreversible ischemic parenchymal injury, resolution of secondary inflammatory processes as a result of irreversible ischemic damage, or could instead reflect resolution or healing of reversible ischemic injury. The associations of WMH regression with measures of global cerebral atrophy and cognitive change over time would be expected to differ between these possibilities as highlighted in Table 1 and Figure 3.
If WMH regression were the result of gliotic scarring and microstructural encephalomalacia, such change could be associated with increased cerebral atrophy and stable or worsening cognition. In such a scenario, acute small subcortical strokes and lacunar infarcts, which mimic WMH, could account for WMH regression through the natural course of temporal evolution. Such permanent lesions eventually reduce in diameter over time, which could in part account for the observed WMH regression seen in this and other studies [22,30]. The current data, however, argue against this possibility as retraction and temporal evolution of ischemic lesions would be expected to result in increased rather than the decreased global brain atrophy observed in our study.
It is also possible that WMH regression may be due to resolution of inflammatory processes and focal edema related to irreversible ischemic injury. This hypothesis is supported by prior work demonstrating that parenchymal edema develops early during the WM vascular injury process [31]. Several studies supporting the concept of resolving edema as an explanation for WMH regression are based on observations in patients with cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL) or stroke [32]. If this possibility were responsible for WMH regression, measures of atrophy would increase in the setting of WMH regression as inflammatory edema resolved, and cognitive function would likely stabilize or improve. Although this is a possibility for some WMH lesions, the overall lack of increased atrophy argues against this mechanism as a primary determinant of WMH regression.
Lastly, it is possible that WMH regression is most closely related to resolution of reversible ischemic changes or to healing or regenerative processes after such injury. In such a scenario, WMH regression would be associated with either no change or increased brain volume, and would also be associated with improvement in cognitive test performance. This hypothesis is best supported by our observations.
Our data highlight the importance of considering WMH change over time as a dynamic process, which can be related to both positive and negative structural and cognitive outcomes. Future work should explore the many demographic, risk, and treatment variables that may have influenced longitudinal WMH change in our cohort of subjects, perhaps suggesting disease modifying strategies to prevent or reverse vascular cognitive impairment and dementia. Further longitudinal studies are also needed to determine if there is interchange between groups—alternation between WMH progression and regression states over time for example.
Limitations of the study involve time of observation in a chronic process, generalizability, and sensitivity of measures. The ADNI cohort has unique characteristics based on its inclusion/exclusion criteria, which include an emphasis on earlier stage disease including normal and MCI subjects as well as criteria that excludes those with significant cerebrovascular disease and/or CVD risk factors. As such, the cohort is not generalizable to epidemiologic and community-based cohorts that may have higher proportions of cognitively impaired individuals and/or those with increased cerebrovascular risk factors and/or WMH burden. We used global net change in the WMH between the baseline and the follow up visits, limiting our ability to detect regional change in WMH progression, stability, or regression compared to more detailed spatial location analyses used by others previously [22]. Finally, we only used two time points for demonstrating the dynamic nature of WMH; more sampling of the time evolution curve of WMH is desirable in future.
Despite these limitations, the present study allowed an exploration of cognitive and brain volume changes that are associated with dynamic WMH changes. The present data demonstrate that WMH regression is not a mere imaging artifact or artifact due to methodologic procedures, but rather represents a real biological phenomenon that may, at least in part, reflect recovery from and resolution of reversible cerebrovascular injury.

5. Conclusions

Our data indicate that WMH regression is present in at least a quarter of this ADNI sample. Those with WMH regression had a reduction in cognitive decline and decreased brain atrophy compared to those with WMH progression, with no differences in either atrophy or memory performance between the regression and stable groups. Identifying the underlying mechanisms that result in WMH regression, and how these mechanisms may be promoted to produce more favorable clinical outcomes should be further investigated.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2076-3425/9/7/170/s1, Table S1: Raw measurements (cubic centimeters) in the three groups for brain and ventricular volume.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, O.M.A., C.E.B., and G.A.J.; methodology, O.M.A. and C.E.B.; software, O.M.A. and C.E.B.; formal analysis, O.M.A. and C.E.B.; investigation, O.M.A., C.E.B., and G.A.J.; resources, G.A.J., B.T.G., and D.M.W.; data curation, O.M.A. and C.E.B.; writing—original draft preparation, O.M.A., C.E.B., C.D.S., L.B.G., B.T.G., and G.A.J.; writing—review and editing, A.A.B., R.R.M., L.B.G., B.T.G., C.D.S., D.M.W., and G.A.J.; supervision, B.T.G., C.D.S., and G.A.J.; project administration, O.M.A. and C.E.B.; funding acquisition, D.M.W., and G.A.J.

Funding

This research was funded by Data collection and sharing for this project was funded by the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) (National Institutes of Health Grant U01 AG024904) and DOD ADNI (Department of Defense award number W81XWH-12-2-0012). ADNI is funded by the National Institute on Aging, the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering, and through generous contributions from the following: AbbVie, Alzheimer’s Association; Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery Foundation; Araclon Biotech; BioClinica, Inc.; Biogen; Bristol-Myers Squibb Company; CereSpir, Inc.; Cogstate; Eisai Inc.; Elan Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; Eli Lilly and Company; EuroImmun; F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd and its affiliated company Genentech, Inc.; Fujirebio; GE Healthcare; IXICO Ltd.; Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy Research & Development, LLC.; Johnson & Johnson Pharmaceutical Research & Development LLC.; Lumosity; Lundbeck; Merck & Co., Inc.; Meso Scale Diagnostics, LLC.; NeuroRx Research; Neurotrack Technologies; Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation; Pfizer Inc.; Piramal Imaging; Servier; Takeda Pharmaceutical Company; and Transition Therapeutics. The Canadian Institutes of Health Research is providing funds to support ADNI clinical sites in Canada. Private sector contributions are facilitated by the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health (www.fnih.org). The grantee organization is the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, and the study is coordinated by the Alzheimer’s Therapeutic Research Institute at the University of Southern California. ADNI data are disseminated by the Laboratory for Neuro Imaging at the University of Southern California. This study was also funded by NIH P30 AG028383, UH2 NS100606, NR014189, and R01 AG042419.

Acknowledgments

We thank Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative for access to their data.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Pantoni, L.; Garcia, J.H. The significance of cerebral white matter abnormalities 100 years after binswanger’s report: A review. Stroke 1995, 26, 1293–1301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Schmidt, R.; Fazekas, F.; Kapeller, P.; Schmidt, H.; Hartung, H.-P. Mri white matter hyperintensities three-year follow-up of the austrian stroke prevention study. Neurology 1999, 53, 132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Veldink, J.H.; Scheltens, P.; Jonker, C.; Launer, L.J. Progression of cerebral white matter hyperintensities on mri is related to diastolic blood pressure. Neurology 1998, 51, 319–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Arvanitakis, Z.; Fleischman, D.A.; Arfanakis, K.; Leurgans, S.E.; Barnes, L.L.; Bennett, D.A. Association of white matter hyperintensities and gray matter volume with cognition in older individuals without cognitive impairment. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016, 221, 2135–2146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Van den Berg, E.; Geerlings, M.I.; Biessels, G.J.; Nederkoorn, P.J.; Kloppenborg, R.P. White matter hyperintensities and cognition in mild cognitive impairment and alzheimer’s disease: A domain-specific meta-analysis. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2018, 63, 515–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Sachdev, P.; Wen, W.; Chen, X.; Brodaty, H. Progression of white matter hyperintensities in elderly individuals over 3 years. Neurology 2007, 68, 214–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Maillard, P.; Fletcher, E.; Lockhart, S.N.; Roach, A.E.; Reed, B.; Mungas, D.; DeCarli, C.; Carmichael, O.T. White matter hyperintensities and their penumbra lie along a continuum of injury in the aging brain. Stroke 2014, 45, 1721–1726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Van Leijsen, E.M.; Bergkamp, M.I.; van Uden, I.W.; Cooijmans, S.; Ghafoorian, M.; van der Holst, H.M.; Norris, D.G.; Kessels, R.P.; Platel, B.; Tuladhar, A.M.; et al. Cognitive consequences of regression of cerebral small vessel disease. Eur. Stroke J. 2019, 4, 85–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Van Leijsen Esther, M.C.; de Leeuw, F.-E.; Tuladhar Anil, M. Disease progression and regression in sporadic small vessel disease–insights from neuroimaging. Clin. Sci. 2017, 131, 1191–1206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Schwarz, C.; Fletcher, E.; DeCarli, C.; Carmichael, O. Fully-automated white matter hyperintensity detection with anatomical prior knowledge and without flair. Inf. Process. Med. Imaging 2009, 21, 239–251. [Google Scholar]
  11. Fischl, B.; Salat, D.H.; Busa, E.; Albert, M.; Dieterich, M.; Haselgrove, C.; Van Der Kouwe, A.; Killiany, R.; Kennedy, D.; Klaveness, S. Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 2002, 33, 341–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Fischl, B.; Salat, D.H.; Van Der Kouwe, A.J.; Makris, N.; Ségonne, F.; Quinn, B.T.; Dale, A.M. Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage 2004, 23, S69–S84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Crane, P.K.; Carle, A.; Gibbons, L.E.; Insel, P.; Mackin, R.S.; Gross, A.; Jones, R.N.; Mukherjee, S.; Curtis, S.M.; Harvey, D. Development and assessment of a composite score for memory in the alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative (adni). Brain Imaging Behav. 2012, 6, 502–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Gibbons, L.E.; Carle, A.C.; Mackin, R.S.; Harvey, D.; Mukherjee, S.; Insel, P.; Curtis, S.M.; Mungas, D.; Crane, P.K.; Initiative, A.S.D.N. A composite score for executive functioning, validated in alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative (adni) participants with baseline mild cognitive impairment. Brain Imaging Behav. 2012, 6, 517–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Jack, C.R., Jr.; Bernstein, M.A.; Fox, N.C.; Thompson, P.; Alexander, G.; Harvey, D.; Borowski, B.; Britson, P.J.; LWhitwell, J.; Ward, C.; et al. The alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative (adni): Mri methods. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 2008, 27, 685–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Petersen, R.; Aisen, P.; Beckett, L.; Donohue, M.; Gamst, A.; Harvey, D.; Jack, C.; Jagust, W.; Shaw, L.; Toga, A. Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative (adni) clinical characterization. Neurology 2010, 74, 201–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Van Leijsen, E.M.C.; van Uden, I.W.M.; Ghafoorian, M.; Bergkamp, M.I.; Lohner, V.; Kooijmans, E.C.M.; van der Holst, H.M.; Tuladhar, A.M.; Norris, D.G.; van Dijk, E.J.; et al. Nonlinear temporal dynamics of cerebral small vessel disease: The RUN DMC study. Neurology 2017, 89, 1569–1577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. DeCarli, C.; Maillard, P.; Fletcher, E. Four Tissue Segmentation in Adni II. Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Department of Neurology and Neurology and Center for Neuroscience, University of California at Davis. Available online: https://www.alz.washington.edu/WEB/adni_proto.pdf (accessed on 18 December 2013).
  19. Jenkinson, M.; Beckmann, C.F.; Behrens, T.E.; Woolrich, M.W.; Smith, S.M. Fsl. Neuroimage 2012, 62, 782–790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Smith, S.M.; Jenkinson, M.; Woolrich, M.W.; Beckmann, C.F.; Behrens, T.E.; Johansen-Berg, H.; Bannister, P.R.; De Luca, M.; Drobnjak, I.; Flitney, D.E. Advances in functional and structural mr image analysis and implementation as fsl. Neuroimage 2004, 23, S208–S219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Coutu, J.-P.; Goldblatt, A.; Rosas, H.D.; Salat, D.H. White matter changes are associated with ventricular expansion in aging, mild cognitive impairment, and alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2016, 49, 329–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ramirez, J.; McNeely, A.A.; Berezuk, C.; Gao, F.; Black, S.E. Dynamic progression of white matter hyperintensities in alzheimer’s disease and normal aging: Results from the sunnybrook dementia study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016, 8, 62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Benjamini, Y.; Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Methodol. 1995, 57, 289–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. DeCarli, C.; Murphy, D.; Tranh, M.; Grady, C.; Haxby, J.; Gillette, J.; Salerno, J.; Gonzales-Aviles, A.; Honvitz, B.; Rapoport, S. The effect of white matter hyperintensity volume on brain structure, cognitive performance, and cerebral metabolism of glucose in 51 healthy adults. Neurology 1995, 45, 2077–2084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Cees De Groot, J.; De Leeuw, F.E.; Oudkerk, M.; Van Gijn, J.; Hofman, A.; Jolles, J.; Breteler, M.M. Cerebral white matter lesions and cognitive function: The rotterdam scan study. Ann. Neurol. 2000, 47, 145–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Silbert, L.C.; Nelson, C.; Howieson, D.B.; Moore, M.M.; Kaye, J.A. Impact of white matter hyperintensity volume progression on rate of cognitive and motor decline. Neurology 2008, 71, 108–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Longstreth, W.; Arnold, A.M.; Beauchamp, N.J.; Manolio, T.A.; Lefkowitz, D.; Jungreis, C.; Hirsch, C.H.; O’leary, D.H.; Furberg, C.D. Incidence, manifestations, and predictors of worsening white matter on serial cranial magnetic resonance imaging in the elderly: The cardiovascular health study. Stroke 2005, 36, 56–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Wardlaw, J.M.; Chappell, F.M.; Valdés Hernández, M.D.C.; Makin, S.D.J.; Staals, J.; Shuler, K.; Thrippleton, M.J.; Armitage, P.A.; Muñoz-Maniega, S.; Heye, A.K.; et al. White matter hyperintensity reduction and outcomes after minor stroke. Neurology 2017, 89, 1003–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Cho, A.H.; Kim, H.-R.; Kim, W.; Yang, D.W. White matter hyperintensity in ischemic stroke patients: It may regress over time. J. Stroke 2015, 17, 60–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Lammie, G.; Brannan, F.; Wardlaw, J. Incomplete lacunar infarction (type i b lacunes). Acta Neuropathol. 1998, 96, 163–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Wardlaw, J.M.; Valdés Hernández, M.C.; Muñoz-Maniega, S. What are white matter hyperintensities made of? Relevance to vascular cognitive impairment. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2015, 4, 001140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Yao, M.; Jouvent, E.; During, M.; Godin, O.; Herve, D.; Guichard, J.P.; Zhu, Y.C.; Gschwendtner, A.; Opherk, C.; Dichgans, M.; et al. Extensive white matter hyperintensities may increase brain volume in cerebral autosomal-dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy. Stroke 2012, 43, 3252–3257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. White matter hyperintensity distribution in the sample studied. (A) The true distribution of the data, showing notable leptokurtosis. Black arrows indicate standard deviation, demonstrating why standard deviation was not deemed an appropriate criterion for separating groups. (B) Divisions of the three white matter hyperintensity (WMH) groups. Visualization only.
Figure 1. White matter hyperintensity distribution in the sample studied. (A) The true distribution of the data, showing notable leptokurtosis. Black arrows indicate standard deviation, demonstrating why standard deviation was not deemed an appropriate criterion for separating groups. (B) Divisions of the three white matter hyperintensity (WMH) groups. Visualization only.
Brainsci 09 00170 g001
Figure 2. Descriptive figure showing the dynamic nature of the white matter hyperintensity changes over time and the associated change in brain volume and cognitive performance. The coronal image cartoon represents the brain and ventricle size (scale of change exaggerated for emphasis), while the battery represents memory performance (improved memory corresponding to more battery capacity and green). The first column represents a normal brain with no WMH and normal memory, the second and third columns the baseline and two-year follow up showing changes over the course of the study. The upper row represents WMH stability. Brain volume and memory remained stable in the follow up visit as shown in A2 and A3. The middle row illustrates the case of progression of WMH over time. In B3, the WMH region enlarges, brain volume decreases along with ventricular increases and memory declines compared to B2. Finally, the lower row represents cases of WMH regression over time. In C3, the WMH lesion volume shrinks, brain volume is maintained, and memory is improved when compared to C2.
Figure 2. Descriptive figure showing the dynamic nature of the white matter hyperintensity changes over time and the associated change in brain volume and cognitive performance. The coronal image cartoon represents the brain and ventricle size (scale of change exaggerated for emphasis), while the battery represents memory performance (improved memory corresponding to more battery capacity and green). The first column represents a normal brain with no WMH and normal memory, the second and third columns the baseline and two-year follow up showing changes over the course of the study. The upper row represents WMH stability. Brain volume and memory remained stable in the follow up visit as shown in A2 and A3. The middle row illustrates the case of progression of WMH over time. In B3, the WMH region enlarges, brain volume decreases along with ventricular increases and memory declines compared to B2. Finally, the lower row represents cases of WMH regression over time. In C3, the WMH lesion volume shrinks, brain volume is maintained, and memory is improved when compared to C2.
Brainsci 09 00170 g002
Figure 3. Conceptual descriptive figure showing the possible mechanisms that lead to white matter hyperintensity regression overtime and their associated effect on the brain volume and the cognitive performance (same conventions as in Figure 2). The upper row demonstrates the first potential mechanism of white matter hyperintensity (WMH) regression due to ischemic non-reversible injury. In A3, the WMH lesion contracts (black arrows) resulting in brain volume reduction compared to A2. Because the contraction is merely mechanical, memory performance is not changed compared to A2. The middle row illustrates inflammation as a potential cause of WMH regression. In B3, the edema resolves, the brain volume consequently decreases but memory is improved compared to B2. The difference between A3 and B3 is in expected memory performance. Lower row represents a third explanation of WMH regression, most consistent with our data. In C3, WMH lesion volume regresses, but brain volume and memory performance increase compared to C2. The difference between C3 and B3 is in brain atrophy, which is greater in B3.
Figure 3. Conceptual descriptive figure showing the possible mechanisms that lead to white matter hyperintensity regression overtime and their associated effect on the brain volume and the cognitive performance (same conventions as in Figure 2). The upper row demonstrates the first potential mechanism of white matter hyperintensity (WMH) regression due to ischemic non-reversible injury. In A3, the WMH lesion contracts (black arrows) resulting in brain volume reduction compared to A2. Because the contraction is merely mechanical, memory performance is not changed compared to A2. The middle row illustrates inflammation as a potential cause of WMH regression. In B3, the edema resolves, the brain volume consequently decreases but memory is improved compared to B2. The difference between A3 and B3 is in expected memory performance. Lower row represents a third explanation of WMH regression, most consistent with our data. In C3, WMH lesion volume regresses, but brain volume and memory performance increase compared to C2. The difference between C3 and B3 is in brain atrophy, which is greater in B3.
Brainsci 09 00170 g003
Table 1. Possible etiologies for cerebrovascular-related white matter hyperintensities that regress over time and the expected associations with cerebral atrophy and cognitive performance.
Table 1. Possible etiologies for cerebrovascular-related white matter hyperintensities that regress over time and the expected associations with cerebral atrophy and cognitive performance.
Possible Etiology for WMHPotential Cause of RegressionExpected Association with Cerebral AtrophyExpected Association with Cognitive Performance
Irreversible ischemic injuryGliotic contraction and microscopic encephalomalaciaIncreased atrophyNo change in cognitive performance
Inflammation associated with irreversible ischemic injuryResolution of inflammation and edema with restoration of normal function in the penumbraIncreased atrophy secondary to reduced inflammatory edemaImprovement in cognitive performance
Reversible ischemic injuryHealing processDecreased atrophyImprovement in cognitive performance
Table 2. Demographic, clinical, imaging, and change scores for subjects demonstrating progression, stability, and regression in white matter hyperintensity volumes. WMH is reported in cubic centimeters; Memory and executive function (EF) are reported in standardized scores, and Atrophy composite, brain volume, and ventricular volume are z-scored. Raw scores for brain and ventricular volume per group can be viewed in Table S1.
Table 2. Demographic, clinical, imaging, and change scores for subjects demonstrating progression, stability, and regression in white matter hyperintensity volumes. WMH is reported in cubic centimeters; Memory and executive function (EF) are reported in standardized scores, and Atrophy composite, brain volume, and ventricular volume are z-scored. Raw scores for brain and ventricular volume per group can be viewed in Table S1.
CriteriaProgressors
(n = 190)
Regressors
(n = 93)
Stable
(n = 68)
Significancen; (Progressors, Regressors, Stable)
Age; (mean, SD)72.2 (6.9)72.0 (7.3)70.3 (7.2)0.163190, 93, 68
Education; (mean, SD)16.4 (2.6)16.7 (1.6)16.7 (2.4)0.654190, 93, 68
Female; (n, %)98 (51.6)40 (43.0)34 (50.0)0.393190, 93, 68
Currently Married; (n, %)140 (73.7)72 (77.4)50 (73.5)0.764190, 93, 68
Cognitively Normal; (n, %)71 (37.4)38 (40.9)32 (47.1)0.371190, 93, 68
MCI (n, %)119 (62.6)55 (59.1)36 (52.9)0.371190, 93, 68
Baseline WMH; (mean, SD)6.9 (10.3)8.2 (10.6)1.9 (2.0)-190, 93, 68
Follow Up WMH; (mean, SD)8.7 (11.6)6.9 (9.5)1.9 (2.0)-190, 93, 68
Δ Memory; (mean, SD)−0.07 (0.35)0.02 (0.32)0.05 (0.32)-184, 89, 65
Δ EF; (mean, SD)−0.06 (0.59)0.00 (0.62)0.04 (0.56)-182, 90, 65
Δ Atrophy Comp; (mean, SD)0.19 (1.40)−0.17 (1.36)−0.52 (1.08)-179, 90, 63
Δ Brain Volume; (mean, SD)−0.07 (0.90)0.15 (1.0)0.18 (0.78)-187, 93, 68
Δ Ventricular Volume; (mean, SD)0.12 (0.94)−0.08 (0.69)−0.34 (0.65)-182, 90, 63
Abbreviations: n, number; SD, standard deviation; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; AD, Alzheimer’s disease; WMH, white matter hyperintensities; EF, executive function composite.
Table 3. ANCOVA results examining brain volume composite, memory change, and EF change in all three groups. Age and gender were used as covariates.
Table 3. ANCOVA results examining brain volume composite, memory change, and EF change in all three groups. Age and gender were used as covariates.
ANCOVAPost-Hoc Comparisonsp-Valuep-Value (FDR-Corrected)
Dependent Variable
Δ Memory 0.017 *0.028 *
Progression/Regression0.024 *0.036 *
Progression/Stable0.019 *0.036 *
Regression/Stable0.7660.766
Δ EF 0.4920.492
Progression/Regression0.3980.398
Progression/Stable0.2930.293
Regression/Stable0.7900.790
Δ Atrophy Composite 0.001 0.005 **
Progression/Regression0.027 *0.041 *
Progression/Stable0.001‡0.003 **
Regression/Stable0.1720.172
Δ Ventricular Volume 0.011 *0.028 *
Progression/Regression0.036 *0.054
Progression/Stable0.007 **0.021 *
Regression/Stable0.4430.433
Δ Brain Volume 0.0610.076
Progression/Regression0.0540.090
Progression/Stable0.0600.090
Regression/Stable0.8870.887
* indicates significant at p < 0.05. ** indicates significant at p ≤ 0.01. indicates significant at p ≤ 0.001. Abbreviations: WMH, white matter hyperintensities; EF, executive function composite.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Al-Janabi, O.M.; Bauer, C.E.; Goldstein, L.B.; Murphy, R.R.; Bahrani, A.A.; Smith, C.D.; Wilcock, D.M.; Gold, B.T.; Jicha, G.A. White Matter Hyperintensity Regression: Comparison of Brain Atrophy and Cognitive Profiles with Progression and Stable Groups. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 170. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9070170

AMA Style

Al-Janabi OM, Bauer CE, Goldstein LB, Murphy RR, Bahrani AA, Smith CD, Wilcock DM, Gold BT, Jicha GA. White Matter Hyperintensity Regression: Comparison of Brain Atrophy and Cognitive Profiles with Progression and Stable Groups. Brain Sciences. 2019; 9(7):170. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9070170

Chicago/Turabian Style

Al-Janabi, Omar M., Christopher E. Bauer, Larry B. Goldstein, Richard R. Murphy, Ahmed A. Bahrani, Charles D. Smith, Donna M. Wilcock, Brian T. Gold, and Gregory A. Jicha. 2019. "White Matter Hyperintensity Regression: Comparison of Brain Atrophy and Cognitive Profiles with Progression and Stable Groups" Brain Sciences 9, no. 7: 170. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9070170

APA Style

Al-Janabi, O. M., Bauer, C. E., Goldstein, L. B., Murphy, R. R., Bahrani, A. A., Smith, C. D., Wilcock, D. M., Gold, B. T., & Jicha, G. A. (2019). White Matter Hyperintensity Regression: Comparison of Brain Atrophy and Cognitive Profiles with Progression and Stable Groups. Brain Sciences, 9(7), 170. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9070170

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop