Next Article in Journal
A New Perspective on Assessing Cognition in Children through Estimating Shared Intentionality
Next Article in Special Issue
Student Characteristics, Institutional Factors, and Outcomes in Higher Education and Beyond: An Analysis of Standardized Test Scores and Other Factors at the Institutional Level with School Rankings and Salary
Previous Article in Journal
The Influence of Culture Capital, Social Security, and Living Conditions on Children’s Cognitive Ability: Evidence from 2018 China Family Panel Studies
Previous Article in Special Issue
Adaptive Intelligence: Intelligence Is Not a Personal Trait but Rather a Person × Task × Situation Interaction
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Role of Intelligence and Self-Concept for Teachers’ Competence

1
Centre for Educational Measurement (CEMO), University of Oslo, P.O. Box 1161, Blindern, 0318 Oslo, Norway
2
Department of Primary Mathematics Education, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Unter den Linden 6, 10099 Berlin, Germany
3
Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Habelschwerdter Allee 45, 14195 Berlin, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Intell. 2022, 10(2), 20; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020020
Submission received: 12 January 2022 / Revised: 12 March 2022 / Accepted: 24 March 2022 / Published: 28 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Intelligence, Competencies, and Learning)

Abstract

:
Research on intelligence and competence has developed widely independent of each other. The present paper aims at relating these traditions and at integrating the dominant models to fill gaps in the respective theories. We test the structural models derived from this integration in a series of confirmatory factor analyses and a latent moderated structural equations approach using teachers as an example. The data reveal that both fluid intelligence (gf) and domain-specific knowledge affect teachers’ ability to solve the domain-specific items. Teachers’ academic self-concept related to mathematics explains individual differences beyond gf. An interaction effect between gf and self-concept exists for teachers’ pedagogical content and general pedagogical knowledge, but not for their mathematics knowledge. This finding indicates that a positive self-concept cannot compensate for a lack of gf, but it supports the acquisition of domain-specific knowledge in case of high gf, probably because it facilitates overcoming challenges.

1. Introduction

The term intelligence is used in cognitive psychology to describe a variety of cognitive abilities at different hierarchical levels. More general abilities are assumed to underly more specific ones (Jensen 1998). In educational research in contrast, there are hardly any studies using the term intelligence. Instead, a competing terminology has become prominent that uses the term competence as the disposition underlying human behavior (see, e.g., Blömeke et al. 2015).
The aims of the present paper are two-fold: Firstly, we intend to relate the competence terminology prominent in education to the intelligence terminology prominent in cognitive psychology and to integrate the dominant models from these two traditions. Both academic disciplines could benefit from such an integration because it has the potential to fill gaps in the respective theories. Secondly, we intend to test the models we can derive from an integrated theory using teachers as an example.

2. Conceptual Framework

2.1. The Cattell–Horn–Carroll Theory of Cognitive Abilities

There are still some unresolved incongruencies between different traditions of modeling intelligence. Nevertheless, it is possible to summarize that the Cattell–Horn–Carroll (CHC) theory of cognitive abilities has evolved as a theory widely accepted in cognitive psychology and supported by validity evidence (Flanagan and Harrison 2012; Schneider and McGrew 2012). The CHC theory is based on Carroll’s (1993) three stratum model and Cattell’s (1963) and Horn’s (1968) extended gf-gc model. It describes the hierarchical organization of cognitive abilities where broader mental processes are placed on higher levels and more task-specific abilities on lower levels in the system of human cognitions. Cognitive abilities on higher levels are hypothesized to affect the acquisition of abilities on lower levels. Consequently, individuals with high achievement in one domain often show high achievement in other domains (Gustafsson and Undheim 1996).
There are a variety of cognitive abilities included in the CHC model. From an educational perspective, the most important distinction is between those that are difficult to change because they are, to a large extent, caused by biological and neurological factors and those that are mostly developed through opportunities to learn. Gf (fluid intelligence) is the ability to process information and to analytically distinguish between objects. Inductive and deductive reasoning are crucial indicators and regarded as particularly relevant when a situation is new, and automatic processes no longer work (Schneider and McGrew 2012). Gf is a dimension commonly regarded as relatively difficult to change, and if so, only over long time (Ritchie and Tucker-Drob 2018).
In contrast, gc (crystallized intelligence) includes a broad class of cognitive abilities mostly acquired through opportunities to learn. A recent development has been to split up this construct into cognitive abilities related to the broader culture and society on the one hand (called gc) and domain-specific knowledge (called gkn) on the other hand (Schneider and McGrew 2018). The latter dimension is an umbrella term for “specialized knowledge (knowledge not all members of a society are expected to have)” (Schneider and McGrew 2012, p. 123). It is acquired through “intensive systematic practice and training (over an extended period of time)” and is maintained through “regular practice and motivated effort (a.k.a., expertise)” (McGrew 2009, p. 6). Gkn includes a range of distinct cognitive abilities (Flanagan and Dixon 2014).
Another CHC dimension that is developed through opportunities to learn, already included in Cattell’s (1963) and Horn’s (1968) extended gf-gc model, is gq (quantitative knowledge). Gq describes the “knowledge related to mathematics” (Schneider and McGrew 2012, p. 127).
From an educational perspective, it is crucial to identify cognitive abilities that are mostly developed through opportunities to learn and to distinguish them from those that are more difficult to change. Only then can we design education in line with the effort needed to support human development. Moreover, only then we can evaluate the effects of education properly because the proportion of variance explained by general cognitive ability can be controlled for (McClelland 1973).
Disentangling teacher cognitions in such a way is urgently needed. The sample used in this study was drawn from the population of early childhood teachers because, to our knowledge, there has not yet been a study that has attempted this, even though the value of education has been repeatedly questioned for this group of teachers (Strauss 2005). Without clarifying the role of general cognitive abilities, it is almost impossible to evaluate to what extent such criticism is justified or how to design teacher education so that it supports the development of teacher competence.

2.2. Relation of the Competence Model to the CHC Model

In contrast to intelligence models that intend to identify and classify cognitive abilities in a top-down manner, competence models intend to identify and classify the dispositions underlying observable behavior. Spencer and Spencer (1993, p. 9) defined competence in this sense as “an underlying characteristic of an individual that is causally related to criterion-referenced effective and/or superior performance in a job or situation. Underlying characteristic means that the competency is a fairly deep and enduring part of a person’s personality.” One could describe this modeling approach as a bottom-up approach, since it first identifies the criterion and then tries to identify the traits involved.
The most important goal with modeling competencies in this way is to identify and describe those competence dimensions that are learnable and thus can be influenced by education. Koeppen et al. (2008) defined competence in this sense as “domain-specific cognitive dispositions that are required to successfully cope with certain situations or tasks, and that are acquired by learning processes” (p. 68). Blömeke et al. (2015) extended this understanding of competence as purely cognitive by suggesting considering dispositions as a multi-dimensional set of not only cognitive, but also affective-motivational-volitional characteristics. In their argumentation, they refer back to Snow’s (1994) concept of two pathways that contribute to achievement, namely a cognitive and a commitment pathway, and the broad range of studies that support such a concept. Students’ cognitive abilities are typically the strongest predictors of student achievement. However, including motivational and similar characteristics increases the predictive validity, although to a smaller extent, particularly with respect to teacher-set grades, but less so with respect to standardized test scores (Lavrijsen et al. 2021; Steinmayr et al. 2019). Academic self-concept proved to be the most influential construct in this context (ibid.).
The Blömeke et al. (2015) competence model further clarified that these cognitive and affective-motivational dispositions are domain-specific, but in a general way, beyond single tasks and situations within this domain. The dispositions are stable continuous traits that, in turn, underlie task- and situation-specific cognitive skills not organized in an academic-disciplinary way, as is knowledge, but along the specific demands of narrowly defined situations or tasks. Blömeke et al. (2015) propose regarding the transformation of dispositions into observable behavior as fully or partially mediated by task- and situation-specific cognitive skills.
From this description, it should be clear that there is strong overlap between the CHC and the Blömeke, Gustafsson, and Shavelson competence models. The cognitive dimension of competence is conceptualized similarly to gkn as domain-specific and learnable. It is highly specialized knowledge that has been acquired during a long process of education and requires regular practice. We are not the first ones to point out such an overlap of competence and intelligence models. Previously, Wilhelm and Nickolaus (2013) and Trapp et al. (2019) had done this, although they did not specify the overlap in detail.
The affective-motivational-volitional dimension of the Blömeke, Gustafsson, and Shavelson competence model overlaps with the investment traits as conceptualized by Ackerman (1996) and Ziegler et al. (2012). The investment theory has provided evidence for the existence of traits that support the development of certain cognitive abilities because these support “the tendency to seek out, engage in, enjoy and continuously pursue opportunities for effortful cognitive activities” (von Stumm et al. 2011, p. 225). Ackerman (1996) examined a set of personality and motivational traits and provided evidence for their effects on gc beyond the effects of gf. Ziegler et al. (2012) extended this research with the openness-fluid-crystallized-intelligence model by providing evidence for an interaction effect of gf and personality traits, in particular, openness. However, very few studies exist that test the interaction hypothesis (e.g., (Zhang and Ziegler 2015) with respect to the interaction of openness and gf; (Lechner et al. 2019) with respect to the interaction of openness, domain-specific interest, and gf).
The core differences between the intelligence and competence traditions are, first, the absence of general cognitive abilities that are difficult to change, in particular of gf, in the Blömeke, Gustafsson, and Shavelson model. Although this is a decision that was made intentionally, since the objective was to identify traits that can be influenced by education, this absence could play out in an unintended negative way. An evaluation of the effects of education could be hampered by hidden third-variable (e.g., gf) effects. Conceptually, it can be argued that gf is less relevant for experts because their specialized knowledge can only be gained through specific opportunities to learn during many years of education. However, specialized knowledge also represents cognitive traits where investments should be crucial, since we know from previous research that gf shows effects in all cognitive domains. Many studies and meta-analyses demonstrate, for example, that gf is able to explain substantial amounts of variance in domain-specific school achievement (e.g., Brunner 2008; Gustafsson 1994).
Second another core difference between the two traditions is the role of the criterion. Both the CHC and the competence models are structural theories that examine the dimensions and facets of the respective constructs. While no criterion per se is considered in the intelligence tradition, modeling competence first identifies the criterion in terms of human behavior and then examines the underlying dispositions.
Third, the investment theory has so far focused on selected personality and motivational traits. Self-related cognitions have not been included, even though they would be considered a dimension of competence models given that studies on predictors of student achievement point to the relevance of self-related cognitions when it comes to explaining individual differences (e.g., Marsh and Martin 2011). Most recently, Demetriou et al. (2019) examined relationships between school achievement, gf, and self-concept using a sample of 10- to 17-years-olds. They found a decreasing effect of gf on school achievement but an increasing effect of self-concept with age. Since we intend to examine the cognitive structure of teachers, this result is very relevant.

2.3. Teachers’ Competencies

Teachers acquire their competencies through a long process of schooling, teacher education, and professional development. This also applies to early childhood (EC) teachers used as a sample in this study (Dunekacke et al. 2021). Based on the seminal work by Shulman (1987), teachers’ professional knowledge can be modelled as a three-dimensional construct that includes content knowledge, pedagogical content knowledge, and general pedagogical knowledge. With respect to content, the present study focuses on the domain of mathematics and EC teachers’ task to support children’s mathematical learning in early childhood education. Their professional knowledge then includes mathematics content knowledge (MCK), mathematics pedagogical content knowledge (MPCK), and general pedagogical knowledge (GPK) (Dunekacke et al. 2021).
MCK is the knowledge about numbers, sets, and operations; shape, space, and change; quantity, measurement, and relations; data, combinatorics, and chance (Bruns et al. 2021; Dunekacke et al. 2015a). In the case of EC teachers, this dimension has strong conceptual overlap with gq, since the level of mathematics does not exceed the level of mathematics learned during schooling. There are rarely opportunities to learn mathematics in EC teacher education that go beyond this level of mathematics. However, note that in case of other teacher groups, for example those trained for teaching mathematics in upper-secondary schools, MCK acquired during teacher education would include elements of university mathematics. This type of MCK would have to be classified as gkn because it is highly specialized mathematical knowledge which we cannot expect that all or even many members of a society have.
The MPCK of EC teachers is the knowledge of how to diagnose children’s developmental state in mathematics and how to design an informal learning environment that supports the mathematical learning of children between age 3 and 6 (Dunekacke et al. 2015b; Lee 2010; Torbeyns et al. 2020). GPK includes general foundations from educational theory, psychology, and instructional research related to early childhood and learning processes of 3- to 6-year-olds (Blömeke et al. 2017; Malva et al. 2019).
This framework, the alignment of the framework and its measures, as well as the inferences to be drawn from these measures have been validated in a range of studies (Blömeke et al. 2017; Dunekacke et al. 2015a, 2015b; Jenßen et al. 2015a, 2015b; Blömeke et al. 2022). The data for these studies were collected in Germany. However, the framework also reflects the professional tasks and opportunities to learn of EC teachers in other countries (e.g., Clements et al. 2004; NAEYC 2009).
As both the Blömeke, Gustafsson, and Shavelson model and the investment theory indicate, a purely knowledge-based approach to explaining individual differences in EC teachers’ gq (MCK) and gkn (MPCK and GPK) may be limited given the long educational process underlying the development of these knowledge dimensions. Academic self-concept can be defined as an individual’s belief about their abilities. Such self-related cognitions have been found to be relevant for a broad range of developmental and educational outcomes (Marsh et al. 2016). According to Marsh (1990) and Bong and Skaalvik (2003), academic self-concept is a domain-specific construct and needs to be examined with scales specific for the cognitive construct under investigation. Modeling the dispositions underlying human behavior in such a way may represent a more holistic perspective, thus enabling the explanation of more variance.

2.4. The Structure of EC Teachers’ Competencies

Based on the theory described above, gf, gq/MCK, and gkn (MPCK and GPK), as well as academic self-concept, are core dispositions involved in EC teachers’ work of supporting children’s mathematical development. However, there is ambiguity with respect to their structure and hierarchy. Carroll (1993) had placed more specialized cognitive abilities on the third, or lowest, level of his taxonomy. In contrast, the most recent CHC theory places gf, gq, and gkn on the second level and narrower abilities on the level below (Flanagan and Dixon 2014).
Evidence for the relation of gf to the other constructs has been provided by comparing first-order models with hierarchical models. The latter typically fit better to the data (Demetriou et al. 2019). In one-dimensional first order models, g explains variance in all indicators without distinguishing between the different constructs. In correlated first-order models, specific constructs are distinguished but still placed on the same level. In hierarchical models, gf also influences the specific constructs and thus, indirectly, their indicators (Gignac 2008). In this case, the effects of gf are fully mediated by the specific factors (Yung et al. 1999). Alternatively, gf directly influences the indicators of the different specific constructs as part of a hierarchical bifactor (Holzinger and Swineford 1937) or nested-factor model (Gustafsson and Balke 1993).
The exact role of academic self-concept is also an open question. In an early study, Gose et al. (1980) provided evidence for the additional potential of self-concept to explain variance in reading, language, and mathematics student achievement beyond gf. Later on, a study by Schicke and Fagan (1994) supported this result with respect to several student cohorts at different grade levels. Further studies revealed similarly positive relations of self-concept to domain-specific achievement beyond the effects of gf (Lavrijsen et al. 2021; Steinmayr et al. 2019). In addition, Guo et al. (2016) pointed to a potential interaction of self-concept and gf.
Not surprisingly, given the absence of gf in competence models and corresponding research, the organization of EC teachers’ domain-specific cognitions and their relation to broader general cognitions can be regarded as a desideratum. An additional reason for this desideratum might be that standardized and validated assessments covering EC teachers’ professional knowledge are rare (European Commission et al. 2019). Therefore, as yet we have only limited research with respect to the structure of EC teachers’ competencies, restricted to a few knowledge dimensions and to preservice EC teachers still in teacher education. The same restrictions apply to other teacher groups (Kleickmann et al. 2013 examined preservice primary and secondary school teachers; Roloff et al. 2020 studied practicing secondary school teachers).
With respect to preservice EC teachers (n = 353), a study by Jenßen et al. (2019) revealed a strong impact of gf (in terms of verbal, numerical, and figural intelligence assessed with the screening version of I-S-T 2000R; Liepmann et al. 2012) on MCK and MPCK. Nevertheless, a nested model revealed that MCK and MPCK had a significant additional impact on the ability to solve the domain-specific items. The initially strong latent correlation between MCK and MPCK, if gf was not controlled for (r = .67), could largely be explained by gf modelled as a higher-order factor. The size of the correlation was reduced substantially in the nested model (r = .30). Moreover, as typical for more complex models, the nested-factor model fit better to the data than a pure g-factor model where all indicators loaded on one general factor g only. The nested model also explained more variance.
Blömeke and Jenßen (2016) used data from the same sample to examine the impact of gf on the relationship between preservice EC teachers’ MCK or MPCK, respectively, and their skill for perceiving specific mathematics-related situations. A latent regression of this perception skill on the two knowledge constructs without including gf revealed strong effects (MPCK: β = .64, MCK: β = .47). The decrease in the effects of MCK or MPCK, respectively, on the skill to perceive mathematics-related situations (MPCK: β = .42, MCK: β = −.08) in a nested model including gf revealed the relevance of the latter for the interplay between the constructs, particularly with respect to the relationship between MCK and the skill to perceive math-related situations. The nested models were additionally tested against second-order models. Differences in the results were negligible.
We have not found any study that examined the cognitive structure of practicing teachers or that included GPK. Moreover, potential investment traits, such as academic self-concept, have not yet been examined. Our study adds to the body of research in both respects.

2.5. Study Aims and Hypotheses

The first aim of the empirical portion of this paper is to clarify the role of gf for practicing EC teachers’ domain-specific knowledge. We test the common models against each other with the following hypotheses:
H1. 
There are strong positive latent correlations between MCK, MPCK, and GPK if gf is not controlled for.
H2. 
Gf is positively correlated with MCK, MPCK, and GPK (see Figure 1).
H3. 
A pure g model, where all indicators load on one factor (see Figure 2), reveals an inacceptable model fit because it does not take into account the differential effects of gf, MCK, MPCK, and GPK on EC teachers’ ability to solve the corresponding items.
H4. 
Gf explains variance in MCK, MPCK, and GPK, but there are positive (partial) correlations between MCK, MPCK, and GPK after controlling for gf in a bifactor (S-1) model (see Figure 3).
The second aim of the empirical portion of this paper is to clarify the role of academic self-concept for EC teachers’ ability to solve the domain-specific items:
H5. 
We hypothesize that EC teachers’ self-concept related to mathematics explains individual differences in teachers’ MCK, MPCK, and GPK beyond gf.
H6. 
Academic self-concept moderates the influence of gf on MCK, MPCK, and GPK (see Figure 4).

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Sample

Practicing EC teachers were the target population of the present study. 210 EC teachers took the test battery. They were recruited by contacting EC institutions from all municipalities in two German federal states (Berlin and Brandenburg) via email and asking for voluntary participation. Therefore, they do not constitute representative samples. EC teachers were surveyed online without any time limitation. If they desired, they could postpone the second part of the survey until the next day. The teachers were, on average, M = 28 years old (SD = 7.4; min–max = 21–57). The majority (84%) indicated female as gender, while 16% indicated male as gender. Almost all EC teachers in the sample (93%) spoke German as their first language. About one-third of the sample (34%) had been trained at EC colleges, while the other two-thirds had received their training at post-secondary schools. Their median grade in their last class of school mathematics (a variety of levels, depending on the school year and type of class) was a 3 on a scale from 1 (best) to 6 (worst), with 4 as the minimum passing grade. These characteristics satisfactorily reflect the heterogeneity of EC teachers in Germany.

3.2. Measures of EC Teachers’ Competencies

Similar to Zhang and Ziegler (2015), we used figural reasoning as an indicator of gf. This was assessed with the figural facet of the intelligence structure test on the basis of the Berlin Model of Intelligence Structure (BIS) (Jäger 1982). Validation studies revealed that the figural subtest of the Intelligence-Structure Test 2000R (Amthauer et al. 2001) provides test scores corresponding to visual-based thinking (Beauducel et al. 2001; Ziegler et al. 2012). The figural facet of the screening version (IST-Screening; Liepmann et al. 2012) consists of 20 items. A CFA applying the WLSMV estimator for categorical data revealed a good fit to the data and supported our one-dimensional assumption (χ2 = 191.0, df = 170, p = .13; RMSEA = .03, 90%CI[.00; .05]; CFI = .94). Scale reliability as indicated by McDonald’s omega for congeneric scales (McDonald 1999) was good (ω = .90, SE = .02). We could therefore build item parcels using a factorial approach by sequentially assigning items to three parcels based on factor loadings, starting with the highest loading. This procedure led to a perfectly identified congeneric measurement model with standardized factor loadings between .64 and .78, applying the MLR estimator.
With respect to each dimension of EC teachers’ professional knowledge, large item pools were developed in collaboration with academic and practical experts. Item selection was completed on the basis of both conceptual considerations (American Educational Research Association et al. 2014) and a series of cognitive labs, unstandardized pre-pilot, standardized pilot, and validation studies (Jenßen et al. 2015a, 2015b; Dunekacke et al. 2015a, 2015b). The resulting tests consisted of multiple-choice, bundled, and open-response items. In all cases, gender-neutral language was used to reduce the risk of stereotype threats (Cadinu et al. 2005). For item examples, see Appendix A.
MCK was assessed with 23 items covering the four subdimensions described above. McDonald’s omega was very good (ω = .94, SE = .01). A one-factor congeneric measurement showed a good approximative fit (χ2 = 377.5, df = 230, p < .001; RMSEA = .06, 90%CI[.05; .06]; CFI = .99, applying the WLSMV estimator), indicating that the 23 items are approximately unidimensional. Four parcels were built using a substantive approach by assigning five or six items, respectively, to parcels based on the subdimensions leading the test development. A one-factor congeneric measurement model showed a very good model fit (MLR estimator; χ2 = 0.4, df = 2, p = .82; RMSEA = .00, 90%CI[.00; .08]; CFI = 1.00). The standardized factor loadings were between .85 and .94.
The second facet was mathematics pedagogical content knowledge (MPCK). It was assessed with 35 items. McDonald’s omega was very good (ω = .95, SE = .01). A one-factor congeneric measurement model showed a good approximate fit (WLSMV estimator; χ2 = 620.1, df = 561, p < .05; RMSEA = .02, 90%CI[.01; .03]; CFI = .99). We built three parcels by assigning items to parcels based on their loadings. The parcels showed strong standardized loadings between .92 and .95 in a one-factor congeneric measurement model.
The third facet was general pedagogical knowledge (GPK). It was assessed with 30 items covering four subdimensions. McDonald’s omega was very good (ω = .95, SE = .01). A one-factor congeneric measurement model with all 30 items as observed variables showed a good approximate fit (WLSMV estimator; χ2 = 499.3, df = 405, p < .001; RMSEA = .03, 90%CI[.02; .04]; CFI = .99). We built four parcels based on a substantive approach by assigning items to parcels based on the subdimensions leading test development. A one-factor congeneric model with the four parcels as indicators showed a very good fit to the data (χ2 = 0.9, df = 2, p = .64; RMSEA = .00, 90%CI[.00; .11]; CFI = 1.00) with standardized factor loadings between .82 and .93.
EC teachers’ academic self-concept was assessed with three domain-specific items related to mathematics. The statements were formulated negatively to maximize variance. We recoded the negatively worded statements to positive ones to facilitate the interpretation. McDonald’s omega was good (ω = .90, SE = .02). A one-factor congeneric measurement model revealed standardized factor loadings between .82 and .93. The three items are (our translation): “Nobody is capable of everything. I just do not have any talent for math;” “Mathematics does not particularly suit me;” “When it comes to things in mathematics that I do not understand, I know from the start: I will never understand this.”
See Table 1 for correlations of all parcels.

3.3. Data Analysis

The hypotheses H1 and H2 were analyzed using a CFA with correlated first-order factors for each construct (e.g., Eid et al. 2017b). Hypothesis H3 was tested assuming a one-factor-congeneric measurement model for all observed variables. Hypothesis H4 was analyzed using a bifactor (S-1) model (Eid et al. 2017b; Eid et al. 2018) with the gf factor as a general reference factor and three correlated specific factors for MCK, MPCK, and GPK.
Hypothesis H5 was analyzed using a multivariate latent regression analysis with the factors of MCK, MPCK, and GPK as dependent variables and the factors of gf and self-concept as independent variables. In order to analyze hypothesis H6, this multivariate regression model was extended by including interactions between gf and self-concept in a latent moderated structural equations approach (LMS; Klein and Moosbrugger 2000). Moreover, in this case, a maximum likelihood estimator with robust standard errors was used by applying a numerical integration algorithm. As suggested in the literature, we included the main effects of all variables to avoid the confounding of the main and interaction effects or the possibility that changes in the scale could result in arbitrary estimates (Aiken and West 1991).
All analyses were carried out with the Mplus 8.4 software package (Muthén and Muthén 1998–2019) using the robust maximum likelihood estimator (MLR). Missing data existed on two constructs, gf and academic self-concept, because teachers had dropped out after the knowledge tests. The missingness was handled by using the full-information maximum likelihood (FIML) procedure implemented in Mplus. The FIML procedure provides unbiased parameter estimates and standard errors under the missing-at-random assumption (Enders and Bandalos 2001). Testing the missing-completely-at-random assumption with the background information available revealed that most variables did not display significant differences for teachers with full as compared to those with missing data. This applied to teachers’ age, the level of their school degree, and their cultural capital, as well as to the education background of their fathers and mothers. However, significant differences existed with respect to gender (Cohen’s d = −.20, CI = −.59–.19) in favor of female teachers showing fewer missing values and language background (Cohen’s d = .42, CI = .02–.81) in favor of teachers with German as the language spoken at home showing fewer missing values.

4. Results

4.1. Role of EC Teachers’ gf for Their Domain-Specific Knowledge

4.1.1. H1 and H2: Latent Correlations of MCK, MPCK, and GPK, as Well as gf

A CFA model with correlated first-order factors for gf, MCK, MPCK, and GPK showed a very good fit (χ2 = 79.2, df = 71, p = .24; RMSEA = .02, 90%CI[.00;.05]; CFI = 1.00). The model revealed strong latent correlations between the three dimensions of EC teachers’ knowledge (see Table 2) if gf is not controlled for. In particular, MPCK and GPK were empirically almost indistinguishable (r = .95). The two domains related to mathematics were less strongly, but still substantially correlated with each other (MCK/MPCK: r = .54; MCK/ GPK: r = .48). There were also high latent correlations between gf and the three knowledge dimensions. The correlation was particularly high for MCK (r = .84), and lower, but equally high for MPCK and GPK (r = .52). Hence, our analyses support hypothesis H1 and H2.1

4.1.2. H3: One-Dimensional Structure of EC Teachers’ Cognitive Abilities

In accordance with our hypothesis (H3), the data revealed an unacceptable model fit for the pure g-factor model where just one factor was hypothesized to underly all 14 observed variables, i.e., the indicators of gf, MCK, MPCK, GPK (χ2 = 898.5, df = 77, p < .001; RMSEA = .23, 90%CI[.21; .24]; CFI = .67). The standardized factor loadings varied between .37 for two of the gf parcels and >.90 for the three MPCK parcels. For the gf parcels, the variance explained by the underlying factor was very low (<0.2) and barely significant. The modification indices suggest many correlated error variables, in particular of indicators belonging to the same construct, showing that the structure is multidimensional. Regarding EC teachers’ cognitive abilities as one-dimensional is thus not supported by the data, which is in line with the well-fitting model with correlated first-order factors presented in Table 1, and our hypothesis H3.

4.1.3. H4: Hierarchical Structure of EC Teachers’ Cognitive Abilities: Bifactor (S-1) Model

The bifactor (S-1) model fit the data very well (χ2 = 70.3, df = 63, p = .25; RMSEA = .02, 90%CI[.00; .05] CFI = 1.00). The percentage of variance explained by the gf factor was very high for the indicators of MCK (between 54 and 64 percent), and lower, but still substantive, for the indicators of MPCK (between 20 and 24 percent) and PCK (between 13 and 21 percent). The correlations between the specific factors are partial correlations (after controlling for gf). These correlations (see Table 3) are smaller than the zero-order correlations presented in Table 2, and not statistically significant for MCK and GPK. Therefore, as a whole, H4 has to be rejected. However, it is noteworthy that the correlation between the two pedagogical knowledge dimensions is still very high. These results show that the general gf factor can explain, to a relatively large degree, the correlations between the content knowledge (MCK) and the pedagogical knowledge (MPCK, GPK), but not the correlations between the pedagogical knowledge dimensions.

4.2. Role of Academic Self-Concept

The second aim of the empirical portion of this paper was to examine the role of self-related cognitions for the ability to solve domain-specific items. In the first step, we extended the CFA model with correlated first-order variables presented in Table 2 by including a factor for self-concept. This model fit the data approximately well (χ2 = 145.3, df = 109, p < .05; RMSEA = .04, 90%CI[.02; .06]; CFI = .99). Self-concept is significantly correlated with all other constructs (see Table 4). Whereas there were only comparatively small correlations with MPCK and GPK, the correlations were stronger with respect to MCK and gf.
The multivariate latent regression model fit the data well (χ2 = 145.3, df = 109, p < .05; RMSEA = .04, 90%CI[.02; .06]; CFI = .99). In line with our hypothesis (H5), EC teachers’ academic self-concept explained individual differences in teachers’ MCK beyond gf (see Table 5). However, in contrast to our hypothesis, the regression coefficient was not significantly different from 0 for MPCK and GPK. Both independent variables together explained a substantive amount of variance in all dependent variables, but the variance explained in the dependent variables differed by construct: while the variance in MCK could, to a large extent, be explained by gf and self-concept (R2 = .76), the proportion was lower in the case of MPCK and GPK (R2 = .27 in both cases).
A latent moderated modeling approach including an interaction effect between gf and academic self-concept (see Figure 4) revealed no moderator effect with respect to MCK (see Table 6). This means that H6 must be rejected with respect to this dimension. In contrast, the interaction effect was significant for both MPCK and GPK. This result suggests that in the case of high gf, the effect of a positive self-concept is stronger than in the case of low gf.
In line with these results, the variance explained in MCK was not higher in the latent moderated approach than in the model without an interaction term, while it was substantially higher in the case of MPCK (R2 = .36) and GPK (R2 = .34).

5. Discussion

Confronting theories stemming from cognitive psychology and education may benefit both traditions. The CHC theory uses an established terminology and provides a broad picture of how to think about the structure of human cognitions (Schneider and McGrew 2012). A comparison with the Blömeke, Gustafsson, and Shavelson model of competence (2015) revealed that substantial conceptual overlap exists between gq and MCK on the one hand and gkn and MPCK and GPK on the other hand. However, from an educational point of view, the CHC theory neglects, to some extent, the final objective: explaining human behavior and identifying ways to influence it. The Blömeke, Gustafsson, and Shavelson model of competence, in contrast, emphasizes behavior as the criterion and relates the dispositions needed to show this behavior to the criterion. However, this model does not include broader general cognitive abilities, such as gf, beyond the domain-specific ones.
The latter is a challenge not many educators may have yet realized. Relations between achievement domains are at risk for appearing as excessively strong if a common underlying factor is neglected. The effects of opportunities to learn can hardly be evaluated properly if general cognitive abilities are not partialled out. In fact, our study revealed interesting insights with respect to the role of gf for the population of EC teachers. A bifactor (S-1) model fitted the data well, which means that both general cognitive ability and domain-specific knowledge influenced EC teachers’ ability to solve the domain-specific items. Competence models are therefore advised to include gf—not as a cognitive ability that can be influenced by teacher education to a larger extent, but as a necessary precondition to be considered when evaluating the effects of education.
The benefits but also the challenges of teacher education or professional development are otherwise at risk of remaining hidden, as the following example may reveal: to be able to use their knowledge in practice requires that the different dimensions of EC teachers’ professional knowledge be deeply integrated (Tirosh et al. 2011). In particular, the integration of MCK with the two pedagogical dimensions has been regarded as critical. At first glance, EC teacher education seems to succeed in this respect, given latent correlations around .50 both for MCK and MCPK and for MCK and GPK. However, it now turns out—as we have demonstrated in this article—that these relations were, to a large extent, caused by an underlying factor (gf), reducing estimates to around .20 once gf was controlled for. Thus, the initial correlations did not reflect integration. It then seems to be important to develop educational practices that have the potential to overcome the disintegration of MCK, MPCK, and GPK, in particular, since we know from previous research that a similar phenomenon of seemingly strong latent correlations also applies to pre-service EC teachers (Jenßen et al. 2019). The latent correlation between MCK and MPCK of this target population was at .67, if gf was not controlled for, but only at .30 otherwise.
The investment theory could also benefit from being confronted with models of competence. Defining a criterion and trying to identify underlying traits that support its development may expand the range of characteristics examined. Our data revealed, for example, that academic self-concept affects professional knowledge and can thus be regarded as an investment trait. An interesting, and in case of GPK, even surprising finding was that the interaction effect of gf and academic self-concept was significant for both MPCK and GPK, given that self-concept was domain-specifically operationalized. This finding indicates that a positive self-concept cannot compensate for a lack of gf, but that it contributes to gaining MPCK and GPK in the case of high gf. The underlying mechanism here is probably related to the general finding that a positive self-concept makes it easier to overcome challenges. Moreover, the result may indicate that domain-specific self-concept is related to a broader, more general type of academic self-concept, as pointed out by Marsh (1990).

6. Limitations

Our study was the first one that examined the cognitive structure of practicing EC teachers. It was also the first one that included academic self-concept as a potential investment trait and examined its interaction with gf. However, given that research on the relation of intelligence and competence is scarce and we only used one sample of EC teachers in Germany, it would be important to replicate our findings with other teacher samples, in particular from other countries and other domains (e.g., oral language or science) to avoid making too far-reaching conclusions. Mathematics is only one domain among others where teachers are expected to support children’s development. Relationships between competence and intelligence, as well as relationships between the different knowledge dimensions, could be different in the case of other domains or other groups of teachers, for example, those teaching in upper-secondary school. Testing whether our findings reflect domain- or age-specific particularities would provide a deeper understanding of the nature of teachers’ cognitive abilities. The same applies to testing the generalizability of our models across countries. Such studies should then also examine whether and which abilities are able to predict the intended long-term outcomes, such as child development.
Another limitation of our study was the operationalization of gf as figural reasoning. Although this has been done previously (Zhang and Ziegler 2015), a risk exists that such a narrow representation underestimates relations to constructs based on verbal stimuli (Beauducel et al. 2007) such as GPK. The bifactor (S-1) model revealed in the current operationalization a strong effect of gf on GPK. We cannot rule out the possibility that the effect would have been even stronger in case of a broader operationalization that included verbal intelligence. Since it was not possible for us, due to limited administration time, to include such a facet of intelligence, we cannot test this with our current dataset, and we encourage further studies on this relationship.

7. Conclusions

Beyond EC teachers, the cognitive structure of other teacher populations such as primary, lower- or upper-secondary teachers is largely an open question. Can we reasonably assume that their cognitive structure is similar to that of EC teachers? Differences in opportunities to learn are huge, starting from different entry requirements to different length and depth of domain-specific teacher education, with EC teachers having by far the lowest number of opportunities to learn, both during teacher education (Gasteiger et al. 2020) and professional development (Nasiopoulou et al. 2021). In that sense, one could even speculate that gf might be more relevant in case of EC teachers than of others—but this is an empirical question.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.B.; methodology, S.B. and M.E.; validation, L.J. and M.E.; formal analysis, S.B.; data curation, L.J.; writing—original draft preparation, S.B.; writing—review and editing, L.J. and M.E.; visualization, S.B.; project administration, L.J.; funding acquisition, L.J., M.E. and S.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript. We confirm that we have reported all measures, conditions, and data exclusions relevant for this study.

Funding

The current study is part of the Pro-KomMa project funded by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (FKZ 01PK15003A-C) and part of the funding initiative KoKoHs (Modeling and Measuring Competencies in Higher Education—Validation and Methodological Innovations).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical review was carried out as part of the application for funding process. Ethical approval was waived for this observational study since no interventions were carried out.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study that the data can be used for research purposes.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the authors due to privacy restrictions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Item examples from the knowledge tests.
Figure A1. Example item from the MCK test (translated).
Figure A1. Example item from the MCK test (translated).
Jintelligence 10 00020 g0a1
Figure A2. Example item from the MPCK test (translated).
Figure A2. Example item from the MPCK test (translated).
Jintelligence 10 00020 g0a2
Figure A3. Example item from the GPK assessment (translated).
Figure A3. Example item from the GPK assessment (translated).
Jintelligence 10 00020 g0a3

Note

1
Since MPCK and GPK represent conceptually different constructs, it is not appropriate to collapse these, despite very strong latent correlations. Moreover, a follow-up analysis revealed that collapsing is not supported from an empirical point of view because the fit of a uni-dimensional model was worse than a model with two distinct constructs.

References

  1. Ackerman, Phillip L. 1996. A theory of adult intellectual development: Process, personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence 22: 227–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Aiken, Leona S., and Stephen G. West. 1991. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  3. American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, and National Council on Measurement in Education. 2014. Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  4. Amthauer, Rudolf, Burkhard Brocke, Detlev Liepmann, and André Beauducel. 2001. Intelligenz-Struktur-Test 2000R (I-S-T 2000R). Göttingen: Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
  5. Beauducel, André, Burkhard Brocke, and Detlev Liepmann. 2001. Perspectives on fluid and crystallized intelligence: Facets for verbal, numerical, and figural intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences 30: 977–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Beauducel, André, Detlev Liepmann, Jörg Felfe, and Wim Nettelnstroth. 2007. The Impact of Different Measurement Models for Fluid and Crystallized Intelligence on the Correlation with Personality Traits. European Journal of Psychological Assessment 23: 71–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Blömeke, Sigrid, and Lars Jenßen. 2016. A question of validity: Clarifying the hierarchical nature of teacher cognition. In Cognitive Abilities and Educational Achievement: Measurement and Determinants: A Festschrift in Honor of Jan-Eric Gustafsson. Edited by M. Rosén, Kajsa Yang Hansen and Ulrika Wolff. Methodology of Educational Measurement and Assessment. Berlin: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  8. Blömeke, Sigrid, Armin Jentsch, Natalie Ross, Gabriele Kaiser, and Johannes König. 2022. Opening up the black box: Teacher competence, instructional quality, and students’ learning progress. Learning and Instruction 79: 101600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Blömeke, Sigrid, Jan-Eric Gustafsson, and Richard J. Shavelson. 2015. Beyond dichotomies: Competence viewed as a continuum. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology 223: 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Blömeke, Sigrid, Lars Jenßen, Marianne Grassmann, Simone Dunekacke, and Hartmut Wedekind. 2017. Process mediates structure: The relation between preschool teacher’s education and preschool teachers’ knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology 109: 338–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bong, Mimi, and Einar M. Skaalvik. 2003. Academic Self-Concept and Self-Efficacy: How Different Are They Really? Educational Psychology Review 15: 1–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Brunner, Martin. 2008. No g in education? Learning and Individual Differences 18: 152–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Bruns, Julia, Hedwig Gasteiger, and Carolin Strahl. 2021. Conceptualising and measuring domain-specific content knowledge of early childhood educators: A systematic review. Review of Education 9: 500–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cadinu, Mara, Anne Maass, Alessandra Rosabianca, and Jeff Kiesner. 2005. Why do women underperform under stereotype threat? Psychological Science 16: 572–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Carroll, John B. 1993. Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  16. Cattell, Raymond B. 1963. Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology 54: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Clements, Douglas H., Julie Sarama, and Ann-Marie DiBiase, eds. 2004. Engaging Young Children in Mathematics: Standards for Early Childhood Mathematics. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  18. Demetriou, Andreas, Smaragda Kazi, George Spanoudis, and Nikolaos Makris. 2019. Predicting school performance from cognitive ability, self-representation, and personality from primary school to senior high school. Intelligence 76: 101381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Dunekacke, Simone, Lars Jenßen, and Sigrid Blömeke. 2015a. Effects of mathematics content knowledge on pre-school teachers’ performance: A video-based assessment of perception and planning abilities in informal learning situations. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education 13: 267–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Dunekacke, Simone, Lars Jenßen, and Sigrid Blömeke. 2015b. Mathematikdidaktische Kompetenz von Erzieherinnen und Erziehern. Validierung des KomMa-Leistungsungstests durch die videogestützte Erhebung von Performanz. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik 61: 80–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Dunekacke, Simone, Lars Jenßen, and Sigrid Blömeke. 2021. The role of opportunities to learn in early childhood teacher education from two perspectives: A multilevel model. Zeitschrift fuer Erziehungswissenschaft 24: 1429–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Eid, Michael, Christian Geiser, Tobias Koch, and Moritz Heene. 2017. Anomalous results in G-factor models: Explanations and alternatives. Psychological Methods 22: 541–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Eid, Michael, Stefan Krumm, Tobias Koch, and J. Schulze. 2018. Bifactor models for predicting criteria by general and specific factors: Problems of nonidentifiability and alternative solutions. Journal of Intelligence 6: 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Enders, Craig K., and Deborah L. Bandalos. 2001. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling 8: 430–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. European Commission, EACEA, and Eurydice. 2019. Key Data on Early Childhood Education and Care in Europe. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union. [Google Scholar]
  26. Flanagan, Dawn P., and Patti L. Harrison, eds. 2012. Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues, 3rd ed. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  27. Flanagan, Dawn P., and Shauna G. Dixon. 2014. The Cattell-Horn-Carroll theory of cognitive abilities. In Encyclopedia of Special Education. Edited by Cecil R. Reynolds, Kimberly J. Vannest and Elaine Fletcher-Janzen. New York: Wiley Online Library. [Google Scholar]
  28. Gasteiger, Hedwig, Esther Brunner, and Ching-Shu Chen. 2020. Basic conditions of early mathematics education—A comparison between Germany, Taiwan and Switzerland. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education 19: 111–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Gignac, Gilles E. 2008. Higher-order models versus direct hierarchical models: G as superordinate or breadth factor? Psychology Science Quarterly 50: 21–43. [Google Scholar]
  30. Gose, Aileen, Sharon Wooden, and Douglas Muller. 1980. The relative potential of self-concept and intelligence as predictors of achievement. The Journal of Psychology 104: 279–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Guo, Jiesi, Benjamin Nagengast, Herbert W. Marsh, Augustin Kelava, Hanna Gaspard, Holger Brandt, Jenna Cambria, Barbara Flunger, Anna-Lena Dicke, Isabelle Häfner, and et al. 2016. Probing the unique contributions of self-concept, task values, and their interactions using multiple value facets and multiple academic outcomes. AERA Open 2: 12332858415626884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Gustafsson, Jan-Eric. 1994. Hierarchical models of intelligence and educational achievement. In Intelligence, Mind, and Reasoning: Structure and Development. Edited by Andreas Demetriou and A. Efklides. Amsterdam: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
  33. Gustafsson, Jan-Eric, and Gudrun Balke. 1993. General and specific abilities as predictors of school achievement. Multivariate Behavioral Research 28: 407–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Gustafson, Jan-Eric, and Johan Olav Undheim. 1996. Individual differences in cognitive functions. In Handbook of Educational Psychology. Edited by David C. Berliner and Robert C. Calfee. New York: Macmillan, pp. 186–242. [Google Scholar]
  35. Holzinger, Karl J., and Frances Swineford. 1937. The bi-factor method. Psychometrika 2: 42–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Horn, John. L. 1968. Organization of abilities and the development of intelligence. Psychological Review 75: 242–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jäger, Adolf O. 1982. Mehrmodale Klassifikation von Intelligenzleistungen: Experimentell kontrollierte Weiterentwicklung eines deskriptiven Intelligenzstrukturmodells. Diagnostica 28: 195–225. [Google Scholar]
  38. Jensen, Arthur R. 1998. The g factor. In The Science of Mental Ability. Westport: Praeger. [Google Scholar]
  39. Jenßen, Lars, Simone Dunekacke, and Sigrid Blömeke. 2015a. Qualitätssicherung in der Kompetenzforschung. Kompetenzen von Studierenden. Beiheft der Zeitschrift Für Pädagogik 61: 11–31. [Google Scholar]
  40. Jenßen, Lars, Simone Dunekacke, Jan-Eric Gustafsson, and Sigrid Blömeke. 2019. Intelligence and knowledge: The relationship between preschool teachers’ cognitive dispositions in the field of mathematics. Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft 22: 1313–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Jenßen, Lars, Simone Dunekacke, Michael Eid, and Sigrid Blömeke. 2015b. The relationship of mathematical competence and mathematics anxiety: An application of latent state-trait theory. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie 223: 31–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kleickmann, Thilo, Dirk Richter, Mareike Kunter, Jürgen Elsner, Michael Besser, Stefan Krauss, and Jürgen Baumert. 2013. Teachers’ content knowledge and pedagogical content knowledge: The role of structural differences in teacher education. Journal of Teacher Education 64: 90–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Klein, Andreas G., and Helfried Moosbrugger. 2000. Maximum likelihood estimation of latent interaction effects with the LMS method. Psychometrika 65: 457–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Koeppen, Karoline, Johannes Hartig, Eckhard Klieme, and Detlev Leutner. 2008. Current issues in competence modeling and assessment. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology 216: 61–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lavrijsen, Jeroen, Maarten Vansteenkiste, Michiel Boncquet, and Verschueren Karine. 2021. Does motivation predict changes in academic achievement beyond intelligence and personality? A multitheoretical perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Lechner, Clemens M., Ai Miyamoto, and Thomas Knopf. 2019. Should students be smart, curious, or both? Fluid intelligence, openness, and interest co-shape the acquisition of reading and math competence. Intelligence 76: 101378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lee, Joohi. 2010. Exploring kindergarten teachers’ pedagogical content knowledge of mathematics. International Journal of Early Childhood 42: 27–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Liepmann, Detlev, Andre Beauducel, Burkhard Brocke, and Wim Nettelnstroth. 2012. Intelligenz-Struktur-Test Screening (IST-Screening). Göttingen: Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
  49. Malva, Liina, Ali Leijen, and Aleksandar Baucal. 2019. Measuring pre-service teachers’ general pedagogical knowledge: What are the results telling? New Trends and Issues Proceedings on Humanities and Social Sciences 6: 135–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Marsh, Herbert W. 1990. The structure of academic self-concept: The Marsh/Shavelson model. Journal of Educational Psychology 82: 623–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Marsh, Herbert W., and Andrew J. Martin. 2011. Academic self-concept and academic achievement: Relations and causal ordering. British Journal of Educational Psychology 81: 59–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Marsh, Herbert W., Reinhard Pekrun, Stephanie Lichtenfeld, Jiesi Guo, A. Katrin Arens, and Kou Murayama. 2016. Breaking the doubleedged sword of effort/trying hard: Developmental equilibrium and longitudinal relations among effort, achievement, and academic self-concept. Developmental Psychology 52: 1273–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. McClelland, David C. 1973. Testing for competence rather than for “intelligence”. American Psychologist 28: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. McDonald, Roderick P. 1999. Test Theory: A Unified Treatment. Mahwah: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  55. McGrew, Kevin S. 2009. CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence 37: 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Muthén, Linda K., and Bengt O. Muthén. 1998–2019. Mplus User’s Guide, 8th ed. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
  57. NAEYC. 2009. Developmentally Appropriate Practice Childhood Programs Serving Children from Birth through Age 8: A Position Statement National Association for the Education of Young Children. Available online: https://www.naeyc.file/positions/PSDAP.pdf (accessed on 23 March 2022).
  58. Nasiopoulou, Panagiota, Pia Williams, and Annika Lantz-Andersson. 2021. Preschool teachers’ work with curriculum content areas in relation to their professional competence and group size in preschool: A mixed-methods analysis. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research 66: 533–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Ritchie, Stuart J., and Elliot M. Tucker-Drob. 2018. How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychological Science 29: 1358–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Roloff, Janina, Uta Klusmann, Oliver Lüdtke, and Ulrich Trautwein. 2020. The Predictive Validity of Teachers’ Personality, Cognitive and Academic Abilities at the End of High School on Instructional Quality in Germany: A Longitudinal Study. AERA Open 6: 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Schicke, Michelle C., and Thomas K. Fagan. 1994. Contributions of self-concept and intelligence to the prediction of academic achievement among grade 4, 6, and 8 students. Canadian Journal of School Psychology 10: 62–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Schneider, W. Joel, and Kevin S. McGrew. 2012. The Cattell-Horn-Carroll Model of Intelligence. In Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues, 3rd ed. Edited by Dawn P. Flanagan and P. L. Harrison. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 99–144. [Google Scholar]
  63. Schneider, W. Joel, and Kevin S. McGrew. 2018. The Cattell-Horn-Carroll theory of cognitive abilities. In Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests and Issues. Edited by Dawn P. Flanagan and Erin M. McDonough. New York: Guilford Press, vol. 4, pp. 73–130. [Google Scholar]
  64. Shulman, Lee. 1987. Knowledge and teaching: Foundations of the new reform. Harvard Educational Review 57: 1–22. [Google Scholar]
  65. Snow, Richard E. 1994. Abilities in academic tasks. In Mind in Context: Interactionist Perspectives on Human Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg and Richard K. Wagner. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  66. Spencer, Lyle M., and Signe M. Spencer. 1993. Competence at Work: Model for Superior Performance. New York: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
  67. Steinmayr, Ricarda, Anne F. Weidinger, Schwinger Malte, and Birgit Spinath. 2019. The importance of students’ motivation for their academic achievement: Replicating and extending previous findings. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 1730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Strauss, Sidney. 2005. Teaching as a natural cognitive ability: Implications for classroom practice and teacher education. In Developmental Psychology and Social Change. Edited by David Pillemer and Sheldon White. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 368–88. [Google Scholar]
  69. Tirosh, Dina, Pessia Tsamir, Esther Levenson, and Michal Tabach. 2011. From preschool teachers’ professional development to children’s knowledge: Comparing sets. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education 14: 113–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Torbeyns, Joke, Sandy Verbruggen, and Fien Depaepe. 2019. Pedagogical content knowledge in preservice preschool teachers and its association with opportunities to learn during teacher training. ZDM 52: 69–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Trapp, Stefanie, Sigrid Blömeke, and Matthias Ziegler. 2019. The openness-fluid-crystallized-intelligence (OFCI) model and the environmental enrichment hypothesis. Intelligence 73: 30–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. von Stumm, Sophie, Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic, and Phillip L. Ackerman. 2011. Re-visiting intelligence-personality associations: Vindicating intellectual investment. In Handbook of Individual Differences. Edited by Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic, Sophie von Stumm and Adrian Furnham. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 217–41. [Google Scholar]
  73. Wilhelm, Oliver, and Reinhold Nickolaus. 2013. Was grenzt das Kompetenzkonzept von etablierten Kategorien wie Fähigkeit, Fertigkeit oder Intelligenz ab? Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft 16: 23–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Yung, Yiu-Fai, David Thissen, and Lori D. McLeod. 1999. On the relationship between the higher-order factor model and the hierarchical factor model. Psychometrika 64: 113–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Zhang, Jing, and Matthias Ziegler. 2015. Interaction Effects between Openness and Fluid Intelligence Predicting Scholastic Performance. Journal of Intelligence 3: 91–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Ziegler, Matthias, Erik Danay, Moritz Heene, Jens Asendorpf, and Markus Bühner. 2012. Openness, fluid intelligence, and crystallized intelligence: Toward an intergrative model. Journal of Research in Personality 46: 173–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The latent correlation model (H2). Note. MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence.
Figure 1. The latent correlation model (H2). Note. MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence.
Jintelligence 10 00020 g001
Figure 2. The pure g model (H3). Note. MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence.
Figure 2. The pure g model (H3). Note. MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence.
Jintelligence 10 00020 g002
Figure 3. The bifactor (S-1) model (H4). Note. MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence.
Figure 3. The bifactor (S-1) model (H4). Note. MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence.
Jintelligence 10 00020 g003
Figure 4. The moderation model (H6). Note. MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence; Self = self-concept in mathematics; * = interaction.
Figure 4. The moderation model (H6). Note. MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence; Self = self-concept in mathematics; * = interaction.
Jintelligence 10 00020 g004
Table 1. Correlations between all parcels.
Table 1. Correlations between all parcels.
MCK1MCK2MCK3MCK4MPCK1MPCK2MPCK3GPK1GPK2GPK3GPK4IST1IST2IST3
MCK2.80
MCK3.87.79
MCK4.87.78.85
MPCK1.49.42.48.46
MPCK2.48.42.48.48.89
MPCK3.48.42.48.45.88.86
GPK1.42.34.44.43.81.82.82
GPK2.36.32.34.35.76.75.73.77
GPK3.36.30.37.38.74.78.78.80.69
GPK4.42.39.44.41.77.81.77.80.70.75
IST1.61.53.56.56.40.37.32.36.33.34.37
IST2.72.65.65.65.42.40.36.40.32.29.39.68
IST3.65.60.65.60.40.38.33.38.27.26.37.60.65
Self1.56.54.59.53.28.25.29.27.21.20.21.35.49.34
Self2.50.46.54.47.22.21.24.16.20.12.16.34.51.28
Self3.58.53.60.53.25.19.23.22.16.11.21.40.48.40
MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; IST = fluid intelligence; Self = academic self-concept.
Table 2. CFA model with correlated first-order factor: latent correlations and standard errors (in parentheses).
Table 2. CFA model with correlated first-order factor: latent correlations and standard errors (in parentheses).
ConstructMCKMPCKGPK
MPCK.54 (.06) ***
GPK.48 (.06) ***.95 (.02) ***
Gf.84 (.06) ***.52 (.08) ***.52 (.08) ***
MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence. ***: p < .001.
Table 3. Bifactor model: Latent partial correlations between the specific factors (standard errors in parentheses).
Table 3. Bifactor model: Latent partial correlations between the specific factors (standard errors in parentheses).
MCK–MPCKMPCK–GPKMCK–GPK
.26 (.12) *.94 (.02) ***.18 (.12)
MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence. *: p < .05, ***: p < .001.
Table 4. CFA model with correlated first-order factor: latent correlations and standard errors (in parentheses).
Table 4. CFA model with correlated first-order factor: latent correlations and standard errors (in parentheses).
MCKMPCKGPKgf
MPCK.54 (.06) ***
GPK.48 (.06) ***.95 (.02) ***
gf.84 (.06) ***.52 (.08) ***.52 (.08) ***
Self-concept.66 (.08) ***.30 (.09) **.26 (.09) **.57 (.09) ***
MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence. **: p < .01, ***: p < .001.
Table 5. Latent multivariate regression model: standardized regression coefficients for the effects of the independent variables gf and academic self-concept on MCK, MPCK, and GPK (standard errors in parentheses).
Table 5. Latent multivariate regression model: standardized regression coefficients for the effects of the independent variables gf and academic self-concept on MCK, MPCK, and GPK (standard errors in parentheses).
MCKMPCKGPK
Gf.69 (.10) ***.52 (.12) ***.54 (.12) ***
Self-concept.28 (.11) *.01 (.12)−.05 (.12)
MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence. *: p < .05, ***: p < .001.
Table 6. Latent moderated structural equation model: standardized regression coefficients for the effects of the independent variables gf and academic self-concept, and their interaction on MCK, MPCK, and GPK (standard errors in parentheses).
Table 6. Latent moderated structural equation model: standardized regression coefficients for the effects of the independent variables gf and academic self-concept, and their interaction on MCK, MPCK, and GPK (standard errors in parentheses).
MCKMPCKGPK
Gf.73 (.12) ***.55 (.14) ***.58 (.13) ***
Self-concept.21 (.13)−.14 (.14)−.17 (.13)
gf x self.03 (.06).30 (.07) ***.24 (.06) ***
MCK = mathematics content knowledge; MPCK = mathematics pedagogical content knowledge; GPK = general pedagogical knowledge; gf = fluid intelligence; self = self-concept. ***: p < .001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Blömeke, S.; Jenßen, L.; Eid, M. The Role of Intelligence and Self-Concept for Teachers’ Competence. J. Intell. 2022, 10, 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020020

AMA Style

Blömeke S, Jenßen L, Eid M. The Role of Intelligence and Self-Concept for Teachers’ Competence. Journal of Intelligence. 2022; 10(2):20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020020

Chicago/Turabian Style

Blömeke, Sigrid, Lars Jenßen, and Michael Eid. 2022. "The Role of Intelligence and Self-Concept for Teachers’ Competence" Journal of Intelligence 10, no. 2: 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020020

APA Style

Blömeke, S., Jenßen, L., & Eid, M. (2022). The Role of Intelligence and Self-Concept for Teachers’ Competence. Journal of Intelligence, 10(2), 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020020

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop