Beyond Individual Tests: Youths’ Cognitive Abilities, Basic Reading, and Writing
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Cognitive Influences on Basic Reading Skills
1.2. Academic Interrelations: Basic Reading and Writing Skills
1.3. Purpose of This Study
- Which cognitive–basic reading relations generalize across six intelligence and three achievement tests?
- Which cognitive–basic reading relations are moderated by youths’ age?
- What is the magnitude of effects between youths’ basic reading, spelling, and written expression skills?
- What is the directionality of effects between youths’ basic reading and spelling skills?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Measures
2.1.1. Cognitive Tests
2.1.2. Academic Achievement Tests
2.2. Linking the Samples
2.3. Data Analyses
2.3.1. Missing Data
2.3.2. Analysis Step 1: Invariance Tests
2.3.3. Analysis Step 2: Cross-Battery Cognitive–Basic Reading Models
2.3.4. Analysis Step 3: Age Moderation of Cognitive–Basic Reading Relations
2.3.5. Analysis Step 4: Cross-Battery Basic Reading and Writing Interrelations
2.3.6. Model Evaluation
3. Results
3.1. Cognitive–Basic Reading Models
3.2. Developmental Differences in Cognitive–Basic Reading Relations
3.3. Relations Between Children’s Basic Reading and Writing Skills
3.3.1. Basic Reading, Spelling, and Written Expression
3.3.2. Basic Reading–Spelling
4. Discussion
4.1. Cognitive–Basic Reading Relations and Age Moderation
4.2. Basic Reading–Writing Interrelations
4.2.1. Basic Reading, Spelling, and Written Expression
4.2.2. Basic Reading–Spelling
4.3. Limitations and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Ahmed, Yursa, Richard K. Wagner, and Danielle Lopez. 2014. Developmental relations between reading and writing at the word, sentence, and text levels: A latent change score analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology 106: 419–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, and National Council on Measurement in Education. 2014. Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing. Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. [Google Scholar]
- Andersen, Simon C., Mette V. Christensen, Helena S. Nielson, Mette K. Thomsen, Torkil Østerbye, and Meredith K. Rowe. 2018. How reading and writing support each other across a school year in primary school children. Contemporary Educational Psychology 55: 129–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arbuckle, James L. 2015. SPSS Amos 19.0 User’s Guide 2015. Armonk: IBM. [Google Scholar]
- Bellocchi, Stéphanie, Mathilde Muneaux, Andréa Huau, Yohana Lévêque, Marianne Jover, and Stéphanie Ducrot. 2017. Exploring the Link between Visual Perception, Visual-Motor Integration, and Reading in Normal Developing and Impaired Children using DTVP-2. Dyslexia 23: 296–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Benson, Nicholas F. 2008. Cattell-Horn-Carroll Cognitive Abilities and Reading Achievement. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 26: 27–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benson, Nicholas F., John H. Kranzler, and Randy G. Floyd. 2016. Examining the integrity of measurement of cognitive abilities in the prediction of achievement: Comparisons and contrasts across variables from higher-order and bifactor models. Journal of School Psychology 58: 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benson, Nicholas F., Randy G. Floyd, John H. Kranzler, Tanya L. Eckert, Sarah A. Fefer, and Grant B. Morgan. 2019. Test use and assessment practices of school psychologists in the United States: Findings from the 2017 national survey. Journal of School Psychology 72: 29–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berninger, Virginia W., and Lucile Chanquoy. 2012. What writing is and how it changes across early and middle childhood development: A multidisciplinary perspective. In Writing: A Mosaic of New Perspectives. Edited by E. L. Grigorenko, E. Mambrino and D. D. Raleigh: Preiss; London: Psychology Press, pp. 65–84. [Google Scholar]
- Breaux, Kristina C. 2010. WIAT-III Technical Manual with Adult Norms. London: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Caemmerer, Jacqueline M., Danika L. S. Maddocks, Timothy Z. Keith, and Matthew R. Reynolds. 2018. Effects of cognitive abilities on child and youth academic achievement: Evidence from the WISC-V and WIAT-III. Intelligence 68: 6–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caemmerer, Jacqueline M., Matthew R. Reynolds, and Timothy Z. Keith. 2023. Beyond individuaol tests: Youth’s cognitive abilities on their math and writing skills. Learning and Individual Differences 102: 102271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caemmerer, Jacqueline M., Stephanie R. Young, Danika L. S. Maddocks, Natalie R. Charamut, and Eunice Blemahdoo. 2024. Predicting achievement from WISC-V composites: Do cognitive-achievement relations vary based on general intelligence? Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 42: 390–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caemmerer, Jacqueline M., Timothy Z. Keith, and Matthew R. Reynolds. 2020. Beyond individual intelligence tests: Application of Cattell-Horn-Carroll theory. Intelligence 79: 101–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Canivez, Gary L., Marley W. Watkins, and Stefan C. Dombrowski. 2017. Structural validity of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fifth Edition: Confirmatory factor analyses with the 16 primary and secondary subtests. Psychological Assessment 29: 458–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Caravolas, Markéta, Charles Hulme, and Margaret J. Snowling. 2001. The foundations of spelling ability: Evidence from a 3 year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language 45: 751–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carroll, John B. 2003. The higher-stratum structure of cognitive abilities: Current evidence supports g and about ten broad factors. In The Scientific Study of General Intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen. Edited by H. Nyborg. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science, Oxford: Pergamon Press, pp. 5–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clemens, Nathan H., Eric L. Oslund, Leslie E. Simmons, and Deborah Simmons. 2014. Assessing spelling in kindergarten: Further comparison of scoring metrics and their relation to reading skills. Journal of School Psychology 52: 49–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Colom, Roberto, Sherif Karama, Rex E. Jung, and Richard J. Haier. 2010. Human intelligence and brain networks. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 12: 489–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cormier, Damien C., Okan Bulut, Kevin S. McGrew, and Jessica Frison. 2016. The role of Cattell–Horn–Carroll (CHC) cognitive abilities in predicting writing achievement during the school-age years. Psychology in the Schools 53: 787–803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deary, Ian J., Steve Strand, Pauline Smith, and Cres Fernandes. 2007. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence 35: 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elliott, Colin. 2007. Differential Abilities Scale (DAS-II) Manual, 2nd ed. New York: Harcourt Assessment Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Elliott, Colin D., James B. Hale, Catherine A. Fiorello, Cledicianne Dorvil, and Jaime Moldovan. 2010. Differential ability scales-II prediction of reading performance: Global scores are not enough. Psychology in the Schools 47: 698–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Enders, Craig K. 2022. Applied Missing Data Analysis: Second Edition. New York: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Flanagan, Dawn P. 2000. Wechsler-based CHC cross-battery assessment and reading achievement: Strengthening the validity of interpretations drawn from Wechsler test scores. School Psychology Quarterly 15: 295–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Floyd, Randy G., Kevin S. McGrew, and Jeffrey J. Evans. 2008. The relative contributions of the cattell-horn-Carroll cognitive abilities in explaining writing achievement during childhood and adolescence. Psychology in the Schools 45: 132–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Georgiou, George K., Minna Torppa, Karin Landerl, Alain Desrochers, George Manolitsis, Peter F. de Jong, and Rauno Parrila. 2020. Reading and spelling development across languages varying in orthographic consistency: Do their paths cross? Child Development 91: 266–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graham, John W., Bonnie J. Taylor, Allison E. Olchowski, and Patricio E. Cumsille. 2006. Planned missing data designs in psychological research. Psychological Methods 11: 323–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gustafsson, Jan-Eric. 1984. A unifying model for the structure of intellectual abilities. Intelligence 8: 179–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hajovsky, Daniel B., Christopher R. Niileksela, Sunnny C. Olsen, and Morgan K. Sekula. 2023. Do cognitive–achievement relations vary by general ability level? Journal of Intelligence 11: 177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hajovsky, Daniel, Matthew R. Reynolds, Randy G. Floyd, Joshua J. Turek, and Timothy Z. Keith. 2014. A multi-group investigation of latent cognitive abilities and reading achievement relations. School Psychology Review 43: 385–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hajovsky, Daniel B., Ethan F. Villeneuve, Matthew R. Reynolds, Christopher R. Niileksela, Benjamin A. Mason, and Nicholas J. Shudak. 2018. Cognitive ability influences on written expression: Evidence for develop mental and sex-based differences in school-age children. Journal of School Psychology 67: 104–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hand, Eric D., Christopher J. Lonigan, and Cynthia Puranik. 2022. Prediction of kindergarten and first-grade reading skills: Unique contributions of preschool writing and early-literacy skills. Reading and Writing 37: 25–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hu, Li-tze, and Peter M. Bentler. 1999. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling 6: 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Humphreys, Lloyd G. 1973. The misleading distinction between aptitude and achievement tests. In The Aptitude-Achievement Distinction. Proceedings of the Second CTB/McGraw-Hill Conference on Issues in Educational Measurement. Edited by D. R. Green. CTB. New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 262–85. [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman, Alan S., and Nadeen L. Kaufman. 2004. Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children—Second Edition: Technical Manual. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman, Scott B., Matthew R. Reynolds, Xin Liu, Alan S. Kaufman, and Kevin S. McGrew. 2012. Are cognitive g and academic achievement g on and the same g? An exploration on the Woodcock-Johnson and Kaufman tests. Intelligence 40: 123–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keith, Timothy Z. 2019. Multiple Regression and Beyond: An Introduction to Multiple Regression and Structural Equation Modeling, 3rd ed. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Kim, Young-Suk G., and Seo-Hyun Park. 2018. Unpacking pathways using the direct and indirect effects model of writing (DIEW) and the contributions of higher order cognitive skills to writing. Reading and Writing 32: 1319–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Young-Suk G., Cheahyung Park, and Younghee Park. 2015. Dimensions of discourse level oral language skills and their relation to reading comprehension and written composition: An exploratory study. Reading and Writing 28: 633–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martins, Margarida A., Ana Albuquerque, Liliana Salvador, and Cristina Silva. 2013. The impact of invented spelling on early spelling and reading. Journal of Writing Research 5: 215–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maxwell, Scott E., David A. Cole, and Melissa A. Mitchell. 2011. Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation: Partial and complete mediation under an autoregressive model. Multivariate Behavioral Research 46: 816–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mäki, Hanna S., Rinus Voeteen, Marja Vauras, and Elisa Poskiparta. 2001. Predicting writing skill development with word recognition and preschool readiness skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 14: 643–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McArdle, John J. 1994. Structural factor analysis experiments with incomplete data. Multivariate Behavioral Research 29: 409–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McGill, Ryan J. 2015. Interpretation of KABC-II scores: An evaluation of the incremental validity of Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) factor scores in predicting achievement. Psychological Assessment 27: 1417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McGrew, Kevin S., and Susan N. Knopik. 1993. The relationship between the WJ-R Gf-Gc cognitive clusters and writing achievement across the life-span. School Psychology Review 22: 687–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNeill, Brigid C., Gail Gillon, and Megan Gath. 2023. The relationship between early spelling and decoding. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 54: 981–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muthén, Linda K., and Bengt O. Muthén. 2017. Mplus User’s Guide, 8th ed. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
- Niileksela, Christopher R., Matthew R. Reynolds, Timothy Z. Keith, and Kevin S. McGrew. 2016. A special validity study of the WJ IV: Acting on evidence for specific abilities. In WJ IV Clinical Use and Interpretation: Scientist-Practitioner Perspectives. Edited by Dawn P. Flanagan and Vincent C. Alfonso. Cambridge: Academic Press, pp. 65–106. [Google Scholar]
- Oh, Hyeon-Joo, Joseph J. Glutting, Marley W. Watkins, Eric A. Youngstrom, and Paul A. McDermott. 2004. Correct interpretation of latent versus observed abilities: Implications from structural equation modeling applied to the WISC-III and WIAT linking sample. The Journal of Special Education 38: 159–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parkin, Jason R., Craig L. Frisby, and Ze Wang. 2020. Operationalizing the simple view of writing with the Wechsler individual achievement test. Contemporary School Psychology 24: 68–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peng, Peng, Tengfei Wang, Cuicui Wang, and Xin Lin. 2019. A meta-analysis on the relation between fluid intelligence and reading/mathematics: Effects of tasks, age, and social economics status. Psychological Bulletin 145: 189–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Preacher, Kristopher J. 2015. Advances in mediation analysis: A survey and synthesis of new developments. Annual Review of Psychology 66: 825–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rabin, Laura A., Emily Paolillo, and William B. Barr. 2016. Stability in test-usage practices of clinical neuropsychologists in the United States and Canada over a 10-year period: A follow-up survey of INS and NAN members. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists 31: 206–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reynolds, Matthew R., and Joshua J. Turek. 2012. A dynamic developmental link between verbal comprehension-knowledge (Gc) and reading comprehension: Verbal comprehension-knowledge drives positive change in reading comprehension. Journal of School Psychology 50: 841–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reynolds, Matthew R., Timothy Z. Keith, Dawn P. Flanagan, and Vincent C. Alfonso. 2013. A cross-battery, reference variable, confirmatory factor analytic investigation of the CHC taxonomy. Journal of School Psychology 51: 535–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ricketts, Jessie, Kate Nation, and Dorothy V. Bishop. 2007. Vocabulary is important for some, but not all reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading 11: 235–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roth, Bettina, Nicolas Becker, Sara Romeyke, Sarah Schäfer, Florian Domnick, and Frank M. Spinath. 2015. Intelligence and school grades: A meta-analysis. Intelligence 53: 118–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rubin, Donald B. 1987. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. Hoboken: John Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Schaars, Moniek M. H., Eliane Segers, and Ludo Verhoeven. 2017. Word decoding development during phonics instruction in children at risk for dyslexia. Dyslexia 23: 141–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schafer, Joseph L., and John W. Graham. 2002. Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods 7: 147–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, W. Joel, and Kevin S. McGrew. 2018. The Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory of intelligence. In Contemporary Intellectual Assessment, 4th ed. Edited by Dawn P. Flanagan and Erin M. McDonough. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 73–163. [Google Scholar]
- Shanahan, Timothy. 2006. Relations among oral language, reading, and writing development. In Handbook of Writing Research. Edited by Charles A. MacArthur, Steve Graham and Jill Fitzgerald. New York: The Guilford Press, pp. 171–83. [Google Scholar]
- Steacy, Laura M., Ashley A. Edwards, Jay G. Rueckl, Yaacov Petscher, and Donald L. Compton. 2021. Modeling and visualizing the codevelopment of word and nonword reading in children from first through fourth grade: Informing developmental trajectories of children with dyslexia. Child Development 92: e252–e269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strattman, Kathy, and Barbara. W. Hodson. 2005. Variables that influence decoding and spelling in beginning readers. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 21: 165–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watkins, Marley W., Pui-Wa Lei, and Gary L. Canivez. 2007. Psychometric intelligence and achievement: A cross-lagged panel analysis. Intelligence 35: 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wechsler, David. 1991. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Third Edition (WISC-III). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, David. 2003. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (Fourth Edition (WISC-IV)). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, David. 2005. Wechsler Individual Achievement Test, 2nd ed. London: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, David. 2014. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 5th ed. London: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Woodcock, Richard W., Kevin S. McGrew, and Nancy Mather. 2001. Woodcock-Johnson III. Rolling Meadows: Riverside Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Woods, Isaac L., Christopher Niileksela, and Randy G. Floyd. 2021. Do Cattell-Horn-Carroll cognitive abilities predict reading achievement similarly for black children as for other racial/ethnic groups? Contemporary School Psychology 25: 183–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zaboski, Brian A., John H. Kranzler, and Nicholas A. Gage. 2018. Meta-analysis of the relationship between academic achievement and broad abilities of the Cattell-horn-Carroll theory. Journal of School Psychology 71: 42–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhang, Charlene, and Martin C. Yu. 2021. Planned missingness: How to and how much? Organizational Research Methods 25: 623–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Tests | KABC-II | WJ III | WISC-III | WISC-IV | WISC-V | DAS-II | KTEA-II | WIAT-II | WIAT-III |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
KABC-II XBA | 347 | 89 | 123 | 58 | - | - | - | - | - |
KABC-II/KTEA-II | 2223 | - | - | - | - | - | 2223 | - | - |
WISC-IV/DAS-II | - | - | - | 202 | - | 202 | - | - | - |
DAS-II/WIAT-II | - | - | - | - | - | 370 | - | 370 | - |
WISC-IV/WIAT-II | - | - | - | 532 | - | - | - | 532 | - |
WISC-V/WIAT-III | - | - | - | - | 181 | - | - | - | 181 |
WISC-V/KABC-II | 88 | - | - | - | 88 | - | - | - | - |
Total | 2658 | 89 | 123 | 792 | 269 | 572 | 2223 | 902 | 181 |
Achievement Tests | N | Mean | SD |
---|---|---|---|
WIAT (Edition; Specific Achievement Skill) | |||
Essay Composition (III; Written Expression) | 151 | 100.76 | 15.72 |
Pseudo-word Decoding (II and III; Basic Reading) | 1054 | 102.20 | 13.67 |
Spelling (II and III; Basic Writing) | 1083 | 101.59 | 14.26 |
Sentence Composition (III; Written Expression) | 179 | 99.59 | 12.83 |
Word Reading (II and III; Basic Reading) | 1078 | 101.92 | 14.28 |
Written Expression (II; Written Expression) | 871 | 101.91 | 15.49 |
KTEA-II (Specific Achievement Skill) | |||
Decoding Fluency (Basic Reading) | 2021 | 99.88 | 15.05 |
Spelling (Basic Writing) | 2021 | 99.74 | 14.91 |
Word Recognition (Basic Reading) | 2223 | 99.83 | 14.66 |
Written Expression (Written Expression) | 2223 | 100.03 | 15.16 |
Cognitive Tests | |||
DAS-II (Broad Ability; Narrow Ability) | |||
Copying (Gv; Visualization) | 178 | 51.23 | 8.41 |
Digits Backward (Gwm; Working Memory) | 557 | 49.82 | 8.56 |
Digits Forward (Gwm; Memory Span) | 557 | 49.81 | 9.78 |
Early Number Concepts (Gf; Quantitative Reasoning) | 178 | 51.10 | 8.64 |
Matching Letter Like Forms (Gv; Visualization) | 178 | 51.71 | 9.08 |
Matrices (Gf; Induction) | 557 | 50.21 | 9.19 |
Naming Vocabulary (Gc; Lexical Knowledge) | 178 | 51.35 | 9.41 |
Pattern Construction (Gv; Visualization) | 557 | 49.97 | 8.65 |
Rapid Naming (Gs; Rate of Test-Taking) | 557 | 50.47 | 8.97 |
Recall of Designs (Gv; Visual Memory) | 556 | 50.25 | 8.78 |
Recognition of Pictures (Gv; Visual Memory) | 557 | 50.26 | 9.29 |
Recall of Objects-Immediate (Gl; Free Recall Memory) | 557 | 49.06 | 10.32 |
Recall of Objects-Delayed (Gl; Free Recall Memory) | 557 | 50.17 | 9.46 |
Recall of Sequential Order (Gwm; Working Memory) | 557 | 50.09 | 9.48 |
Sequential and Quantitative Reasoning (Gf; Quantitative Reasoning) | 556 | 50.55 | 9.09 |
Speed of Information Processing (Gs; Perceptual Speed) | 557 | 51.07 | 9.21 |
Verbal Comprehension (Gc; Listening Ability; cross-loading: Gf) | 178 | 50.15 | 8.96 |
Verbal Similarities (Gc; Lexical Knowledge) | 557 | 50.66 | 8.48 |
Word Definitions (Gc; Lexical Knowledge) | 556 | 50.19 | 8.84 |
KABC-II (Broad Ability; Narrow Ability) | |||
Atlantis (Gl; Associative Memory) | 2654 | 10.02 | 3.09 |
Atlantis Delayed (Gl; Associative Memory) | 2435 | 9.93 | 2.80 |
Block Counting (Gv; Visualization) | 2655 | 9.97 | 3.00 |
Expressive Vocabulary (Gc; Lexical Knowledge) | 2656 | 9.85 | 2.95 |
Gestalt Closure (Gv; Closure Speed; cross-loading: Gc) | 619 | 10.00 | 2.89 |
Hand Movements (Gwm; Memory Span; cross-loading: Gf) | 2656 | 10.08 | 2.88 |
Number Recall (Gwm; Memory Span) | 2657 | 10.24 | 2.86 |
Pattern Reasoning (Gf; Induction) | 2656 | 10.19 | 2.96 |
Rebus (Gl; Associative Memo) | 2657 | 10.14 | 3.04 |
Rebus Delayed (Gl; Associative Memo) | 2407 | 10.03 | 2.96 |
Riddles (Gc; Lexical Knowledge) | 2657 | 10.14 | 3.04 |
Rover (Gv; Spatial Scanning) | 2652 | 10.15 | 3.02 |
Story Completion (Gf; General Sequential Reasoning) | 2653 | 10.10 | 2.98 |
Triangles (Gv; Visualization) | 2656 | 10.00 | 2.91 |
Verbal Knowledge (Gc; Lexical Knowledge) | 2657 | 10.01 | 2.94 |
Word Order (Gwm; Memory Span) | 2657 | 9.93 | 2.83 |
WISC (Broad Ability; Narrow Ability; Edition) | |||
Arithmetic (Gwm; Working Memory; cross-loaded: Gf, Gs; III–V) | 880 | 10.32 | 2.77 |
Block Design (Gv; Visualization; III–V) | 1178 | 10.18 | 2.84 |
Cancelation (Gs; Perceptual Speed; IV–V) | 998 | 10.03 | 3.02 |
Coding (Gs; Rate of Test Taking; III–V) | 1178 | 10.06 | 2.90 |
Comprehension (Gc; General Verbal Information; III–V) | 1174 | 10.25 | 2.89 |
Digit Span (Gwm; Memory Span and Working Memory; III–V) | 1167 | 10.04 | 2.86 |
Figure Weights (Gf; Quantitative Reasoning; V) | 269 | 9.92 | 2.69 |
Information (Gc; General Verbal Information; III–V) | 1124 | 10.25 | 2.84 |
Letter-Number Sequencing (Gwm; Working Memory; III–V) | 1050 | 10.00 | 2.82 |
Matrix Reasoning (Gf; Induction; IV–V) | 1060 | 10.21 | 2.86 |
Object Assembly (Gv; Closure Speed; III) | 123 | 10.34 | 2.95 |
* Picture Arrangement (Gf; General Sequential Reasoning; III) | 123 | 10.65 | 3.45 |
Picture Completion (Gv; Flexibility of Closure; cross-loaded: Gc; III and IV) | 324 | 10.33 | 3.00 |
Picture Concepts (Gf; Induction; IV–V) | 1060 | 10.28 | 2.92 |
Picture Span (Gwm; Working Memory; V) | 269 | 9.75 | 2.66 |
Similarities (Gc; Lexical Knowledge; III–V) | 1179 | 10.18 | 2.87 |
Symbol Search (Gs; Perceptual Speed; III–V) | 1143 | 10.21 | 2.93 |
Visual Puzzles (Gv; Visualization; V) | 268 | 10.06 | 2.62 |
Vocabulary (Gc; Lexical Knowledge; III–V) | 1178 | 10.14 | 2.91 |
WJ III (Broad Ability; Narrow Ability) | |||
Analysis-Synthesis (Gf; General Sequential Reasoning) | 87 | 102.91 | 17.19 |
Auditory Working Memory (Gwm; Working Memory) | 88 | 105.40 | 13.91 |
Concept Formation (Gf; Induction) | 89 | 105.36 | 13.90 |
Decision Speed (Gs; Perceptual Speed) | 88 | 100.56 | 16.18 |
General Information (Gc; General Verbal Information) | 89 | 98.37 | 16.16 |
Numbers Reversed (Gwm; Working Memory) | 89 | 100.62 | 14.31 |
Picture Recognition (Gv; Visual Memory; cross-loaded: Gl) | 89 | 100.79 | 12.58 |
Spatial Relations (Gv; Visualization) | 89 | 100.62 | 11.33 |
Verbal Comprehension (Gc; Lexical Knowledge) | 89 | 102.55 | 14.24 |
Visual-Auditory Learning (Gl; Associative Memory) | 89 | 94.65 | 19.76 |
Visual Matching (Gs; Perceptual Speed) | 89 | 95.84 | 13.37 |
Model | χ2 (df) | Δχ2 (Δdf) | Δp | CFI /TLI | RMSEA | SRMR | aBIC | AIC | BIC |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. All 6 Broad Cognitive Ability Paths to Basic Reading | 2890.05 (1474) | - | - | 0.96 | 0.02 | 0.09 | 370,849.32 | 370,133.66 | 371,583.33 |
2. Significant Broad Cognitive Ability Paths to Basic Reading | 2894.10 (1477) | 4.05 (3) | 0.26 | 0.96 | 0.02 | 0.09 | 370,838.07 | 370,131.71 | 371,562.55 |
3. g Direct Effect to Basic Reading | 3108.95 (1478) | 214.85 | <.001 | 0.95 | 0.02 | 0.09 | 371,047.83 | 370,344.56 | 371,769.13 |
4. Basic Reading to Written Expression | 3092.73 (1582) | - | - | 0.96 | 0.02 | 0.10 | 397,013.72 | 396,254.69 | 397,792.22 |
5. Spelling to Written Expression | 2956.22 (1502) | - | - | 0.96/0.95 | 0.02 | 0.10 | 373,701.52 | 372,961.08 | 374,460.96 |
6. Basic Reading to Spelling | 3097.67 (1551) | - | - | 0.96 | 0.02 | 0.10 | 393,053.64 | 392,316.30 | 393,809.90 |
7. Spelling to Basic Reading | 3100.40 (1551) | - | - | 0.96 | 0.02 | 0.10 | 393,056.37 | 392,319.03 | 393,812.63 |
8. Bidirectional Basic Reading–Spelling Paths (Nonrecursive) | 3096.22 (1550) | 1.46 (1) | 0.23 | 0.96 | 0.02 | 0.10 | 393,057.28 | 392,316.84 | 393,816.72 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Caemmerer, J.M.; Scudder, A.M.; Keith, T.Z.; Reynolds, M.R. Beyond Individual Tests: Youths’ Cognitive Abilities, Basic Reading, and Writing. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 120. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12110120
Caemmerer JM, Scudder AM, Keith TZ, Reynolds MR. Beyond Individual Tests: Youths’ Cognitive Abilities, Basic Reading, and Writing. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(11):120. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12110120
Chicago/Turabian StyleCaemmerer, Jacqueline M., Audrey M. Scudder, Timothy Z. Keith, and Matthew R. Reynolds. 2024. "Beyond Individual Tests: Youths’ Cognitive Abilities, Basic Reading, and Writing" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 11: 120. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12110120
APA StyleCaemmerer, J. M., Scudder, A. M., Keith, T. Z., & Reynolds, M. R. (2024). Beyond Individual Tests: Youths’ Cognitive Abilities, Basic Reading, and Writing. Journal of Intelligence, 12(11), 120. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12110120