Comparative Research of Visual Interpretation of Aerial Images and Topographic Maps for Unskilled Users: Searching for Objects Important for Decision-Making in Crisis Situations
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Current State-of-Art
- biological condition (age, gender, eyesight—sharpness, color vision, outages in the field of vision - , IQ, etc.)
- psychological and social status (setting, vigilance, interest, motivation, emotional tuning, adaptation, priming).
3. Data Collection
- the content of the interpretation was specified (what type of objects will be searched/interpreted by the respondent);
- the interpretation method (classification, listing, delimitation) was given;
- a suitable source image and a paired control map were chosen to allow a clear answer (in order to find a railway bridge, an image containing a larger number of bridges, but only a single railway bridge, had to be found);
- the structure of the tests and their evaluation was prepared;
- results were statistically processed and conclusions drawn;
- cooperation with experts from the Integrated Rescue System—firefighters, medical first aid rescuers and police officers was established in checking the suitability of used imagery as typical in their work.
- selected critical objects of the built-up area, representing obstacles to movement, possible source of risk or possibility of escape from the risk in progress (bridges, roads, watercourses, airports).
- map server “National Geoportal INSPIRE”;
- map server “mapy.cz”.
- basic topographic maps, scale 1:10,000, 1:25,000 and 1:50,000;
- color vertical aerial imagery, resolution 0.5 m.
- vertical aerial image—map (see examples in Section 4.2.);
- orthophotomap—map.
- closed answer (forced choice from proposed answers), e.g., the encircled object is: (a) road bridge, or (b) rail bridge),
- open answer, e.g., two chosen objects are in the picture,
- identification of an object by clicking, each click equal one objects (from the total known number of objects in the picture),
- selection from multiple objects in an image, on a map, e.g., many object are encircled, select objects of one class only,
- shape identification—line, polygon, mark (a click of the mouse on the desired object shape, its contour line, etc.), e.g., click on the object´s outline corners (at least 80% of the outline length must be correct to accept the object identification as correct).
- the whole group of respondents (151 persons);
- men (67 persons);
- women (84 persons);
- lay people (working outside disaster management, cartography and geography, non-geographic students, bachelor degree students of geography—1st year) (74 persons);
- specialists (completed university studies in the field of cartography and geography, students of higher years of master and bachelor studies) (77 persons);
- soldiers (active, in reserve) and University of Defense students, professional soldiers from other workplaces (12 persons, as a part of the specialist group);
- disaster management personnel (hereafter “crisists”): firefighters, rescuers, police officers, (10 persons, as a part of the specialist group).
4. Results of Aerial Imagery and Map Reading
4.1. Criteria and Objects of Evaluation
- success (correctness or accuracy) of identification;
- speed of interpretation.
- specific critical objects;
- specific critical lines.
- Identification of a railway bridge;
- Identification of a road bridge;
- Identification of a watercourse;
- Identification of a road;
- Identification of a railway station;
- Identification of an airport facility.
4.2. Commentary to Statistical Data on Aerial Imagery and Map Reading
4.3. Comparison of Statistical Data Processing Results with Research Hypotheses
5. Discussion and Conclusions
- used to a limited extent compared to maps for providing a more detailed view of the place of intervention or the current view of the crisis situation;
- as an intelligible source for the personnel of the Integrated Rescue System—parallel with maps.
- people with disaster management experience (soldiers and “crisists”—see Table 5) are quicker in solving tasks on an aerial image;
- the success rate of solving tasks on an aerial image was the same for men and women, as well as for groups of people with or without experience of disaster management;
- the advantage for their use in disaster management is their good comprehensibility (better than that of maps) and overall less demand for abstract thinking, which can be lowered in stress;
- for the purpose of disaster management and fast orientation in a particular location, the aerial image should be supplemented by a clear description and other thematic drawing;
- the aerial imagery is used in disaster management in the Czech Republic, the use of vertical as well as oblique images will be further expanded.
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Konecny, M.; Brezinová, S.; Lukas, H.; Drapela, M.V.; Kubicek, P.; Talhofer, V.; Reznik, T.; Stachon, Z.; Kolejka, J.; et al. Dynamicka geovizualizace v krizovem managementu, 1st ed.; Masarykova Univerzita: Brno, Czech Republic, 2011; p. 385. [Google Scholar]
- Scialfa, C.T. The role of sensory factors in cognitive aging research. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 2002, 56, 153–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weale, R.A. Retinal illumination and age. Trans. Illum. Eng. Soc. 1961, 26, 95–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blaut, J.M. The mapping abilities of young children: Children can. Ann. Am. Geogr. 1997, 87, 152–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Downs, R.M.; Liben, L. The final summation: The defence rests. Ann. Am. Geogr. 1997, 87, 178–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liben, L.S.; Downs, R.M. Canism and cannotianism: A straw child. Ann. Assoc. Am. Geogr. 1997, 87, 159–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spencer, C.; Blades, M.; Morsley, K. The Child in the Physical Environment: The Development of Spatial Knowledge and Cognition; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Madden, D.J.; Allen, P.A. Adult age differences in the rate of information extraction during visual search. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 1991, 46, 124–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, E.T.; Fujawa, G.; Shikano, T. Perceptual processing and search efficiency of young and older adults in a simple-feature search task: A staircase approach. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2002, 57, 324–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salthouse, T.A.; Somberg, B.L. Isolating the age deficit in speeded performance. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 1982, 37, 59–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salthouse, T.A. Aging and measures of processing speed. Biol. Psychol. 2000, 54, 35–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baker, D.H.; Graf, E.W. Equivalence of physical and perceived speed in binocular rivalry. J. Vis. 2008, 8, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Linn, M.C.; Petersen, A.C. Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Dev. 1985, 56, 1479–1498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Voyer, D.; Voyer, S.; Bryden, M.P. Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychol. Bull. 1995, 117, 250–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nowaczyk, R.H. Sex-related differences in the color lexicon. Lang. Speech 1982, 25, 257–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pérez-Carpinell, J.; Baldovi, R.; de Fez, M.D.; Castro, J. Color memory matching: Time effect and other factors. Color Res. Appl. 1998, 23, 234–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hund, A.M.; Gill, D.M. What constitutes effective way finding directions: The role of descriptive cues and task demands. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 217–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halpern, D.; Crothers, M. The sex of cognition. In Sexual Orientation: Toward Biological Understanding; Ellis, L., Ed.; Praeger: Westport, CT, USA, 1997; pp. 187–197. [Google Scholar]
- Galea, L.; Kimura, D. Less skilled readers have less efficient suppression mechanism. Psychol. Sci. 1993, 4, 294–298. [Google Scholar]
- Birenbaum, M.; Kelly, A.; Levi-Keren, M. Stimulus features and sex differences in mental rotation test performance. Intelligence 1994, 19, 51–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jenny, B.; Kelso, N.V. Color design for the color vision impaired. Cartogr. Perspect. 2007, 57, 61–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brewer, C.A. Spectral schemes: Controversial color use on maps. Cartogr. Geogr. Inf. Syst. 1997, 24, 203–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olson, J.M.; Brewer, C.A. An evaluation of color selections to accommodate map users with color-vision impairments. Ann. Assoc. Am. Geogr. 1997, 87, 103–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hollingworth, A.; Henderson, J. Object identification is isolated from scene semantic constrain: Evidence from object type and token discrimination. Psychol. Sci. 1999, 10, 319–343. [Google Scholar]
- Chun, M. Contextual cueing of visual attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 170–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosch, E. Natural categories. Cogn. Psychol. 1973, 4, 328–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosch, E.; Mervis, C. Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cogn. psychol. 1975, 7, 573–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lloyd, R.; Patton, D.; Cammack, R. Basic-level geographic categories. Prof. Geogr. 1996, 48, 181–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dyce, M. Canada between the photograph and the map: Aerial photography, geographical vision and the state. J. Hist. Geogr. 2013, 39, 69–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Atkinsonova, R.L.; Atkinson, R.C.; Smith, E.E.; Bem, D.J.; Nolen-Hoeksema, S. Psychologie, 1st ed.; Victoria Publishing: Praha, Czech Republic, 1995; p. 863. [Google Scholar]
- Morong, K.; Sasinka, C. Hypothesis—Online software platform for objective experimental testing. In Proceedings of the International Conference Applying Principles of Cognitive Psychology in Practice, Brno, Czech Republic, 15–16 May 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Majidi, B.; Patra, J.C.; Zheng, J. Modular interpretation of low altitude aerial images of non-urban environment. Digit. Signal Process. 2014, 26, 127–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chang, K.T.; Antes, J.R. Sex and cultural differences in map reading. Am. Cartogr. 1987, 14, 29–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sikl, R. Zrakove vnimani; Grada Publishing: Praha, Czech Republic, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Kempf, D.S.; Palan, K.M.; Laczniak, R.N. Gender differences in information processing confidence in an advertising context: A preliminary study. Adv. Consum. Res. 1997, 24, 443–449. [Google Scholar]
- Mather, G. Foundations of Perception; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, W.; McCoy, N. Self-confidence, self-esteem, and assumptions of sex role in young men and women. Percept. Motor Skill 2000, 90, 751–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Step Designation | Data Collection Methodological Steps |
---|---|
A | Compare the success in task solution on aerial images and control maps (displaying the same area as the image). |
B | Compare the speed and success in tasks solutions on control maps. |
C | Analyze the success rate and the speed of visual interpretation of the images with respect to respondents’ gender and professional or work experience. |
D | Evaluate the success in task solution to identify strategically important objects on aerial images. |
Part of the Test | Content | Number of Tasks | Number of Images and Maps | Type of Result |
---|---|---|---|---|
respondent data | respondent data, gender, age, education, work experience with pictures and maps | 10 | 0 | number, multiple choice |
training tasks | introductory test tasks to get practice in the answering method | 4 | 8 | (0,1) text, coordinates, marking (mouse click), time |
test tasks | real test with aerial images and maps | 5 | 5 | (0,1) text, coordinates, marking (mouse click), time |
Code | Hypotheses to Aerial Image and Map Interpretation | Result | I:M Average Success Rate Ratio |
---|---|---|---|
H1 | The success rate of aerial image interpretation all objects is the same as that of map. | Not confirmed | 97.19:92.38 (%) |
H2 | The speed of aerial image interpretation all objects is the same as that of map. | Not confirmed | 20.569:30.864 (time spent in s/object) |
H3 | The success (accuracy) in identifying the course of a watercourse on aerial images is the same as on maps. | Success rate is higher on maps | 9.27:28.48 (%) |
H4 | The success of interpreting critical objects on aerial images is the same as on maps. | Success rate is higher on aerial images | 82.05:73.84 (%) |
H5 | The speed of interpreting critical objects on aerial images is the same as on maps. | Success rate is higher on aerial images | 119.309:146.511 (time spent in s/object) |
Task Description | Identification of Critical Objects in Aerial Imagery and Maps | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Railway Bridge | Road Bridge | Watercourse | Road | Railway Station | ||||||
Identification of Objects on an Aerial Image and Map | I Image | M Map | I Image | M Map | I Image | M Map | I Image | M Map | I Image | M Map |
Number of correct answers (from possible 151) | 146 | 144 | 101 | 146 | 14 | 43 | 147 | 126 | 122 | 116 |
Number of correct answers (%) | 96.69 | 95.36 | 66.89 | 96.69 | 9.27 | 28.48 | 97.35 | 83.44 | 80.79 | 76.82 |
Average time (s) | 10.72 | 8.53 | 17.13 | 8.33 | 56.94 | 37.31 | 1831.64 | 1944.63 | 11.41 | 19.41 |
Max. time (s) | 46.16 | 53.45 | 97.15 | 35.51 | 260.79 | 100.89 | 12.13 | 12.88 | 44.78 | 111.08 |
Min. time (s) | 3.36 | 3.33 | 4.87 | 1.12 | 8.92 | 9.57 | 59.64 | 31.65 | 3.03 | 0.36 |
Median (s) | 9.43 | 7.36 | 13.53 | 6.97 | 48.10 | 34.79 | 4.66 | 1.93 | 9.22 | 15.51 |
1. quartile (s) | 6.78 | 5.72 | 9.57 | 5.39 | 33.28 | 26.23 | 10.70 | 11.50 | 6.11 | 10.04 |
3. quartile (s) | 13.18 | 9.47 | 19.58 | 9.20 | 70.34 | 44.75 | 8.10 | 7.41 | 13.37 | 23.88 |
Standard deviation | 5.83 | 5.26 | 12.07 | 5.06 | 36.19 | 16.21 | 14.14 | 17.52 | 7.79 | 14.91 |
S.n. | Hypotheses on the Interpretation of an Aerial Image and a Map | Hypothesis Validity |
---|---|---|
1 | Men and women are equally successful in interpreting aerial images. | confirmed |
2 | Men and women are equally fast in interpreting aerial images. | confirmed |
3 | Laymen and experts are equally successful in interpreting aerial images. | confirmed |
4 | Laymen and experts are equally fast in interpreting aerial images. | confirmed |
5 | Soldiers and civilians are equally successful in interpreting aerial images. | confirmed |
6 | Soldiers and civilians are equally fast in interpreting aerial images. | not confirmed, soldiers are faster |
7 | Crisists and non-crisists are equally successful in interpreting aerial images. | confirmed |
8 | Crisists and non-crisists are equally fast in interpreting aerial images. | not confirmed, crisists are faster |
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Svatonova, H.; Kolejka, J. Comparative Research of Visual Interpretation of Aerial Images and Topographic Maps for Unskilled Users: Searching for Objects Important for Decision-Making in Crisis Situations. ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2017, 6, 231. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijgi6080231
Svatonova H, Kolejka J. Comparative Research of Visual Interpretation of Aerial Images and Topographic Maps for Unskilled Users: Searching for Objects Important for Decision-Making in Crisis Situations. ISPRS International Journal of Geo-Information. 2017; 6(8):231. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijgi6080231
Chicago/Turabian StyleSvatonova, Hana, and Jaromir Kolejka. 2017. "Comparative Research of Visual Interpretation of Aerial Images and Topographic Maps for Unskilled Users: Searching for Objects Important for Decision-Making in Crisis Situations" ISPRS International Journal of Geo-Information 6, no. 8: 231. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijgi6080231
APA StyleSvatonova, H., & Kolejka, J. (2017). Comparative Research of Visual Interpretation of Aerial Images and Topographic Maps for Unskilled Users: Searching for Objects Important for Decision-Making in Crisis Situations. ISPRS International Journal of Geo-Information, 6(8), 231. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijgi6080231