Next Article in Journal
Determiner Asymmetry in Mixed Nominal Constructions: The Role of Grammatical Factors in Data from Miami and Nicaragua
Next Article in Special Issue
Introducing the Special Issue: MOBILizing Language Learning in the 21st Century
Previous Article in Journal
A View of the CP/DP-(non)parallelism from the Cartographic Perspective
Previous Article in Special Issue
Achieving Independent Language Learning through the Mobilization of Ubiquitous Instructional Technology Resources
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Improving Motivation to Learn English in Japan with a Self-Study Shadowing Application

by
Jennifer Louise Teeter
Institute for Liberal Arts and Sciences, Kyoto University, Yoshida-honmachi, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
Languages 2017, 2(4), 19; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2040019
Submission received: 28 February 2017 / Revised: 21 August 2017 / Accepted: 29 August 2017 / Published: 27 September 2017
(This article belongs to the Special Issue MOBILizing Language Learning in the 21st Century)

Abstract

:
The scarcity of opportunities to communicate in English in Japan proves a challenge for learners, as significant improvements in English as a Foreign Language) (EFL) listening and speaking will not materialize without consistent practice and a motivation to study. Furthermore, analysis of standardized test scores shows that university students’ scores tend to decrease after their first year of study (Sumida 2015; Mikada 2016). In order to overcome these difficulties, a team of teachers at a university in Japan introduced a mobile-assisted language learning (MALL) shadowing application where students can train their ears while also practicing speaking with shadowing, a technique recognized as effective for improving speaking and listening (Kadota 2014; Hamada 2016). This paper describes the introduction of this application in general education English classes. It then considers its impact on the motivation, attitudes towards communicating orally in English, and perception of English ability of 1001 first-year university students, the majority science and engineering majors, who used it over one semester. Preliminary results of a pre- and post-intervention Likert questionnaire indicate that through this system, linguistic self-confidence, interest in English, ideal L2 self, attitudes towards communicating in the L2, and perceptions of English ability were potentially enhanced.

1. Introduction

The importance of acquiring oral English communication proficiency continues to grow in Japan. Japan-based multi-national corporations, such as Uniqlo and Rakuten have made English their official language [1] and researchers in the fields of science and engineering are experiencing more pressure to produce academic papers and present at conferences in English [2]. These societal changes have reverberated at the policy level, as the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT) is working to improve students’ English communication skills through significant curriculum changes. At present, foreign language education, which is essentially synonymous with “white middle class [American] English” [3], is compulsory in the fifth and sixth grades of elementary school and all of junior high. Starting from 2020, the new curriculum changes are making foreign language education classes mandatory from primary third grade, and 120 primary schools are currently serving as test schools for the new textbooks that will be used [4]. In addition, MEXT has mandated that all these classes at the junior and senior high school level be taught in English [5].
In turn, universities in Japan are increasingly responding to these same pressures to improve students’ English communicative competence by increasing the number of English language and English-medium courses in their curricula [6]. However, with an overall lack of English language exposure outside of the classroom for most students, opportunity for study is often limited to one to a few 90-min sessions a week. Although there is a prevalence of classes titled “Oral Communication” in universities across the country, a variety of factors, including class size, lack of oral communication-focused activities, and other classroom issues, often prevent these classes from being true to their given names. In order for a person to retain new words and phrases, high quality, frequent and deliberate practice is necessary [7]. Additionally, studies of how proficiency test scores change after students enter university show that by the end of students’ first year, scores on Test of English for International Communication (TOEIC) and Test of English as a Foreign Language- Institutional Testing Program (TOEFL-ITP) decline [8,9]. In addition, at the university where this study was conducted, surveys show that listening scores are relatively low compared to reading [10,11].
Given the situation of English-language scarcity in Japan, motivation and attitudes towards English are factors that could also influence students’ English learning success. Motivation has been defined as a “combination of effort plus desire to achieve the goal of learning the language plus favorable attitudes towards learning the language” [12] (p. 10). Ellis describes how motivation and attitudes impact the amount of exertion a student puts into language learning: “motivation involves the attitudes and affective states that influence the degree of effort that learners make to learn an L2” [13] (p. 75). The definition by Ajzen of an attitude is commonly used in second language acquisition literature, “an attitude is a disposition to respond favorably or unfavorably to the object, person, institution, or event” [14] (p. 3). In other words, if learners have positive attitudes towards the target language, their motivation to learn the language could be higher. Furthermore, because motivation and attitudes towards learning a target language are considered predictors of second language learning achievement, enhancing student motivation and attitudes towards English can have a positive impact on their second language learning. The lack of English study opportunity has impacts on every English learner, but motivation can give them the drive to effectively utilize listening and speaking opportunities that exist, and create opportunities as well. Assessing how and if learning techniques and teaching strategies employed in certain contexts influence student engagement is important to delivering effective language courses [15,16].
To address the difficulties outlined above in learning to speak English, the use of mobile-assisted language learning (MALL) technology in English as a Second Language (ESL) and English as a Foreign Language (EFL) classrooms is gaining traction because of its ability to provide flexible access to language learning materials. Shadowing is one technique considered effective for improving listening and speaking [17], yet students often lack access to appropriate shadowing materials. Research also suggests that shadowing [18] and MALL technology [19] independently of each other can enhance learner motivation. Nonetheless, either a small number of participants or a lack of detailed analysis of motivational variables limits studies in both areas. Exploring the impacts on learners’ motivation of combining shadowing with MALL technology may prove fruitful. Therefore, taking advantage of the high ownership rates of smart phones by university students, a team of researchers at a high-ranking university in Japan introduced a self-study shadowing application for smart phones, tablets, and computers (“shadowing intervention”) for first-year university students mainly majoring in science and engineering (S&E) [11,20]. The purpose of this shadowing intervention is to provide students with opportunities to practice speaking and listening without the restraints of time and space. Keeping in mind that learners’ motivation is connected, even if indirectly, to language learning success, this study explores how the use of the shadowing application influences learners’ motivation and gains in listening and speaking proficiency. Drawing upon previous studies based on motivation and attitudes towards L2 learning [17,21], this article will address the following questions:
  • How do student perceptions of their English listening and speaking ability change due to the shadowing intervention?
  • How does students’ motivational intensity related to studying listening and speaking change after shadowing for 12 weeks?
  • What are students’ attitudes towards shadowing?
  • How does time spent shadowing correlate with self-evaluation of English listening and speaking ability?
  • How does time spent shadowing correlate with changes in motivation?
This article first provides an overview of past studies on the use of MALL technology in education and shadowing for second language acquisition both in general and in the Japanese context. After outlining work in motivation studies, it then hypothesizes on how combining MALL with shadowing can potentially enhance students’ motivation and proficiency. A description of the research design involving a Likert-based questionnaire follows. While analysis is primarily statistical in nature, this study by no means attempts to support the assumption that statistical analysis paints a complete picture of students’ motivation and attitudes. Like Pigott, the author of this study concurs—statistics is not the “de rigueur means of investigation” [22] (p. 28); statistical studies like this may, however, provide some insight into students’ perceptions. Finally, analysis of the results and suggestions for future research will follow.

2. Literature Review

2.1. Mobile-Assisted Language Learning

Research into mobile learning entails more than simply examining learning outcomes through teaching materials delivered via mobile devices, such as smart phones, tablets, mp3 players, and personal data assistants. Rather the definition has evolved and is now referred to as “the study of how the mobility of learners augmented by personal and public technology can contribute to the process of gaining new knowledge, skills and experience” [23] (p. 235). Examining how learners’ interaction with mobile technology transcends the constraints of time and space to transform the learning process is key. Mobile-assisted language learning (MALL is a sub-branch of computer-assisted language learning (CALL) and mobile learning where the teaching of languages and/or language learning is enhanced or assisted through the use of mobile devices [24].
Educators have been debating the merits and demerits of using mobile technology in the classroom, especially given the growth in student ownership of mobile devices. Results from a 2015 Pew Research Center survey indicate that smart phone ownership in Japan for people aged 18–34 is 77% [25] and high school student ownership is estimated to be as high as 93% [26]. Rather than resist the growth of the so-called “digital native” population, framed as being highly familiar with information and communication technology, educators are beginning to embrace it [27]. It is important to note however, that assumptions about students’ familiarity with technology can lead to poor pedagogical decisions. Overemphasizing access to technology can divert attention from examining whether the technology in question can make teaching and learning more effective [28].
While bearing this in mind, MALL applications are proving beneficial to learners in non-immersion settings by creating seamless learning opportunities for language learners [29]. Several studies have demonstrated the potential of MALL use to positively impact speaking and listening skills [30,31], increase learner recollection of target vocabulary, and enhance learner autonomy [32]. One small-scale study at a Japanese university found that daily vocabulary emails improved vocabulary retention in comparison with traditional methods. Students in the study reported that they prefer this use of MALL technology to paper-based materials [33]. More studies are needed, however, that specifically analyze how MALL can be utilized to enhance teaching, different aspects of learning, motivation, and attitudes.

2.2. Shadowing

In the field of second language acquisition (SLA), shadowing is a broad term referring to a technique where auditory material is repeated almost simultaneously either silently or out loud. Originally used in cognitive psychology to test selective attention [34], it has also been utilized by doctors for language therapy to help treat stuttering [35], and by interpreters for training purposes [36]. Murphey outlined three variations of shadowing: verbatim, focusing on certain words, and interactive with another speaker [37]. Hamada further delineated categories to include pre-shadowing/bottom-up shadowing where a script is read and studied prior to shadowing, and post-shadowing/top-down shadowing carried out without preliminary use of a script [38]. The study employed verbatim shadowing, either pre-shadowing or top-down shadowing.
Several studies of shadowing examined the positive impacts it can have on speaking proficiency. In Foote and McDonough’s recent study in Canada, sixteen participants practiced shadowing short dialogues for eight weeks on iPods (Apple Inc., Cupertino, CA, USA), and then did an extemporaneous speaking task. Results based on twenty-two non-expert raters indicated that their comprehensibility, and fluency improved [39]. Rongna and Hayashi’s, in their study involving students of Japanese as a foreign language, suggested that shadowing influenced speech rate and accuracy of pitch accent positively [40], with Yamane et al. (cited in Bovee and Stewart, [34]) citing improvements in student pitch range from 17.7 to 68.3% after studying English through shadowing [41]. Torikai, et al. concluded that shadowing enhances several aspects of speaking including accuracy in production of reduced forms, connected speech, and prosody [42]. A shadowing study by Mochizuki using materials from an EFL textbook showed improvements in standardized speaking test results after six classes [43]. With studies demonstrating overall improvements seen through test results, as well as specific improvements in aspects such as speech rate, pitch, prosody, and connected speech, shadowing clearly has potential to improve learners’ speaking proficiency.
Research has also been conducted on how to implement shadowing practice. Nakayama and Suzuki found that self-monitored shadowing led to improvements in shadowing performance when compared with pair-monitored shadowing [44]. Mori demonstrated that combining shadowing with oral reading improved English rhythm, intonation, and final lengthening [45]. The double blind study by Bovee and Stewart using native-speaker raters, concluded that shadowing is more effective for low-level learners of English and contended that a “ceiling effect” limits “the degree to which mid- and high-level students could improve” [34] (p. 894). More research that focuses on how shadowing practice can be adapted effectively for varying speaking proficiency levels could provide practical insights for learners and educators.
Other scholars point to the benefits of shadowing for improving listening comprehension. Hamada, in a study of 43 Japanese university level students, concluded that phoneme perception improved through nine lessons including shadowing practice [46]. Tamai’s studies determined that in comparison to transcription, shadowing practice helped students obtain higher scores on listening comprehension tests [47], and that students in lower levels demonstrate more gains from shadowing than those in higher levels [48]. In a study with 41 students working to acquire an Aviation English Proficiency Test qualification, Kato saw improved TOEIC listening test scores after 5 months of shadowing (weekly 15 min shadowing sessions) [49]. While these studies focus on proficiency test score improvement, students’ perceptions of improvements of their own listening ability are lacking, as are studies of improvement for the type of listening comprehension needed for conversation.
The impact of shadowing practice on overall language learning is also being studied, with Japan becoming one of the core sites for such investigations. Kadota reported that shadowing helps increase the capacity of the working memory [50]. Nakayama and Armstrong examined how the priming effect can stimulate the recall of previously learned material when shadowing [51]. Recent studies have been focused on fine-tuning and identifying best practices in shadowing pedagogy, and discovering techniques of reducing anxiety and improving motivation through the practice of shadowing [38].

2.3. Motivation

Although scholars disagree about what and how different factors intertwine over time and space to affect second language acquisition, the literature points to internal factors such as age, personality, intrinsic motivation, experience, cognition, and native language. External factors include method of instruction, curriculum, power relationships, extrinsic motivation, and access to L2 speakers [52]. While intrinsic and extrinsic motivation are not the only variables that influence language gain, and they certainly do not capture the entire story of one person’s process of language acquisition, research has suggested that motivation is a predictor of success in language learning [53], as it both sparks and sustains the learning process [17]. Some scholars view motivation as equal in importance to language aptitude [54].
Since Gardner and Lambert’s seminal work in the Canadian context [55], SLA motivation has been a focus of extensive scholarly attention. This research is often separated into three overlapping periods: the social psychological period (1959–1990), the cognitive-situated period (late 1980s–2000), and the socio-dynamic period (around 2000–Present) [56].
The social-psychological period is characterized by theorizing based on Gardner’s research situated in Montreal where a perceived ethno-linguistic divide between French and English speakers was noted. In his theories, successful language learning is influenced by integrativeness, or the desire to identify with or integrate into the cultural community of the target language. This integrative process forms a key part of Gardner and Smythe’s Socio-Educational Model of Second Language Acquisition, which characterizes the types of theories developed in this period [57]. Later in this period, instrumentality, the pragmatic gains that learning a language brings such as future jobs or higher salary, were added as a secondary dimension to the theory.
Commonly referred to as the “educational shift” in motivation, in the late 1980s and 1990s, scholars, such as Crookes and Schmidt [58], influenced by work in educational psychology, argued that the social-psychological perspective did not adequately depict the entirety of SLA motivation. Researchers suggested that the classroom atmosphere had a significant impact and added education-oriented approaches into theorizing. The micro-components of this analysis included the organization courses, effectiveness of teaching methods, and relevance of tasks. Cognitive aspects of motivation related to learners’ view of their “self” were also introduced to the examination of motivation. For example, in one representative theory from the period, self-determination theory (SDT) [59], motivation is comprised of intrinsic, extrinsic, and integrative dimensions. In Noels’ conception of SDT, motivation has external, intrinsic, and amotivational orientations. In studies based on her theory, she has found that “the key to fostering university-level students’ motivation is by supporting the development of their autonomy and their competence in language learning” plus a plan of action where students can check their progress [15] (p. 26).
In the late 1990s, academic thought on motivation began to integrate notions of power relations. Norton Peirce, noting that existing concepts of motivation did not “capture the complex relationship between relations of power, identity, and language learning” [60] (p. 17), argued that investment, which acknowledges a learner’s multiple identities that change over time and space, captures a language learner’s desire to speak more effectively than motivation. A learner invests in language learning knowing that they will gain resources that will enhance their cultural capital and social power.
The socio-dynamic period, the most recent period in SLA motivation research, takes into consideration critiques, including Norton Peirce’s, that motivation is fluid and changing depending on time and space and is influenced by structures of power. In addition to classroom variables, the importance of a learner’s personal and social experience and attributes is recognized. Dörnyei’s L2 Motivational Self-System, representative of the Socio-dynamic Period, considers how dimensions of ideal L2 self, the ought-to L2 self, and L2 learning experience interact in developing a person’s SLA motivation [17].

2.4. Motivation Research in Japan

Recently, a significant number of theoretical and empirical studies on motivation have been undertaken in Japan in the university context. Ryan’s study aimed to validate Dörnyei’s L2 Motivational Self system [21] and Irie and Brewster [61] reported preliminary findings of their longitudinal study examining how ideal L2 self serves as a guide in language learning. Munezane found in her quantitative studies that connecting learners to their ideal self in the classroom has positive impacts on motivation [62]. Therefore, expanding research to identify effective methods for connecting students with their ideal L2 selves in and outside of the classroom could prove beneficial.
The influence of anxiety, or the feeling of apprehension about using a foreign language, on language learning and motivation is another highly relevant variable in the Japanese context. In Japan, even amongst highly motivated and diligent students, high levels of apprehension about communicating in English are viewed as one of the major, if not the strongest, barriers to willingness to communicate in English [63]. Feelings of negative anxiety about speaking were also found to predict willingness to communicate and achievement [17,64,65,66,67]. Studies on foreign language anxiety in Japan have included explorations into which situations cause anxiety [68] and how anxiety is linked to motivation [69]. Research on practical techniques for learners to identify their own levels of anxiety and overcome the hurdles anxiety brings to the process of language learning is lacking.
Other variables of interest that have a correlation with motivation include linguistic self-confidence and attitudes towards the L2 [70]. Linguistic self-confidence refers to a learner’s belief in their ability to become proficient in a target language and is related to the quality of contact with the language [71]. Since the study of English in Japan entails minimum contact with English-speaking communities, it is an important factor to consider. A longitudinal study of Japanese university students found that low linguistic self-confidence had a negative influence on motivation [72]. While proven to be an important factor, further study-specific techniques for improving a student’s linguistic self-confidence are lacking. Attitudes towards learning English also influence language acquisition with positive attitudes viewed as a predictor of language learning success, and negative attitudes shown as having a negative impact on language learning [13,16]. While proven to be an important factor, practical studies on how these attitudes change over time based on learning experiences are needed.

2.5. Motivation and Science and Engineering Majors

Several studies have evaluated the L2 motivation of engineering and science majors. In one survey including 397 tertiary-level students, the researchers concluded that engineering majors study English for more pragmatic reasons than those who expressed a need for English in the future [73]. In terms of differences between first-year science and engineering majors classified into high proficiency (HP) and low proficiency (LP) groups, Falout and Maruyama concluded that both proficiency groups showed favorable attitudes to the English-speaking community and attribute their demotivation to disappointment in performance, course content and pace, and teacher’s pedagogical approach [74]:
LP students more often internalized the causes of their demotivation. HP students showed more control over their affective states and learning situations. They especially despised the pedagogy of humiliation, though both sets vented most about teachers.
[74] (p. 7)
A pedagogy that does not focus on pointing out learners’ mistakes, but instead allows them independence to self-correct and learn at their own pace may be effective in countering demotivation.
Additionally, anecdotal evidence suggests that teachers tend to assume that S&E majors do not “like” English. These results add evidence that suggests otherwise—they might feel demotivated, but that does not mean they do not have positive attitudes towards the English-community. Finally, Apple’s study found that S&E students tended to show an interest in communicating in English, but believe their speaking ability to be below average, and have relatively high anxiety towards English used in the classroom. He also concluded that for S&E majors, ought-to L2 self has more of an impact on L2 achievement than ideal and future selves [75]. Based on the results of Kaneko’s study of 38 undergraduate science majors who experienced seminars in English conducted by Japanese scientists, she suggested that helping students “form a clearer image of their Ideal L2 self” by providing Japanese role models could improve motivation [76] (p. 21). The results of these studies cannot necessarily be generalized to apply to all S&E majors, but do suggest some tendencies. Based on these results, however, more research on how to tailor pedagogy to the varying needs of science and engineering students is necessary.
Identifying and assessing teaching strategies, techniques, and pedagogies can have a positive influence on student motivation and learning. Bernaus and Gardner concluded after a study of students’ perceptions of teacher strategies that, in order for “strategies to be effective in influencing students’ motivation they must be perceived as such by the students. We recommend, therefore, that teachers assess their students’ perceptions of any strategies they employ” [16] (p. 399). This assessment can be used to guide teachers in developing effective materials for each teaching context. Developing teaching methods that diminish or eliminate anxiety-causing situations and enhance self-efficacy can benefit language learners in Japan.

2.6. Motivation and Mobile-Assisted Language Learning

Research on the influence of MALL on motivational change, however, is limited. One study using a Funds of Knowledge approach found that motivation to write increased using a digital hand writing application on i-Pads (Apple Inc.) [77]. Researchers studying the responses of 60 ESL undergraduates’ engagement with the Taplingua (Taplingua SL, Madrid, Spain) application for listening and exercises for grammar and vocabulary inferred that motivation increased with frequency of use of the application as the semester progressed [19]. A study at a Romanian medical university specifically asked students how they believed MALL would enhance their motivation. Responses pointed to accessibility, the informal nature of MALL in making learning fun, and benefits of students controlling their own learning [78]. While these studies incorporated analysis on student achievement, they fail to assess specific variables that cause the “self” to take action.

2.7. Motivation and Shadowing

There is also room for more inquiry into how the practice of shadowing is related to motivation. Several studies have noted that post-shadowing has the potential to decrease anxiety and increase motivation. The two Japanese adult participants in one study reported improved listening and speaking proficiency and lower anxiety levels through a hybrid read aloud plus shadowing methodology [18]. Another mixed methods study by Hamada in which eight Japanese sophomores were interviewed found that initial motivation levels in intermediate learners of English were critical in determining how much a learner was motivated by shadowing. Based on these results, he suggested that allowing students to consistently “check their improvement would enhance the effectiveness of shadowing training” [79] (p. 19). In a 5-week mixed-methods study in Taiwan with 25 high school students, the researcher concluded that shadowing enhanced students’ attitudes towards learning English and self-confidence towards speaking English. Although several thought shadowing to be boring, the students recognized it as a way for them to improve their English [80]. Studies with greater numbers of participants and a longer period of time that shadowing is practiced can shed further light on the impact of shadowing on motivation.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Participants

The participants in this study were 1001 first-year university students at a university in Japan, ages ranging from 18 to 23, mostly majoring in the sciences (engineering (30%); medicine (19%), economics (10%), agriculture (12%). Only 29% of the participants majored in the liberal arts (literature, humanities, law and education) (see Figure 1). In total, 72% of the participants identified as male and 28% as female. In total, 97% identified as Japanese. In total, 36% of the participants had visited an English-speaking country with only 5% staying longer than three months. In terms of contact with English, 72% of the students never encountered English speakers outside of class. Outside of class, 76% listen to English media less than 30 min a week and 61% listen to English music less than 30 min a week. The general curriculum did not require listening comprehension-focused classes. After the study was introduced, the participants provided informed consent and were given the option to remove themselves from the study at anytime.
The university where the study took place is a highly competitive research-oriented university maintaining the second highest ranking in Japan, and an average hensachi of 72.2 [81]. Hensachi is an indicator of the difficulty of the entrance exams for a university department compared to all universities nationwide. The average hensachi for universities is around 50 with the highest at 80 and the lowest at 34. While the university has not conducted a comprehensive study of students’ English proficiency levels, based on unofficial calculations, TOEFL-ITP scores at the university are an average of 78 out of 120, with an average writing score of 22 out of 30 [82]. The average scores for the listening and reading sections of TOEIC summer intensive courses at the university from 2011 to 2014 were 56.37 and 65.39 respectively [11].

3.2. Shadowing Application Description

In order to address the issue of student access to listening comprehension materials for practice, researchers at the university developed a shadowing application accessible via smart phones, computers, and tablets [11,20]. The shadowing application provides students with materials through their devices, enabling them to practice shadowing at their own convenience without having to make extra time to go to a computer lab at the university. It is also hypothesized that students will start to feel less anxiety towards learning English since the shadowing system allows students to practice listening and speaking where learners want instead of in the classroom which, according to Apple, can trigger anxiety in some S&E majors [75].
When students log into their class on the app, a list of shadowing materials assigned to them appears. After selecting a shadowing exercise, they are brought to a screen which shows them the name of the material, gives them an option for showing or hiding the script, a play and stop button for playing the shadowing material, and a record button for recording the student’s voice as they shadow (see Figure 2). Students can record themselves shadowing the material as many times as they want and a graph of their pitch is generated for each recording. Students listen to the recording through earphones that have a microphone attached to capture their utterances. After students record themselves shadowing, they can play back and listen to their voice. As Hamada noted [79], this type of self-monitoring may help students develop autonomy, enhancing the efficacy of the shadowing practice. Multiple recordings can be made of the same material and students can choose which recording to submit to their teacher.

3.3. Shadowing Intervention

Four teachers in charge of a total of 30 academic writing classes which met weekly, with an average of 37 students each, assigned students shadowing homework weekly to complete outside of class through their shadowing application on their tablets, smart phones, or computers. In order to gauge listening comprehension gains by use of the shadowing system, the lead researcher on the team Kayoko Ito, conducted as a pre-intervention and post-intervention TOEIC practice listening tests to examine how students’ proficiency levels changed. Results indicate that if students practiced shadowing for over an hour a week, their TOEIC test scores improved (t = 1.96, p = 0.05). Furthermore, students who first registered high scores on the tests maintained their high scores [11,20]. This is significant because university students’ standardized test scores tend to decrease after their first year [8,9]. In other words, the results suggest that the shadowing intervention might have had an impact on students maintaining or improving their scores since most students were not taking additional listening comprehension classes.
Following the pre-intervention TOEIC test, the shadowing system was introduced to students in class. Students were taught how to login, download necessary extensions for the system, and do pre- and post-shadowing. They were also taught how to maneuver the interface which included locating the shadowing material to be studied, listening to the shadowing material, reading the script for the shadowing material, making recordings of their shadowing, listening to their recordings, and submitting shadowing recordings to their teacher. Most of the students were taking classes in CALL classrooms, with the exception of ten classes. For students in CALL classrooms, students accessed the shadowing system through Windows computers with microphones attached.
Over 12 weeks, from April to July 2015, students were assigned five different shadowing materials weekly and asked to submit their what they considered their best shadowed recordings for each material to their teacher through the shadowing system. The shadowing materials originated from texts produced to help learners prepare for Section 3 (short conversations) and Section 4 (short talks) of TOEIC with agreement from the publishers. Below is an example of one of the shadowing materials [83] (p. 57):
Man: Excuse me, ma’am. I’d like to open a checking account here and make about a $30,000 deposit. Please tell me what to do.
Woman: Just fill out this form, but with that amount, you may want to put a part of it into a certificate of time deposit. It’ll give you a much higher interest rate.
Man: I see. That’s a kind of savings account, right?
Woman: That’s correct, but you leave the money in the account for a set period of time, from three months up to several years. You shouldn’t take it out until it matures, or there will be a penalty.
Man: OK, then, I’ll put $20,000 into one of those and the remaining $10,000 in checking.
Question: What does the woman recommend the man do?
Students were also encouraged, though not required, to do additional shadowing practice with other uploaded materials whether they were teacher-uploaded or other materials made available to all students for the TOEIC test and Eiken test, an English proficiency test specific to Japan. In accordance with Noels’ suggestion for enhancing student motivation [15], providing both student autonomy and a teacher-guide in this manner, may enhance student engagement. All shadowing materials were up to one minute in length, and included American, Australian, and UK varieties of English.
Students were allowed to proceed in their shadowing practice with pre-shadowing or post-shadowing, and with or without viewing the scripts. Similar to the Foote and McDonough study [39], the students were allowed to choose their favored method of shadowing practice to more accurately simulate how students would shadow if not in a research study.

3.4. Instrumentation

This study used post-task protocol analysis, as suggested by Murphey [37] which helps elucidate how learners evaluate their ability after practicing shadowing over a prolonged period of time. A 47-item questionnaire in Japanese was administered to students prior to and after 14 weeks of shadowing study through SurveyMonkey (www.surveymonkey.com). All items besides those asking for demographic data and study time were formatted as six-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree; 2 = disagree; 3 = slightly disagree: 4 = partly disagree 5 = agree; 6 = strongly agree) questions with no neutral option.
The scales for the questionnaire (see Appendix A) were adapted from previously administered and tested surveys exploring the L2 Self-Motivation System but instead of asking about English overall, the items were framed to ask specifically about listening comprehension. Items were adapted from Taguchi et al. [84], Suzuki et al. [85], and Ryan [21]. The number of items per hypothetical construct was:
  • Instrumental (I) (6).
  • Linguistic self-confidence (LSC) (4).
  • Anxiety (A) (5).
  • Attitudes towards listening comprehension practice in the L2 (1).
  • Interest in oral communication in the L2 (IL) (6).
  • Ideal L2 self (ILS) (9).
In addition to these items, questions regarding how often students meet speakers of English, hear English spoken in the media, and listen to music in English were included. There were also questions regarding
  • Number of friends and family who speak English;
  • Experience in foreign countries, both English-speaking and not-English speaking;
  • Self-assessed level of listening and speaking proficiency;
  • Past and intended time studying English; and
  • Gender.
An additional seven items were added to the post-intervention questionnaire to gauge students’ enjoyment of the practice of shadowing, perception of its efficacy for improving listening comprehension, and intention to use shadowing in the future. A question asking students to report how much shadowing practice they completed every week was also added.
The pre-intervention survey was administered during the first week of class. The post-shadowing survey was administered during the 14th week of class. Students who had access to computers in the classroom completed the survey at the end of class, while those with no access completed the surveys during or outside of class on their mobile phones or own computers, depending on the teacher’s instructions. An auto-generated compilation of the survey results appeared on respondents’ displays immediately after completion of the survey.

4. Analytical Procedures and Results

4.1. Analytical Procedures

After removing responses from students that did not identify as Japanese (n = 18) and cases due to missing data, the responses of 949 participants for the pre-test and 747 for the post-test were analyzed with SPSS. Since individual identification numbers were not given, the data was treated as two separate groups to be compared.
Several tests were conducted on the pre- and post- intervention data:
  • Independent samples t-tests to examine if there were significant changes over time in motivation and attitudes.
  • Independent sample t-tests on constructs of motivation and attitudes.
  • Independent samples t-tests on perceptions of English speaking and listening proficiency.
  • Cross tabulation to determine how contact with English changed during research period.
  • Spearman’s correlations on all items.
  • Descriptive statistics on additional items in post-test related to shadowing.
  • Reliability analysis with cross tabulation.

4.2. Changes in Motivation and Attitudes: Items

Initially, a series of independent-samples t-tests were conducted on all individual items in order to determine whether means of these items significantly changed over time. These tests were conducted on all the items associated with the constructs above (see Appendix A for the instrument, arranged by construct).
Within this section, for readability, only those tests found to achieve statistical significance will be discussed. While no significant results were found with respect to the Instrumental construct, three significant results were found with regard to the linguistic self-confidence (LSC) construct and one with regard to the anxiety (A) construct (see Table 1). Here, significant mean differences were found with regard to Question 7 (“English listening comprehension is easy”), t(1570.53) = −3.36, p < 0.01, Question 9 (“I am good at English learning English”), t(1631.56) = −2.75, p < 0.01, and Question 20 (“I know effective ways to study English listening comprehension”), t(1694) = −4.27, p < 0.001. In all of these items, there was a significantly higher mean in the post-test in comparison with the pre-test. With regard to the anxiety questions, statistical significance was found with regard to Question 10 (“I feel like other students in my class are better than me at English”), t(1694) = 2.648, p < 0.01 and the mean was higher in the pre-test.
Additionally, three significant results were found with regard to the questions relating to interest in oral communication in the L2 (IL) ((see Table 2). Specifically, a significant mean difference was found in relation to Question 11 (“I get excited when my teacher speaks to the class in English”), t(1663.210) = −1.981, p < 0.05, Question 31 (“I really want to be able to understand spoken English”), t(1694) = 2.428, p < 0.05, and Question 32 (“Being able to understand spoken English is important to me”), t(1694) = 3.300, p < 0.01. Significantly higher means were found in the post-test for Question 11, while significantly lower means were found in the post-test for Questions 31 and 32.
With respect to ideal L2 self (ILS), significance was found in relation to Question 21 (“I believe I will be able to understand spoken English with little difficulty if I keep practicing listening comprehension”), t(1640.069) = −2.420, p < 0.05, and Question 24 (“I can image myself speaking English to other English speakers like a fluent speaker in 2 years”), t(1694) = −2.359, p < 0.05. Significantly higher means were found in the post-test in Questions 21 and 24.

4.3. Changes in Motivation Analyzed by Construct

The constructs were then analyzed by calculating the means of the constituent items in SPSS, with independent-samples t-tests then conducted in order to determine whether there were significant mean differences between the pre-test and post-test scores associated with these constructs (Figure 3). Here, statistical significance was found with respect to instrumental questions, t(1371.228) = −10.999, p < 0.001 and linguistic self-confidence, t(1694) = −4.272, p < 0.001. Significantly higher means in the post-test were found in the cases of instrumental questions and linguistic self-confidence.

4.4. English Speaking and Listening Proficiency and Contact with English

Two additional analyses were conducted on these data. First, independent-samples t-tests were conducted in order to determine whether there was a significant increase in English proficiency rating over time. This t-test was found to achieve statistical significance, t(1626.538) = 4.171, p < 0.001, with the descriptive statistics associated with these measures indicating that self-rated proficiency significantly improved over time. Additionally, a cross tabulation was conducted between how often respondents met speakers of English comparing pre-test and post-test scores. The results of this analysis indicated that responses to this measure only changed minimally between pre-test and post-test administrations of the survey.

4.5. Correlations

A series of additional Spearman’s correlations were conducted to assess the relationship between self-reported frequency of shadowing (“How much time did you practice shadowing every week this semester?”) and all of the items in the survey. With respect to this set of correlations, statistical significance was found with respect to the majority of the variables. Specifically, statistical significance was found in relation to all items except for Questions 8, 10, 16, and 18. Primarily, only questions that registered weak to strong correlations will be reported here.
All instrumental questions items reported correlations of weak to moderate. Linguistic self-confidence items Question 9 (“I am good at learning English”), ρ = 0.136, p < 0.001 and Question 20 (“I know effective ways to study English listening comprehension”), ρ = 0.177, p < 0.001 registered weak to moderate correlations.
Interest in communication in the L2 questions also registered weak to moderate correlations: Question 11 (“I get excited when my teacher speaks to the class in English”), ρ = 0.224, p < 0.001; Question 13 (“I get excited when my teacher speaks to me in English”), ρ = 0.242, p < 0.001; Question 15 (“I get excited when English is spoken to me by other people from my country speak to me in English”), ρ = 0.186, p < 0.001; Question 18 (“I get excited when English is spoken to me by foreigners”), ρ = 0.191, p < 0.001, ); Question 31 (“I really want to be able to understand spoken English”), ρ = 0.209, p < 0.001, and Question 32 (“Being able to understand spoken English is important to me”), ρ = 0.230, p < 0.001.
For anxiety items, negative, very weak to weak correlations were found for Question 12 (“I get uneasy when my teacher speaks to the class in English”), ρ = −0.077, p < 0.01 and Question 14 (“I feel uneasy when English is spoken to me by other people from my country”), ρ = −0.067, p < 0.01. The more students shadowed, the more their anxiety in relation to these items decreased.
Positive weak to moderate correlations were also found with most ideal L2 self items: Question 21 (“I believe I will be able to understand spoken English with little difficulty if I keep practicing shadowing”), ρ = 0.147, p < 0.001; Question 22 (“I can imagine myself listening to news in English with little difficulty 2 years from now”), ρ = 0.207, p < 0.001; Question 23 (“I can imagine myself listening to new in English with little difficulty 4 years from now”), ρ = 0.242, p < 0.001; Question 24 (“I can imagine myself speaking English to other English speakers like a fluent speaker in 2 years”), ρ = 0.176, p < 0.001; Question 25 (“I can imagine myself speaking English to other English speakers like a fluent speaker in 4 years”), ρ = 0.231, p < 0.001; Question 26 (“I can imagine myself speaking and listening to English with little difficulty”), ρ = 0.239, p < 0.001; Question 27 (“I can imagine myself using English in a variety of ways in two years”), ρ = 0.200, p < 0.001; Question 28 (“I can imagine myself using English in a variety of ways in 4 years”), ρ = 0.234, p < 0.001, and Question 29 (“I can imagine myself being able to understand almost all spoken English in the future), ρ = 0.196, p < 0.001).
For attitudes towards listening comprehension and shadowing, the items with weak to moderate correlations include Question 30 (“I really like doing listening comprehension practice”), ρ = 0.292, p < 0.001; Question 36 (“Shadowing is interesting”), ρ = 0.303, p < 0.001; Question 37 (“Shadowing is boring”), ρ = −0.164, p < 0.001; Question 38 (“My listening comprehension skills improved thanks to shadowing”), ρ = 0.309, p < 0.001; Question 39 (“Shadowing is an effective technique for studying English”), ρ = 0.266, p < 0.001; Question 40 (“Thanks to shadowing I want to do more listening comprehension practice”), ρ = 0.319, p < 0.001. Of note, there was a very weak to weak correlation for Question 41 (“I find myself doing shadowing in my everyday life”), ρ = 0.181, p < 0.001. A moderate to strong correlation was found with Question 42 (In the future, I want to use shadowing to practice listening comprehension), ρ = 0.437, p < 0.001.Finally, improvements in English listening proficiency, ρ = −0.114, p < 0.001 were found with higher rates of shadowing (note the scale was opposite to the Likert scale—see Appendix A).

4.6. Descriptive Statistics on Attitudes towards Shadowing Items

Descriptive statistics were conducted on these data in order to provide a preliminary illustration of the data collected (Table 3). The items reported here are relevant to all students’ perceptions of shadowing. It is important to note that it is likely that students who did less shadowing were less likely to report improved listening comprehension thanks to shadowing (Question 38). The median response to the item “Shadowing is interesting” was 3 (close to “Slightly Disagree” with the middle 50% falling between 2 and 4 on the scale. The medians for “Shadowing is boring,” “Shadowing is an effective way of learning English,” and “I want to use shadowing to practice listening comprehension in the future” were 4 (close to “Slightly Agree”) with a range of 3 to 4.

4.7. Reliability Analysis

In this survey, three sets of two items each were examined in order to help determine the reliability present within this study. Specifically, these pairs of items consisted of the following:
-
Question 7 (“English listening comprehension is easy”) and Question 8 (“English listening comprehension is difficult”).
-
Question 35 (“Shadowing is interesting”) and Question 36 (“Shadowing is boring”).
These analyses consisted of a series of cross tabulation tables and examined the correlation or association between responses to these sets of measures. First, with regard to English listening comprehension being easy or difficult, the focus was on individuals who agreed or disagreed to both questions, as these pairs of responses would indicate that the individual was perhaps not completing the survey consistently. In total, 155 respondents out of a total of 1769, or 8.76% provided inconsistent responses. Finally, when comparing “Shadowing is interesting” and “Shadowing is boring,” the number of inconsistent responses was determined to be 224 individuals out of a total of 770, or 29.09%. Overall, these results suggest some discrepancies with respect to individuals’ responses to these survey items.

5. Discussion

Results of the data analysis suggest that students’ linguistic self-confidence improved in relation to English listening comprehension and speaking. In addition to higher mean scores for the construct itself, there were also higher scores for three items measuring it. Furthermore, the post-test means for this construct were higher. While it could be argued that the general curriculum or outside influences caused these changes, the primary content of the classes where the shadowing intervention took place was academic writing, not listening comprehension, so the amount of listening practice in class was limited. English listening comprehension classes are only available as electives, meaning that only a small amount of students could have been influenced by these courses. Additionally, cross tabulation analysis shows that students’ contact with English did not change significantly over the semester. Finally, the results from Ito’s studies [11,20] combined with the increase in students’ self-rated proficiency as found in the data analysis, indicate that students either maintained or improved their TOEIC scores on the listening section thanks to the shadowing system, when these scores normally decline. Students who shadowed more, reported higher linguistic self-confidence and perceived their listening abilities to be higher. It is possible that there is a connection between students’ higher scores and higher linguistic self-confidence ratings.
Positive correlations with higher rates of shadowing practice and interest in communication in the L2 items suggest that shadowing may have an impact on students’ interest in communicating with teachers, fellow students, other people from their country and foreigners. The overall means, however, for this construct, as analyzed through the t-tests, registered a decrease in the post-test results. It is possible that the language gain in speaking and listening through shadowing made communicating effectively in English seem more of a possibility than before for those who did more shadowing.
Anxiety also seems to have been impacted positively, as indicated by a lower score on the item “I feel like other students in the class are better than me at English.” The assertion that shadowing helps reduce anxiety is also supported by the negative correlation found between increased shadowing time and the items related to anxiety when the teacher speaks, as well as the items regarding feelings of uneasiness when people from their country speak to them. The lack of significant change or correlation for the other items might indicate that anxiety is only lowered when students are comparing themselves to others in their class. There is a possibility that the more a person shadowed, the more their anxiety decreased.
The more time a student spent on shadowing the more positive the view of their ideal L2 self became. Furthermore, t-tests showed higher means in the post-test for the item “I can imagine myself speaking English to other English speakers like a fluent speaker in 2 years” for the t-tests. The post-test means for the ideal L2 self construct were also higher.
Finally, overall student views of the efficacy of the shadowing intervention are favorable. Additionally, the more a person shadowed, the more they found shadowing effective in improving their English and an interesting way to study. Higher rates of shadowing also correlated with increased desire to use shadowing and increased motivation to study listening in the future. Students with higher shadowing rates were more likely to use shadowing for listening and speaking practice in other ways besides the shadowing system. Exploring further what motivates these students to use shadowing in their everyday lives through qualitative studies could provide further insight. Results of the descriptive statistics indicate that students may find shadowing boring, yet still see it as an effective tool for learning English and want to use it in the future to practice listening comprehension.
Clearly, not all of the students who took part in this study experienced the same language gains, enhanced motivation, or enjoyment. However, students who did shadow more saw benefits. It is likely that the focus on TOEIC played a role in some students’ lower rates of shadowing. More autonomy in selection of materials could have interested students in the shadowing more, helping them shadow more than 15 min a week. As suggested by Kaneko [76], having role models could help students have a more solid image of their f, thus influencing motivation. The shadowing of other varieties of English besides typical “white-middle class” Englishes often found in TOEIC texts, including Japanese varieties, may help students realize that their non-native variety of English is more than sufficient for communication.
Compared to Hamada’s study of eight students who experienced shadowing in the classroom [79], students viewed shadowing more favorably overall, Results also indicate that it had a positive influence on motivation, mainly ideal L2 self, anxiety, linguistic self-confidence, and attitude towards speaking. This can be explained by the longer amount of time that learners had to become acquainted with shadowing plus the freedom they were given in choosing how to shadow. The MALL technology might also play a role in this difference. The use of technology might have made shadowing easier to practice because it allowed students the choice to study wherever and whenever they liked. Furthermore, as suggested by Noels [15], the autonomy granted by the application for students to monitor themselves and self-correct could have played a role.

6. Limitations of the Study and Future Research Direction

This study had several limitations. There was no control group and the respondents were not given ID numbers so only general tendencies could be explored because changes pertaining to each individual student could not be tracked over time. A TOEIC listening test was administered prior to and after shadowing to gauge changes in student achievement. However, since four different teachers were conducting the classes, the teachers’ different approaches might have influenced how students approached their shadowing practice, taking the TOEIC test, and their attitudes. A mixed methods approach including interviews could help deepen the understanding of students’ perceptions and how they specifically made use of the shadowing application. Nonetheless, this study did shed light on areas for further investigation and suggests that combining MALL technology and shadowing could be beneficial for students’ motivation and English proficiency.

7. Conclusions

This study began with an overview of previous research in MALL, shadowing, and motivation in the field of second language acquisition, emphasizing a need for studies on how motivation can be enhanced by combining MALL and shadowing. It then turned to a description of a quantitative longitudinal study that investigated the effects of a shadowing application for mobile devices on learner motivation, perception of listening and speaking ability, and attitudes towards listening comprehension on 1001 students (mostly first-year science majors) at a university in Japan. During the 12-week intervention, students were given five shadowing tasks to complete outside of class weekly. Statistical analysis indicated that learner motivation and attitudes were higher for students that practiced shadowing more. Overall, the students saw either maintenance of or gains in English listening proficiency levels regardless of time spent shadowing, when under usual circumstances, proficiency test scores have a tendency decrease after the first year of university. Furthermore, students viewed shadowing as an effective method for studying English, with those who shadowed more showing greater interest in using shadowing in the future, and even doing listening comprehension more. This study provides findings that support the hypothesis that studying shadowing utilizing MALL technology could be beneficial to students’ motivation and their actual and perceived English ability. In particular, students’ linguistic self-confidence and ideal L2 self were improved significantly after the shadowing intervention. Anxiety levels decreased slightly and attitude towards English slightly improved, and the more a student used the shadowing application, the more positive their image of English became.
Further studies need to be conducted that investigate how students are influenced by activities introduced in the language classroom, not only in terms of proficiency, but also in terms of motivation and attitudes, as these aspects help to sustain the language learning process. This study contributes to these types of learning by providing insights into how a MALL application, constructed to address the challenges of students with limited exposure to English outside of the classroom, may enhance students’ learning and motivation. Unlike previous studies, it provides a detailed analysis of the multifaceted nature of student motivation including instrumental motivation, linguistic self-confidence, anxiety, attitudes towards listening comprehension practice, interest in oral communication, and ideal L2 self. It also specifically focuses on oral communication rather than English as a whole. The majority of previous studies only focused on overall motivation, all four skills in English, and general interest in studying English.
The pedagogical implications are various, whether in terms of analyzing the effective use of shadowing or assessing whether or not to introduce MALL technology in the classroom in general. Additionally, giving students the liberty to do shadowing wherever and whenever makes the learning process less of a challenge. Providing a larger variety of materials to practice shadowing, or allowing students to choose their own materials to shadow may also make the practice of shadowing more interesting to students, amplifying the benefits of the practice. Finally, further studies that examine shadowing in comparison to other techniques for improving listening and speaking could shed more light on its efficacy.

Acknowledgments

I would like to express my sincere gratitude to David Beglar at Temple University and Tomoko Yashima at Kansai University for their feedback and encouragement. Most importantly, I would like to thank Kayo Ito at Kyoto University for sparking my interest in researching shadowing practice with MALL technology while also supporting me in my research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Instrumental Motivation.
Table A1. Instrumental Motivation.
Item Number Item EnglishItem Japanese
1I practice English listening comprehension because I want to be able to communicate well with other people英語のリスンニング(聴解)を練習する理由は、他の人と英語で上手くコミュニケーションでくるようになりたいからだ
2I practice English listening comprehension because I want to study abroad英語のリスンニング(聴解)を練習する理由は、留学したいからだ
3I practice English listening comprehension because I want to work abroad英語のリスンニング(聴解)を練習する理由は、海外で仕事をしたいからだ
4I practice English comprehension because I want to travel abroad英語のリスンニング(聴解)を練習する理由は、海外旅行をしたいからだ
5I practice English comprehension because I may need it later for my future job英語のリスンニング(聴解)を練習する理由は、将来の仕事に役に立つかもしれないからだ
6I practice English comprehension because I may need it later for my future research英語のリスンニング(聴解)を練習する理由は、将来の研究に役に立つかもしれないからだ
Table A2. Linguistic Self-Confidence.
Table A2. Linguistic Self-Confidence.
Item Number Item EnglishItem Japanese
7English listening comprehension is easy英語リスニング(聴解)は簡単だ
8English listening comprehension is difficult英語リスニング(聴解)は難しい
9I am good at learning English英語学習が得意だ
20I know effective ways to study English listening comprehension英語リスニング(聴解)の効果的な学習法を知っている
Table A3. Anxiety.
Table A3. Anxiety.
Item Number Item EnglishItem Japanese
10I feel like other students in my class are better than me at English私のクラスの他の生徒の方が、私より英語が上手だと感じる
12I get uneasy when my teacher speaks to the class in English先生がクラス全員に英語を話すと、不安になる
14I feel uneasy when my teacher speaks to me in English先生から英語を話されると、不安になる
16I feel uneasy when English is spoken to me by other people from my country同じ国の人から英語を話されると、不安になる
Table A4. Interest in the L2.
Table A4. Interest in the L2.
Item Number Item EnglishItem Japanese
11I get excited when my teacher speaks to the class in English先生がクラス全員に英語を話すと、わくわくする
13I get excited when my teacher speaks to me in English先生から英語を話されると、わくわくする
15I get excited when English is spoken to me by other people from my country同じ国の人から英語を話されると、わくわくする
17I get excited when English is spoken to me by foreigners外国人から英語を話されると、わくわくする
31I really want to be able to understand spoken English話されている英語を理解できるようになりたい
32Being able to understand spoken English is important to me私にとって話されている英語を理解できることは大事だ
Table A5. Intended Study Effort.
Table A5. Intended Study Effort.
Item Number Item EnglishItem Japanese
19I am working hard on improving my English listening comprehension英語リスニング(聴解)を一生懸命勉強している
Table A6. Ideal L2 Self.
Table A6. Ideal L2 Self.
Item Number Item EnglishItem Japanese
21I believe I will be able to understand spoken English with little difficulty if I keep practicing listening comprehensionもしリスニング(聴解)を学習し続けたら、口語英語をほとんど支障なく理解することができるようになると思う
22I can imagine myself listening to news in English with little difficulty 2 years from now2年後に英語のニュースをほとんど支障なく聞き取れている自分を思い描くことができる
23I can imagine myself listening to news in English with little difficulty 4 years from now4年後に英語のニュースをほとんど支障なく聞き取れている自分を思い描くことができる
24I can image myself speaking English to other English speakers like a fluent speaker in 2 years2年以内に英語話者と流暢に英語を話す自分を思い描くことができる
25I can image myself speaking English to other English speakers like a fluent speaker in 4 years4年以内に英語話者と流暢に英語を話す自分を思い描くことができる
26I often imagine myself speaking and listening to English with little difficulty自分がほとんど支障なく英語を話したり聞いたりしていることをしばしば想像する
27I can imagine myself using English in a variety of ways in 2 years2年以内に様々な方法で英語を利用している自分を思い描くことができる
28I can imagine myself using English in a variety of ways in 4 years4年以内に様々な方法で英語を利用している自分を思い描くことができる
29I can imagine myself being able to understand almost all spoken English in the future将来、ほとんどすべての口語英語を理解することができるようになると思う
Table A7. Attitudes towards Listening Comprehension Practice in the L2.
Table A7. Attitudes towards Listening Comprehension Practice in the L2.
Item Number Item EnglishItem Japanese
30I really like doing listening comprehension practice英語リスニング(聴解)の練習をするのはとても好きだ
Table A8. English Contact.
Table A8. English Contact.
Item Number Item EnglishItem Japanese
33How often do you meet speakers of English (including non-native speakers) in a week?一週間にどの程度、英語話者(非母語話者を含む)に会いますか
34How often do you hear English spoken in the media (TV, Movies, et cetera) in a week?メディア(テレビ、英語、など)で、一週間にどの程度、英語を聞きますか
35How often do you listen to English music?一週間にどの程度、英語の音楽を聞きますか
Table A9. Additional Post-Test Questions: Students’ Perceptions of the Utility of Shadowing.
Table A9. Additional Post-Test Questions: Students’ Perceptions of the Utility of Shadowing.
Item Number Item EnglishItem Japanese
36Shadowing is interestingシャドーイングは面白い
37Shadowing is boringシャドーイングはつまらない
38My listening comprehension skills improved thanks to shadowingシャドーイングの練習のおかげ、リスニングがよくなった
39Shadowing is an effective technique for studying Englishシャドーイングは英語学習に効果がある
40Thanks to shadowing, I want to do more listening comprehension practiceシャドーイングのおかげで聞き取り練習をもっとしたくなった
41I find myself doing shadowing in my every day life日常生活においてもシャドーイングをしていることがある
42In the future, I want to use shadowing to practice English listening comprehension今後、リスニングの練習の為にシャドーイングを使っていきたい
  • 43 How much did you practice shadowing this semester?
  • 今学期、一週間にどの程度、シャドーイングをしましたか?
  • Less than 15 min a week週十五分未満
  • 15 to 30 min a week 週十五分から三十分間
  • 30 min to an hour a week 週三十分から一時間
  • 1 to 2 h a week週一時間から二時間
  • More than 2 h a week週二時間以上
  •  
  • 44 English ability question:
  • English ability: Please rate your current listening ability in English by ticking one.
  • 1 Upper Intermediate level and over—Able to understand conversations general matters of daily life and topics of one’s specialty and grasp the gist of lectures and broadcasts.
  • 2 Intermediate level—Able to understand conversations about general matters of daily life.
  • 3 Lower Intermediate level—Able to understand conversations about familiar daily topics.
  • 4 Post-Beginner level—Able to understand a simple conversation such as greeting and introducing someone.
  • 5 Beginner level—Able to understand simple greetings using set words and phrases.
  •  
  • 英語力: あなたの現在のリスニングに関する英語力について次の中から一つに印をつけてください。
  •  
  • 1 中上級レベル以上—日常生活の一般的な事柄や専門的な事柄についての会話の聞き取りができ、講義や放送の大意を理解できる。
  • 2 中級レベル—日常生活の一般的な事柄に関する会話の聞き取りができる。
  • 3 初中級レベル—日常生活の身近な事柄についての会話の聞き取りができる。
  • 4 初級レベル—挨拶や人の紹介などの簡単な会話の聞き取りができる。
  • 5 基礎レベル—決まり文句を用いて簡単な挨拶の聞き取りができる。
  •  
  • 42 Have you spent more than three months in an English-speaking country in the past 10 years?
  • 過去十年間、少なくとも計3ヶ月以上英語圏に滞在したことがありますか?
  • Yes No
  • はい いいえ
  •  
  • 43 Have you spent more than three months in an English-speaking country in your life?
  • 少なくとも計3ヶ月以上英語圏に滞在したことがありますか?
  • Yes No
  • はい いいえ
  •  
  • 44 Have you ever been to an English-speaking country in your life?
  • 今まで英語圏に滞在したことがありますか?
  • Yes No
  • はい いいえ
  •  
  • 45 Gender 性別
  • Male 男 Female 女
  •  
  • 46 Nationality 国籍
  • Japanese 日本人
  • Non-Japanese 外国人
  •  
  • 47 Major
  • Humanities 総合人間学部
  • Literature 文学部
  • Education 教育学部
  • Law 法学部
  • Economics経済学部
  • Medicine 医学部
  • Engineering工学部
  • Agriculture 農学部

References

  1. Maeda, M. 2010. Uniqlo, Rakuten make official language English. Japan Center for Economic Research. July 15. Available online: www.jcer.or.jp/eng/research/pdf/maeda20100715e.pdf (accessed on 15 January 2017).
  2. Teeter, J. 2014. Exploring the potential of TBLT for academic abstract writing. OnTask J. JALT Task Based Learn. SIG 4: 9–20. [Google Scholar]
  3. Kubota, R. 2002. The impact of globalization on language teaching in Japan. In Globalization and Language Teaching. Edited by D. Block and D. Cameron. London, UK: Routledge, pp. 13–28. [Google Scholar]
  4. Monbukagakushō (Ministry of Education, C., Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT), Shōgakkō no Aratana Gaikokugokyōiku ni Okeru Hojo Kyōzai (Hi, Friends! Story Books) Sakusei ni Tsuite (dai 3 4 Gakunen-yō)’ (On the Creation of Supplementary Textbooks for the New Foreign Language Curriculum in Elementary Schools (Hi, Friends! Story Books) (for 3rd and 4th Grade). 2016. Available online: http://www.mext.go.jp/a_menu/kokusai/gaikokugo/1370103.htm (accessed on 20 February 2017).
  5. Monbukagakushō (Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT), Heisei 27-Nendo “Eigo Kyōiku Jisshi Jōkyō Chōsa’ no Kekka ni Tsuite” [On the Results of “Survey on the Implementation Status of English Education” in FY2015]. 2016. Available online: http://www.mext.go.jp/a_menu/kokusai/gaikokugo/1369258.htm (accessed on 22 February 2017).
  6. Okazaki, T. 2017. Eigo kyōiku to senjūmin-zoku gengo fukkō Maori-go, ainu-go o chūshin ni [Education in English and Indigenous Language Revitalization-focusing on Maori and Ainu Languages]. In Gengo to Kyōiku—Kattō Kara Daisan no Michi e (Language and Education—From Conflict to Third Way). Edited by T. Sugino, F. Tanaka and K. Hatano. Tokyo, Japan: Meiji Shoten, in press. [Google Scholar]
  7. Hulstijn, J.H. 2001. Intentional and incidental second language vocabulary learning: A reappraisal of elaboration, rehearsal and automaticity. In Cognition and Second Language Instruction. Edited by P. Robinson. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 258–286. [Google Scholar]
  8. Sumida, A., W. Kobayashi, and T. Gutierrez. 2015. Eigo shūjuku-do tesuto ni mi rareru daigaku 1-nensei no eigodjikara no suii [Tendencies in English proficiency of university first year students as seen through analysis of English proficiency test results]. Nihon Univ. Inst. Hum. Soc. Sci. Bull. 90: 131–144. Available online: http://www.chs.nihon-u.ac.jp/institute/human/kiyou/90/8.pdf (accesed on 10 June 2017).
  9. Mikada, H. 2016. Kōgakubu kōgaku kenkyū-ka ni okeru IR kekka to akushon [IR Results and Actions at the Faculty of Engineering]. In Dai 20-kai Kyōtodaigaku Zengaku Kyōiku Shinpojiumu Hōkoku-Sho: Dēta to Risō ni Motodzuite Kangaeru Kyōdai no Kyōiku Kaikaku:Nyūshi Kara Daigakuin Kyōiku Made [The 20th Kyoto University General Education Symposium Report: On Educational Reform Based on Data and Ideals—From Admissions to Graduate School Education]. Kyoto, Japan: Kyoto University Faculty Development Committee, Available online: http://www.fd.kyoto-u.ac.jp/activity/shimposium2016.pdf (accessed on 15 June 2017).
  10. Kanamaru, T. 2014. Study a new kind of English offered by Kyoto University. Arts Sci. Newslett. 19: 5. Available online: http://www.z.k.kyoto-u.ac.jp/pdf/link/link0339.pdf?1417539122 (accesed on 10 June 2017).
  11. Ito, K., J. Teeter, A. Murakami, and M. Nakano. Strengthening English listening skills: The development of a self-study shadowing system for tablets and smart phones. In Proceedings of the 6th Joint Foreign Language Education and Technology Conference, Cambridge, MA, USA, 12–15 August 2015. [Google Scholar]
  12. Gardner, R.C. 1985. Social Psychology and Second Language Learning. London, UK: Edward Arnold. [Google Scholar]
  13. Ellis, R. 1997. Second Language Acquisition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  14. Ajzen, I. 2005. Attitude, Personality and Behavior, 2nd ed. New York, NY, USA: Open University Press. [Google Scholar]
  15. Noels, K.A. 2013. Learning Japanese; Learning English: Promoting Motivation Through Autonomy, Competence and Relatedness. In Language Learning Motivation in Japan. Edited by M.T. Apple, D. Da Silva and T. Fellner. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 15–34. [Google Scholar]
  16. Bernaus, M., and R.C. Gardner. 2008. Teacher motivation strategies, student perceptions, student motivation, and English achievement. Mod. Lang. J. 92: 378–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Dörnyei, Z. 2009. The L2 Motivational Self System. In Motivation, Language Identity and the L2 Self. Edited by Z. Dörnyei and E. Ushioda. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 9–42. [Google Scholar]
  18. Harmon, D., II. 2014. Teaching Fluency and Rhythm to Learners Using Reading Aloud and Shadowing Dennis Harmon II. In Teaching the SoundsSystem of English. Edited by N. Barbieri and M. Giordano. Tokyo, Japan: Temple University, Japan Campus, pp. 33–43. [Google Scholar]
  19. Rico, M., M.J. Naranjo, G. Delicado, N. Plaza, and E. Domínguez. Let’s move: Mobile learning for motivation in language acquisition. In Proceedings of the 7th ICT for Language Learning, Florence, Italy, 13–14 November 2014; pp. 91–94. [Google Scholar]
  20. Ito, K., and M. Nakano. In Risuningu kyōka no tame ni shadōingu o daburetto ya sumaho de jishu gakushū dekiru shisutemu kōchiku [Strengthening English listening skills: The development of a self-study shadowing system for tablets and smart phones]. In Proceedings of the 55th Annual Conference of The Japan Association for Language Education and Technology, Kansai University, Osaka, Japan, 4–6 August 2015; pp. 74–75. [Google Scholar]
  21. Ryan, S. 2009. Self and identity in L2 motivation in Japan: The ideal L2 self and Japanese learners of English. In Motivation, Language Identity and the L2 Self. Edited by Z. Dörnyei and E. Ushioda. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 120–143. [Google Scholar]
  22. Pigott, J. 2013. Motivation and complex systems theory: An exploratory view of the motivation of four Japanese university students. OnCue 6: 27–47. Available online: http://www.jaltcue.org/files/OnCUE/OCJ6.2/OCJ6.2_pp27-47_Pigott.pdf (accessed on 14 June 2017).
  23. Sharples, M., M. Milrad, I. Arnedillo Sánchez, and G. Vavoula. 2009. Mobile Learning: Small Devices, Big Issues. In Technology-Enhanced Learning: Principles and Products. Edited by N. Balacheff and S. Ludvigsen. Berlin, Germany: Springer, pp. 233–249. [Google Scholar]
  24. Valarmathi, K.E. 2011. Mobile assisted language learning. J. Technol. ELT 2: 1–8. Available online: https://sites.google.com/site/journaloftechnologyforelt/archive/april2011/mobileassistedlanguagelearning (accessed on 14 June 2017).
  25. Poushter, J. 2016. Smartphone ownership and Internet usage continues to climb in emerging economies. Pew Res. Center Glob. Attitudes Trends 22. Available online: www.pewglobal.org/2016/02/22/smartphone-ownership-and-internet-usage-continues-to-climb-in-emerging-economies/ (accessed on 14 June 2017).
  26. Senao, A. 2016. Chōsa dēta: Kōkōsei no sumātofon shoyū-ritsu wa 93. 0%, 2014nen yori 13. 5-pointo appu [Survey data: The ownership rate of smartphones by high school students is 93.0%, 13.5 points higher than 2014]. Mobile Marketing Laboratory. September 20. Available online: https://mmdlabo.jp/investigation/detail_1605.html (accessed on 12 June 2017).
  27. Prensky, M. 2001. Digital natives, digital immigrants. Horizon 9: 1–6. Available online: http://www.marcprensky.com/writing/Prensky%20%20Digital%20Natives,%20Digital%20Immigrants%20-%20Part1.pdf (accessed on 12 June 2017).
  28. Pedró, F. 2012. Trusting the unknown: The effects of technology use in education. In The Global Information Technology Report. Geneva, Switzerland: The World Economic Forum, pp. 135–146. [Google Scholar]
  29. Wu, W.-H., Y.-C.J. Wu, C.-Y. Chen, H.-Y. Kao, C.-H. Lin, and S.-H. Huang. 2012. Review of trends from mobile learning studies: A meta-analysis. Comput. Educ. 59: 817–827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Al-Jarf, R. 2012. Mobile technology and student autonomy in oral skill acquisition. In Left to My Own Devices: Learner Autonomy and MALL Innovation and LEADERSHIP in English Language Teaching. Edited by J. Díaz-Vera. Bingley, UK: Emerald Group, pp. 105–130. [Google Scholar]
  31. Liu, T.-Y., and Y. Chu. 2010. Using ubiquitous games in an English listening and speaking course: Impact on learning outcomes and motivation. Comput. Educ. 55: 630–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Sato, T., F. Murase, and T. Burden. 2015. Is mobile-assisted language learning really useful? An examination of recall automatization and learner autonomy. In Proceedings of the EUROCALL Conference, Padova, Italy, 26–29 August 2015; Edited by F. Helm, L. Bradley, M. Guarda and S. Thouësny. Padova, Italy: Research-Publishing.net, pp. 495–501. [Google Scholar]
  33. Thornton, P., and C. Houser. 2005. Using mobile phones in English education in Japan. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 21: 217–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Bovee, N., and J. Stewart. The utility of shadowing. In Proceedings of the JALT2008 Conference Proceedings, Tokyo, Japan, 31 October–3 November 2009; pp. 888–900. Available online: http://jalt-publications.org/recentpdf/proceedings/2008/E158.pdf (accessed on 15 February 2017).
  35. Harbison, D.C., Jr., R.J. Porter Jr., and E.A. Tobey. 1989. Shadowed and simple reaction times in stutterers and nonstutterers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 86: 1277–1284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Shlesinger, M., and P. Pochhacker. 2011. Aptitude for Interpreting. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. [Google Scholar]
  37. Murphey, T. 2001. Exploring conversational shadowing. Lang. Teach. Res. 5: 128–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hamada, Y. 2014. The effectiveness of pre- and post-shadowing in improving listening comprehension skills. Lang. Teach. 38: 3–10. [Google Scholar]
  39. Foote, J.A., and K. McDonough. 2017. Using shadowing with mobile technology to improve L2 pronunciation. J. Second Lang. Pronunciation 3: 34–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Rongna, A., and R. Hayahi. 2012. Accuracy of Japanese pitch accent rises during and after shadowing training. In Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Speech Prosody, Shanghai, China, 22–25 May 2012; Shanghai, China: Tongji University Press, pp. 143–146. [Google Scholar]
  41. Yamane, S., E. Saito, and T. Yashima. 2004. Ripītingu ga eigo purosodī no shūtoku ni ataeru kōka [The effects of repeating on acquiring prosody]. Kotobano Kagaku Kenkyuu 5: 45–51. [Google Scholar]
  42. Torikai, M., K. Tamai, T. Someya, M. Tanaka, C. Tsuruda, and T. Nishimura. 2003. Hajimete no shadoingu [Shadowing for the first time]. Tokyo, Japan: Gakken Marketing. [Google Scholar]
  43. Mochizuki, H. 2006. Application of shadowing to TEFL in Japan: The case of junior high school students. Stud. Engl. Lang. Teach. KELES 29: 29–44. Available online: http://www.keles.jp/activity/selt/backnumber/2006_no29/ (accessed on 10 February 2017).
  44. Nakayama, T., and A. Suzuki. 2012. Gakushū hōryaku no chigai ga shadowingu no fukushōryō ni ataeru eikyō [A study on learning strategies in shadowing training]. Rimediaru kyōiku kenkyū [J. Jpn. Assoc. Dev. Educ.] 7: 131–140. Available online: https://www.jstage.jst.go.jp/article/jade/7/1/7_KJ00008635818/_pdf (accessed on 15 February 2017).
  45. Mori, Y. 2011. Shadowing with Oral Reading: Effects of Combined Training on the Improvement of Japanese EFL Learners’ Prosody. Lang. Educ. Technol. 48: 1–22. Available online: http://ci.nii.ac.jp/naid/110008686398 (accessed on 15 February 2017).
  46. Hamada, Y. 2016. Shadowing: Who benefits and how? Uncovering a booming EFL teaching technique for listening comprehension. Lang. Teach. Res. 20: 35–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Tamai, K. 1992. “Follow-up” no chōkairyoku ni oyoba su kōka oyobi “follow-up” nōryoku to chōkairyoku no kankei [The effects of followup tasks on listening comprehension and the relationship between follow-up ability and listening comprehension]. STEP Bull., 48–62. Available online: https://www.eiken.or.jp/center_for_research/pdf/bulletin_archives/vol_04.pdf (accessed on 10 June 2017).
  48. Tamai, K. 2005. Risuningu Shidō-Hō to Shite no Shadōin gu no Kōka ni Kansuru Kenkyū [Research on the Effects of Using Shadowing as a Method of Teaching Listening]. Tokyo, Japan: Kazama Shobo. [Google Scholar]
  49. Kato, S. 2009. Kōkū eigo nōryoku shōmei shūtoku o mezashita risuningu shidō no kōsatsu [Listening activities for the acquisition of Aviation English proficiency test]. Bull. Chiba Univ. Lang. Cult., 47–59. Available online: http://mitizane.ll.chiba-u.jp/meta-bin/mt-pdetail.cgi?cd=00066721 (accessed on 10 June 2017).
  50. Kadota, S. 2007. Shadoingu to Ondoku no Kagaku [The Science of Shadowing and Oral Reading]. Tokyo, Japan: Cosmopier. [Google Scholar]
  51. Nakayama, T., and T. Armstrong. 2015. Weak forms in shadowing: How can Japanese EFL learnersperform better on shadowing tasks? Jissen Women’s Univ. CLEIP J., 13–21. Available online: https://jissen.repo.nii.ac.jp/?action=repository_uri&item_id=1323&file_id=22&file_no=2 (accessed on 10 June 2017).
  52. Lightbown, P.M., and N.M. Spada. 2013. How Languages Are Learned. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  53. Masgoret, A.-M., and R.C. Gardner. 2003. Attitudes, Motivation, and Second Language Learning: A Meta-Analysis of Studies Conducted by Gardner and Associates. Lang. Learn. 53: 167–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Noels, K.A., L.G. Pelletier, and R.J. Vallerand. 2000. Why are you learning a second language? Motivational orietation and self-determination theory. Lang. Learn. 50: 57–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Gardner, R.C., and W.E. Lambert. 1959. Motivational variables in second language acquisition. Can. J. Psychol. 13: 191–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Dornyei, Z. 2001. Teaching and Researching Motivation. Harlow, UK: Longman. [Google Scholar]
  57. Gardner, R.C. 2001. Integrative motivation and second language learning. In Motivation and Second Language Acquisition. Edited by Z. Dornyei and R. Schmidt. Honolulu, HI, USA: University of Hawaii Press, pp. 1–20. [Google Scholar]
  58. Crookes, G., and R. Schmidt. 1991. Motivation: Reopening the research agenda. Lang. Learn. 41: 469–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Noels, K. 2001. New orientations in language learning motivation: Toward a model of intrinsic, extrinsic, and integrative orientations and motivations. In Motivation and Second Language Acquisition. Honolulu, HI, USA: University of Hawaii, pp. 43–68. [Google Scholar]
  60. Peirce, B.N. 1995. Social Identity, Investment, and Language Learning. TESOL Q. 29: 9–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Irie, K., and D. Brewster. 2013. One Curriculum, three Stories: Ideal L2 self and L2-Self-Discrepancy profiles. In Language Learning Motivation in Japan. Edited by M. Apple, D.D. Silva and T. Fellner. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 110–128. [Google Scholar]
  62. Munezane, Y. 2013. Motivation, Ideal L2 Self, and valuing of Global English. In Language Learning Motivation in Japan. Edited by M. Apple, D.D. Silva and T. Fellner. Bristol, UK: Multilingual matters, pp. 152–168. [Google Scholar]
  63. Matsuoka, R., and A. Rahimi. 2010. The positive effect of conference participation on reducing L2 communication apprehension. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 9: 1845–1854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Ehrman, M.E. 1996. An exploration of adult language learners’ motivation, self-efficacy, and anxiety. In Language Learning Motivation: Pathways to the New Century. Edited by R.L. Oxford. Honolulu, HI, USA: University of Hawaii, pp. 81–103. [Google Scholar]
  65. Noels, K.A., G. Pon, and R. Clement. 1996. Language, Identity, and Adjustment. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 15: 246–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Tremblay, P.F., and R.C. Gardner. 1995. Expanding the Motivation Construct in Language Learning. Mod. Lang. J. 79: 505–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Julkunen, K. 1989. Situation and Task Specific Motivation in Foreign-Language Learning and Teaching. Joensuu, Finland: University of Joensuu. [Google Scholar]
  68. Andrade, M., and K. Williams. 2009. Foreign language learning anxiety in Japanese EFL university classes: Physical, emotional, expressive, and verbal reactions. Sophia Jr. Coll. Fac. J. 29: 1–24. Available online: https://www.jrc.sophia.ac.jp/pdf/research/bulletin/ver2901.pdf (accessed on 10 February 2017).
  69. Yashima, T., K. Noels, T. Shizuka, O. Takeuchi, S. Yamane, and K. Yoshizawa. 2008. The interplay of classroom anxiety, intrinsic motivation, and gender in the Japanese EFL context. Kansai Univ. J. Foreign Lang. Educ. Res. 17: 41–64. [Google Scholar]
  70. Yashima, T. 2002. Willingness to Communicate in a Second Language: The Japanese EFL Context. Mod. Lang. J. 86: 54–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Dörnyei, Z. 2005. The Psychology of the Language Learner: Individual Differences in Second Language Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  72. Johnson, M.P. 2013. A longitudinal perspective on EFL learning motivation in Japanese engineering students. In Language Learning Motivation in Japan. Edited by M. Apple and D.D.S.T. Fellner. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 189–205. [Google Scholar]
  73. Kimura, Y., Y. Nakata, and T. Okumura. 2001. Language Learning Motivation of EFL Learners in Japan-A Cross-Sectional Analysis of Various Learning Milieus. JALT J. 23: 47–68. Available online: http://jalt-publications.org/archive/jj/2001a/art3.pdf (accessed on 13 June 2017).
  74. Falout, J., and M. Maruyama. 2004. A Comparative Study of Proficiency and Learner Demotivation. Lang. Teach. 28: 3–9. Available online: http://jalt-publications.org/files/pdf-article/tlt38.1-art1.pdf (accessed on 10 June 2017).
  75. Apple, M.T., J. Falout, and G. Hill. 2013. Exploring classroom-based constructs of EFL motivation for science and engineering students in Japan. In Language Learner Motivation in Japan. Edited by M.T. Apple, D. Da Silva and T. Fellner. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 54–74. [Google Scholar]
  76. Kaneko, E. 2012. On the motivation of science majors learning English a foreign language: A case study from Japan. OnCUE 6: 3–26. Available online: jaltcue.org/files/OnCUE/OCJ6.2/OCJ6.2_pp3-26_Kaneko.pdf (accessed on 10 June 2017).
  77. Chen, Y., C. Liska, and T.J. Smith. 2017. Mobile-assisted narrative writing practice for young English language learners from a funds of knowledge approach. Lang. Learn. Technol. 21: 28–41. Available online: http://llt.msu.edu/issues/february2017/chencargersmith.pdf (accessed on 10 June 2017).
  78. Colibaba, A., O. Ursa, C. Arhip, I. Gheorgiu, and S. Colibaba. Take Care of Mobile Learning! In Proceedings of the 7th ICT for Language Learning, Florence, Italy, 13–14 November 2014; pp. 319–322. [Google Scholar]
  79. Hamada, Y. 2015. Uncovering shadowing as an EFL teaching technique for listening: Learners’ perceptions, self-confidence, and motivation. Akita Daigaku Kyōyō Kiso Kyōiku Kenkyū Nenpō [Akita University Annual Report on General Education Research]. pp. 9–22. Available online: http://air.lib.akita-u.ac.jp/dspace/bitstream/10295/2779/1/kkiso17(9).pdf (accessed on 10 February 2017).
  80. Li-Chi, L. 2009. A Study of Using Shadowing As a Task in Junior High School EFL Program in Taiwan. Taipei, Taiwan: National Taiwan University of Science and Technology. [Google Scholar]
  81. Saishin kyōto fu daigaku hensa chi ichiran 2017 (Latest Kyoto Prefecture academic deviation rankings list). 2017. Daigaku Juken Boy. Available online: http://daigakujyuken.boy.jp/indexkyoutofu.html/ (accessed on 10 September 2017).
  82. Aotani, M. Eigo Gakushū. TOEFL Setsumei Kai (hi Eiri. Kōkai. Muryō)—SUPĪKINGU to Sōgō Ryoku (Studying English. TOEFL Briefing Session (Non-Profit, Open to the Public, Free)ーSpeaking and Overall Ability) Aotani Homepage 2013. Available online: http://aoitani.net/TOEFL_12.html (accessed on 10 June 2017).
  83. Obunsha, Eiken 1-kyū yosōmondai doriru shin shiken taiō-ban (onsei DL-tsuki) [Eiken Level 1 Practice Drills for the new version (with recording)]. 2015, Tokyo, Japan: Obunsha.
  84. Taguchi, T., M. Magid, and M. Papi. 2010. Appendix B. In Questionnaires in Second Language Research: Construction, Administration, and Processing. Edited by Z. Dörnyei and E. Ushioda. New York, NY, USA: London, UK: Taylor & Francis. [Google Scholar]
  85. Suzuki, W., A. Leis, A. Ando, and N. Itagaki. 2011. Nihonjin Daigakusei no Eigo Gakushū ni Taisuru Dōkidzuke Chōsa—Dörnyei no L 2 Motivational Self System Nimotozuite—Miyagi Educational University Research Center for Education in International Understanding Annual Report. pp. 15–33. Available online: http://rceiu.miyakyo-u.ac.jp/img-nenpou2010/ron3%20saai.pdf (accessed on 10 June 2017).
Figure 1. Percentage of participants by major.
Figure 1. Percentage of participants by major.
Languages 02 00019 g001
Figure 2. Shadowing recording screen with English translation.
Figure 2. Shadowing recording screen with English translation.
Languages 02 00019 g002
Figure 3. Pre- and post-test means by construct.
Figure 3. Pre- and post-test means by construct.
Languages 02 00019 g003
Table 1. Linguistic Self-Confidence (LSC) and Anxiety (A) items pre- and post-test.
Table 1. Linguistic Self-Confidence (LSC) and Anxiety (A) items pre- and post-test.
Levene’s Test for Equality of Variancest-Test for Equality of Means
FSig.tdfSig. (2-Tailed)Mean DifferenceStd.Error Difference95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
LowerUpper
7. English listening comprehension is easy (LSC)Equal variances assumed4.020.05−3.3816940.001−0.180.05−0.28−0.07
Equal variances not assumed −3.361570.530.001−0.180.05−0.28−0.07
9. I am good at learning English (LSC)Equal variances assumed6.670.01−2.7316940.006−0.150.05−0.26−0.04
Equal variances not assumed −2.751631.560.01−0.150.05−0.26−0.04
20. I know effective ways to study English listening comprehension (LSC)Equal variances assumed0.0610.81−4.2716940.000−0.240.06−0.35−0.13
Equal variances not assumed −4.271599.220.000−0.240.06−0.35−0.13
10. I feel like other students in my class are better than me at English (A)Equal variances assumed0.030.862.6516940.0080.170.070.050.3
Equal variances not assumed 2.6515960.0080.170.070.050.3
Table 2. Interest in the L2 (IL) and ideal L2 self (ILS) items pre- and post-test.
Table 2. Interest in the L2 (IL) and ideal L2 self (ILS) items pre- and post-test.
Levene’s Test for Equality of Variancest-Test for Equality of Means
FSig.tdfSig. (2-Tailed)Mean DifferenceStd.Error Difference95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
LowerUpper
11. I get excited when my teacher speaks to the class in English (IL)Equal variances assumed18.670.00−1.9616940.05−0.120.06−0.25−0.00
Equal variances not assumed −1.981663.210.05−0.120.06−0.25−0.00
31. I really want to be able to understand spoken English (IL)Equal variances assumed0.160.692.4316940.020.140.070.030.25
Equal variances not assumed 2.441633.970.020.140.060.030.25
32. Being able to understand spoken English is important to me (IL)Equal variances assumed1.380.243.316940.000.180.050.070.29
Equal variances not assumed 3.321631.930.000.180.050.070.29
21. I believe I will be able to understand spoken English with little difficulty if I keep practicing listening comprehension (ILS)Equal variances assumed3.9510.05−2.416940.02−0.140.06−0.26−0.03
Equal variances not assumed −2.421640.070.02−0.140.06−0.26−0.03
24. I can image myself speaking English to other English speakers like a fluent speaker in 2 years (ILS)Equal variances assumed0.030.86−2.3616940.02−0.130.05−0.23−0.02
Equal variances not assumed −2.361613.070.02−0.130.05−0.23−0.02
Table 3. Select attitudes towards shadowing.
Table 3. Select attitudes towards shadowing.
36. Shadowing is Interesting 37. Shadowing is Boring39. Shadowing is an Effective Way of Learning English42. In the Future, I Want to Use Shadowing to Practice Listening Comprehension
Mean2.97923.74033.82993.4766
Median3.00004.00004.00004.0000
Std. Deviation1.190381.222911.131601.21225
Range5.005.005.005.00
Percentiles252.00003.00003.00003.0000
754.00004.00004.00004.0000

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Teeter, J.L. Improving Motivation to Learn English in Japan with a Self-Study Shadowing Application. Languages 2017, 2, 19. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2040019

AMA Style

Teeter JL. Improving Motivation to Learn English in Japan with a Self-Study Shadowing Application. Languages. 2017; 2(4):19. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2040019

Chicago/Turabian Style

Teeter, Jennifer Louise. 2017. "Improving Motivation to Learn English in Japan with a Self-Study Shadowing Application" Languages 2, no. 4: 19. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2040019

APA Style

Teeter, J. L. (2017). Improving Motivation to Learn English in Japan with a Self-Study Shadowing Application. Languages, 2(4), 19. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2040019

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop