Competition Strategies during Writing in a Second Language: Age and Levels of Complexity
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Unified Competition Model: Languages and Age Differences
1.2. Competition among Systems and Cognitive Processes
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Hypotheses
2.2. Experimental Sample
2.3. Materials
The Writing Test Structure
2.4. Procedure
3. Results and Discussion
4. Conclusions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Alberta Repository. 2012. Writing Samples. Available online: http://www.learnalberta.ca/content/eslapb/writingsamples/docs/grade1_2.pdf (accessed on 5 June 2013).
- Arfé, Barbara, Julie E. Dockrell, and Bianca De Bernardi. 2016. The effect of language specific factors on early written composition: The role of spelling, oral language and text generation skills in a shallow orthography. Reading and Writing 29: 501–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berkes, Éva, and Suzanne Flynn. 2012. Further evidence in support of the Cumulative-Enhancement Model. Third Language Acquisition in Adulthood, 143–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E., C. McBride-Chang, and G. Luk. 2005. Bilingualism, language proficiency, and learning to read in two writing systems. Journal of Educational Psychology 97: 580–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, Ellen, Craik Fergus, and Anthony Ruocco. 2006. Dual-modality monitoring in a classification task: The effects of bilingualism and ageing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 59: 1968–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Birdsong, David, and Jan Vanhove. 2016. Age of second language acquisition: Critical periods and social concerns. In Bilingualism across the Lifespan: Factors Moderating Language Proficiency. Language and the Human Lifespan Series; Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 163–81. [Google Scholar]
- Chiswick, Barry R., and Paul W. Miller. 2005. Linguistic distance: A quantitative measure of the distance between English and other languages. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 26: 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clegg, John, and Oksan Afitska. 2011. Teaching and learning in two languages in African classrooms. Comparative Education 47: 61–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, Andrew D. 2014. Strategies in Learning and Using a Second Language, 2nd ed. Longman Applied Linguistics. Abingdon: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Connor, Ulla. 1996. Contrastive Rhetoric: Cross-Cultural Aspects of Second Language Writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cook, Vivian J. 1985. Chomsky’s universal grammar and second language learning. Applied linguistics 6: 2–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cox, Jessica G., and Cristina Sanz. 2015. Deconstructing PI for the ages: Explicit instruction vs. practice in young and older adult bilinguals. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 53: 225–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cummins, Jim. 2013. Language and identity in multilingual schools: Constructing evidence-based instructional policies. In Managing Diversity in Education: Languages, Policies, Pedagogies. Edited by D. C. Little, Constant Leung and Piet Van Avermaet. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. [Google Scholar]
- Cyrino, Sonia Maria Lazzarini. 2010. On romance syntactic complex predicates: Why Brazilian Portuguese is different (Sobre predicados complexos sintáticos nas línguas românicas. Estudos da Língua (gem) 8: 187–222. [Google Scholar]
- Dabrowska, Ewa. 2004. Language, Mind and Brain: Some Psychological and Neurological Constraints on Theories of Grammar. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ellis, Rod. 2001. Introduction: Investigating form-focused instruction. Language Learning 51: 1–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ellis, Rod. 2015. Understanding Second Language Acquisition 2nd Edition-Oxford Applied Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- European Commission. 2001. European Common Framework for Languages. Porto: Edições Asa. [Google Scholar]
- Farukh, Ammara, and Mila Vulchanova. 2014. Predictors of Reading in Urdu: Does Deep Orthography Have an Impact? Dyslexia 20: 146–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ferris, Dana, and Barrie Roberts. 2001. Error feedback in L2 writing classes: How explicit does it need to be? Journal of Second Language Writing 10: 161–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Figueiredo, Sandra, Margarida Alves Martins, and Carlos Fernandes da Silva. 2015. Second language education context and home language effect: Language dissimilarities and variation differences in immigrant student’s outcomes. International Journal of Multilingualism. Available online: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/14790718.2015.1079204#.VurO6eup3IU (accessed on 15 February 2016). [CrossRef]
- Flynn, Suzanne, Claire Foley, and Inna Vinnitskaya. 2004. The cumulative-enhancement model for language acquisition: Comparing adults’ and children’s patterns of development in first, second and third language acquisition of relative clauses. International Journal of Multilingualism 1: 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fontoura, Helena A., and Linda S. Siegel. 1995. Reading, syntactic, and working memory skills of bilingual Portuguese-English Canadian children. Reading and Writing 7: 139–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guven, C., and A. Islam. 2015. Age at migration, language proficiency, and socioeconomic outcomes: Evidence from Australia. Demography 52: 513–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Han, Ye, and Fiona Hyland. 2015. Exploring learner engagement with written corrective feedback in a Chinese tertiary EFL classroom. Journal of Second Language Writing 30: 31–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hulstijn, Jan H., Richard Frederick Young, Lourdes Ortega, M. Bigelow, R. DeKeyser, N. C. Ellis, James P. Lantolf, Alison Mackey, and Steven Talmy. 2014. Bridging the gap: Cognitive and social approaches to research in second language learning and teaching. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 36: 361–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kepner, Christine Goring. 1991. An experiment in the relationship of types of written feedback to the development of second-language writing skills. The Modern Language Journal 75: 305–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroll, Judith F., and Ellen Bialystok. 2013. Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25: 497–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lenneberg, Eric H. 1967. The biological foundations of language. Hospital Practice 2: 59–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Ping, Jennifer Legault, and Kaitlyn A. Litcofsky. 2014. Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex 58: 301–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lyster, Roy. 2004. Differential effects of prompts and recasts in form-focused instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 26: 399–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacWhinney, Brian. 2005. A Unified Model of Language Acquisition. Available online: www.learnlab.org/uploads/mypslc/publications/unified (accessed on 20 May 2009).
- MacWhinney, Brian, and Elizabeth Bates. 1989. The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Mira, J., and J. Paredes. 2005. Interlinguistic similarity and language death Dynamics. Europhysics Letters 69: 1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mishina-Mori, Satomi. 2005. Autonomous and interdependent development of two language systems in Japanese/English simultaneous bilinguals: Evidence from question formation. First Language 25: 291–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morett, Laura M., and Brian MacWhinney. 2013. Syntactic transfer in English-speaking Spanish learners. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16: 132–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mykhaylyk, Roksolana, Natalia Mitrofanova, Yulia Rodina, and Marit Westergaard. 2015. The Linguistic Proximity Model: The case of verb-second revisited. In Proceedings of the 39th Boston University Conference on Language Development, Sommerville, MA, USA, November 7–9; Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. [Google Scholar]
- Newport, Elissa L. 2016. Statistical language learning: Computational, maturational, and linguistic constraints. Language and Cognition 8: 447–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Odlin, Terence. 1989. Language Transfer: Cross-Linguistic Influence in Language Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Pajares, Frank. 2003. Self-efficacy beliefs, motivation, and achievement in writing: A review of the literature. Reading &Writing Quarterly 19: 139–58. [Google Scholar]
- Salmon, Kathy, and Mike Ettrich. 2012. Alberta K-12 ESL Proficiency Benchmarks. TESL Canada Journal 29: 180–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwabe, Franziska, Nele McElvany, and Matthias Trendtel. 2015. The school age gender gap in reading achievement: Examining the influences of item format and intrinsic reading motivation. Reading Research Quarterly 50: 219–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shum, Mark Shiu Kee, Wing Wa Ki, and Che Kan Leong. 2014. Cognitive and linguistic factors affecting alphasyllabary language users comprehending Chinese text. Reading in a Foreign Language 26: 153–75. [Google Scholar]
- Silva, Tony. 1993. Toward an understanding of the distinct nature of L2 writing: The ESL research and its implications. Tesol Quarterly 27: 657–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spada, Nina. 1997. Form-focussed instruction and second language acquisition: A review of classroom and laboratory research. Language Teaching 30: 73–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tamburelli, Marco, Eirini Sanoudaki, Gary Jones, and Michelle Sowinska. 2015. Acceleration in the bilingual acquisition of phonological structure: Evidence from Polish–English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18: 713–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trenkic, Danijela, Jelena Mirkovic, and Gerry T. Altmann. 2014. Real-time grammar processing by native and non-native speakers: Constructions unique to the second language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 17: 237–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ullman, M. T. 2015. The declarative/procedural model. In Theories in Second Language Acquisition: An Introduction. New York: Routledge, pp. 135–58. [Google Scholar]
- Uylings, Harry. 2006. Development of the human cortex and the concept of “critical” or “sensitive” periods. In The Cognitive Neuroscience of Second Language Acquisition. Edited by M. Gullberg and P. Indefrey. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing, Ltd, pp. 59–90. [Google Scholar]
- Weber-Fox, Christine, and Helen Neville. 1996. Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioural evidence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 8: 231–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Westergaard, Marit, Natalia Mitrofanova, Roksolana Mykhaylyk, and Yulia Rodina. 2017. Crosslinguistic influence in the acquisition of a third language: The Linguistic Proximity Model. International Journal of Bilingualism 21: 666–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, Eddie. 2014. Bridges and Barriers: Language in African Education and Development. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Wray, A. 2000. Formulaic sequences in second language teaching: Principle and practice. Applied linguistics 21: 463–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, Dongbos. 2013. Linguistic distance effect on cross-linguistic transfer of morphological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics 34: 917–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variable | % | SD | Mean | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | Male | 45.9 | ||
Female | 52.0 | |||
Age groups | 7–9 years old | 9.3 | 2.73 | 12.43 |
10–12 years old | 43.5 | |||
13–15 years old | 32.4 | |||
16–18 years old | 14.8 | |||
Nationality | China | 23.1 | ||
Latin America | 5.6 | |||
Eastern Europe | 28.7 | |||
Africa | 17.6 | |||
Western Europe | 11.1 | |||
Asia | 13.0 | |||
Speakers’ groups | Mandarin Language | 31.6 | ||
Romance Languages | 30.6 | |||
Slavic Languages | 14.3 | |||
Creole Ls (Africa) | 11.2 | |||
Indo-Aryan Languages | 10.2 | |||
Length of Residence | 2001–2005 | 13.3 | ||
2006–2009 | 18.4 | |||
2010–2014 | 53.1 | |||
Parallel L1 Training | No | 79.6 | ||
Yes | 16.3 | |||
Proficiency | A1 | 8.2 | ||
A2 | 9.2 | |||
B1 | 1.0 |
Age Groups (Years) | Vocabulary | Grammar | Syntax | Strategy | Sociolinguistics | Discourse | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
7–9 | Mean | 1.20 | 1.30 | 1.30 | 1.00 | 1.30 | 1.30 |
N | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | |
SD | 0.422 | 0.483 | 0.483 | 0.471 | 0.483 | 0.675 | |
10–12 | Mean | 1.57 | 1.40 | 1.57 | 1.38 | 1.48 | 1.31 |
N | 42 | 42 | 42 | 42 | 42 | 42 | |
SD | 0.770 | 0.828 | 0.859 | 0.764 | 0.804 | 0.780 | |
13–15 | Mean | 1.45 | 1.42 | 1.58 | 1.42 | 1.52 | 1.45 |
N | 31 | 31 | 31 | 31 | 31 | 31 | |
SD | 0.810 | 0.923 | 0.807 | 0.886 | 0.962 | 0.995 | |
16–18 | Mean | 1.67 | 1.58 | 1.75 | 1.50 | 1.75 | 1.58 |
N | 12 | 12 | 12 | 12 | 12 | 12 | |
SD | 1.303 | 1.311 | 1.055 | 1.168 | 1.288 | 1.240 | |
Total | Mean | 1.51 | 1.42 | 1.57 | 1.37 | 1.51 | 1.39 |
N | 95 | 95 | 95 | 95 | 95 | 95 | |
SD | 0.836 | 0.894 | 0.834 | 0.839 | 0.898 | 0.903 |
Language Groups | Vocabulary | Grammar | Syntax | Strategy | Sociolinguistics | Discourse |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mandarin | Poor lexicon (utility words only) | Frequent errors for verbal tense (i.e., “caio” instead ‘caiu’) | Simple and correct syntax | Edit actions concerning conjunctions in the text produced | Not observed | Simple and correct |
Romance (Romanian, ex.) | Simple vocabulary (utility and descriptive words) | Frequent errors for verbal tense | Poor syntax | Oral interference (misspelling) | Less evidence of creativity and basic sociolinguistics | Poor discourse connectors |
Slavic (Russian, ex.) | Simple vocabulary | Errors mainly in lexicon with visual and sound similarities (i.e., “crer” instead of ‘querer’; “centado” instead of ‘sentado’) | Incomplete | Misspelling for verbs | Not observed | Not observed |
Creoles (Guinea, ex.) | Poor vocabulary | Errors in verbal tense, connectors * | Some cohesion revealed by introducing connectors; simple sentences | Poor spelling | Describing different feelings | Sequence organized with connectors * |
Indo-Aryan (Nepali, ex.) | Poor vocabulary | Errors in prepositions | Not observed | Misspelling in grammatical words | Not observed | Not observed |
Afro-asiatic (Arabic) | Poor vocabulary | Simple but correct grammar | Poor syntax | Poor spelling (oral interference) | Inability | Poor relations of ideas sequence |
Language Groups | Vocabulary | Grammar | Syntax | Strategy | Sociolinguistics | Discourse | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mandarin speakers | Mean | 1.36 | 1.12 | 1.64 | 1.40 | 1.32 | 1.24 |
N | 25 | 25 | 25 | 25 | 25 | 25 | |
SD | 0.638 | 0.726 | 0.952 | 0.764 | 0.802 | 0.926 | |
Romance speakers | Mean | 1.58 | 1.52 | 1.68 | 1.52 | 1.68 | 1.58 |
N | 31 | 31 | 31 | 31 | 31 | 31 | |
SD | 0.958 | 0.926 | 0.871 | 0.890 | 0.945 | 0.958 | |
Slavic speakers | Mean | 1.62 | 1.54 | 1.54 | 1.08 | 1.46 | 1.46 |
N | 13 | 13 | 13 | 13 | 13 | 13 | |
SD | 0.650 | 0.660 | 0.660 | 0.760 | 0.660 | 0.776 | |
Creole speakers | Mean | 1.90 | 1.90 | 1.70 | 1.60 | 1.90 | 1.50 |
N | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | |
SD | 0.994 | 1.197 | 0.675 | 0.966 | 1.197 | 0.850 | |
Indo-Aryan speakers | Mean | 1.30 | 1.10 | 1.10 | 1.10 | 1.20 | 1.00 |
N | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | |
SD | 1.059 | 0.994 | 0.876 | 0.994 | 1.033 | 1.054 | |
Afro-Asiatic speakers | Mean | 1.50 | 2.00 | 1.50 | 1.50 | 1.50 | 1.50 |
N | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 | |
SD | 0.707 | 1.414 | 0.707 | 0.707 | 0.707 | 0.707 |
Instruction in L1 | Vocabulary | Grammar | Syntax | Strategy | Sociolinguistics | Discourse | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
No | Mean | 1.48 | 1.41 | 1.49 | 1.29 | 1.47 | 1.33 |
N | 79 | 79 | 79 | 79 | 79 | 79 | |
SD | 0.860 | 0.941 | 0.799 | 0.850 | 0.903 | 0.902 | |
Yes | Mean | 1.60 | 1.60 | 2.20 | 1.80 | 1.80 | 1.90 |
N | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | 10 | |
SD | 0.699 | 0.699 | 0.789 | 0.789 | 0.789 | 0.876 |
© 2019 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Figueiredo, S. Competition Strategies during Writing in a Second Language: Age and Levels of Complexity. Languages 2019, 4, 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4010011
Figueiredo S. Competition Strategies during Writing in a Second Language: Age and Levels of Complexity. Languages. 2019; 4(1):11. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4010011
Chicago/Turabian StyleFigueiredo, Sandra. 2019. "Competition Strategies during Writing in a Second Language: Age and Levels of Complexity" Languages 4, no. 1: 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4010011
APA StyleFigueiredo, S. (2019). Competition Strategies during Writing in a Second Language: Age and Levels of Complexity. Languages, 4(1), 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4010011