Linguistic Diversity, Multilingualism, and Cognitive Skills: A Study of Disadvantaged Children in India
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Indian Context
1.2. Multilingualism and Cognition
- How can we quantify the distinction between multilingualism in the individual and the linguistically and culturally diverse environment of children in Indian primary government schools?
- Is there a relationship between bilingualism and/or sociolinguistic diversity and cognitive skills?
- To what extent do socio-economic status, medium of instruction, and other demographic variables correlate with cognitive skills?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Methods
2.3. Scoring
- 1 point if the conversational partner was of the same gender vs. 2 points if the partner was of a different gender;
- 1 point if the conversational partner was of the same age, 2 points if the conversational partner was of a different age, and 3 points if there were conversational partners of both the same and a different age;
- 1 point if the interactions were in the same language as the child’s home language vs. 2 points if the interactions were in a different language.
- 1 point for income when only one parent worked vs. 2 points when both parents worked;
- 1 point when their house had 1 room vs. 2 points when their house had two or more rooms;
- 1 point when they had 1 gadget at home, 2 points when they had 2 gadgets at home, and 3 points when they had 3 or more gadgets at home;
- 1 point when they had tap water in the house vs. 0 points when they did not;
- 1 point when they did no chores at home vs. 0 points when they did;
- 1 point when they did homework after going back home vs. 0 point when they did not;
2.4. Analysis
3. Results
3.1. How Can We Quantify the Distinction between Multilingualism in the Individual and the Linguistically and Culturally Diverse Environment of Children in Government Primary Schools in India?
3.2. Is there a Relationship between Bilingualism and/or Sociolinguistic Diversity and Cognitive Skills?
3.3. To What Extent Do Socio-Economic Status, Medium of Instruction, and Other Demographic Variables Correlate with Cognitive Skills?
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
References
- Agrawal, Tushar. 2014. Educational inequality in rural and urban India. International Journal of Educational Development 34: 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alcott, Benjamin, and Pauline Rose. 2015. Schools and learning in rural India and Pakistan: Who goes where, and how much are they learning? Prospects 45: 345–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Alcott, Benjamin, and Pauline Rose. 2017. Learning in India’s primary schools: How do disparities widen across the grades? International Journal of Educational Development 56: 42–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arán-Filippetti, Vanessa. 2013. Structure and invariance of executive functioning tasks across socioeconomic status: Evidence from Spanish-speaking children. The Spanish Journal of Psychology 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aran-Filippetti, Vanessa, and Maria Cristina Richaud de Minzi. 2012. A structural analysis of executive functions and socioeconomic status in school-age children: Cognitive factors as effect mediators. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 173: 393–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aslam, Monazza, Rabea Malik, Shenila Rawal, Pauline Rose, and Anna Vignoles. 2019. Do government schools improve learning for poor students? Evidence from rural Pakistan. Oxford Review of Education 45: 802–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bedore, Lisa M., Elizabeth D. Peña, Connie L. Summers, Karin M. Boerger, Maria D. Resendiz, Kai Greene, Thomas M. Bohman, and Ronald B. Gillam. 2012. The measure matters: Language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish-English bilingual children. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England) 15: 616–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Belacchi, Carmen, Barbara Carretti, and Cesare Cornoldi. 2010. The role of working memory and updating in Coloured Raven Matrices performance in typically developing children. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 22: 1010–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bhattacharjea, Suman, Wilima Wadhwa, and Rukmini Banerji. 2011. Inside Primary Schools: A Study of Teaching and Learning in Rural India. Pratham Mumbai Education Initiative. October 7. Available online: http://img.asercentre.org/docs/Publications/Inside_Primary_School/Report/tl_study_print_ready_version_oct_7_2011.pdf (accessed on 15 March 2020).
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2009. Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism 12: 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2013. The impact of bilingualism on language and literacy development. In The Handbook of Bilingualism and Multilingualism. Edited by Tej K. Bhatia and William C. Ritchie. New York: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 624–48. [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, Ellen, and Dana Shapero. 2005. Ambiguous benefits: The effect of bilingualism on reversing ambiguous figures. Developmental Science 8: 595–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, Ellen, Fergus I. M. Craik, Raymond Klein, and Mythili Viswanathan. 2004. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging 19: 290–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Brito, Natalie H., and Kimberly G. Noble. 2014. Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Frontiers in Neuroscience 8: 276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Brouwers, Symen A., Fons J. R. Van de Vijver, and Dianne A. Van Hemert. 2009. Variation in Raven’s Progressive Matrices scores across time and place. Learning and Individual Differences 19: 330–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Burneo-Garcés, Carlos, Francisco Cruz-Quintana, Miguel Pérez-García, Manuel Fernández-Alcántara, Ahmed Fasfous, and Mária Nieves Pérez-Marfil. 2019. Interaction between Socioeconomic Status and Cognitive Development in Children Aged 7, 9, and 11 Years: A Cross-Sectional Study. Developmental Neuropsychology 44: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Byers-Heinlein, Krista, Alena G. Esposito, Adam Winsler, Viorica Marian, Dina C. Castro, and Gigi Luk. 2019. The Case for Measuring and Reporting Bilingualism in Developmental Research. Collabra: Psychology 5: 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Carneiro, Pedro, and James J. Heckman. 2002. The evidence on credit constraints in post-secondary schooling. The Economic Journal 112: 705–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Carneiro, Pedro, and James J. Heckman. 2003. Human capital policy. In Inequality in America: What Role for Human Capital Policies? Edited by James J. Heckman, Alan Krueger and Benjamin Friedman. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 77–239. [Google Scholar]
- Cattell, Raymond B. 1940. A culture-free intelligence test. I. Journal of Educational Psychology 31: 176–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cattell, Raymond B., and A. Karen S. Cattell. 1963. Culture Fair Intelligence Test. Champaign: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. [Google Scholar]
- Ciesielski, Kristina T., Paul G. Lesnik, Robert L. Savoy, Ellen P. Grant, and Seppo P. Ahlfors. 2006. Developmental neural networks in children performing a Categorical N-Back Task. NeuroImage 33: 980–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, Eve V. 2004. How language acquisition builds on cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 472–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, Jonathan D., William M Perlstein, Todd S. Braver, Leigh E. Nystrom, Douglas C. Noll, John Jonides, and Edward E. Smith. 1997. Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature 386: 604–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Colzato, Lorenza S., Maria Teresa Bajo, Wery Van den Wildenberg, Daniela Paolieri, Sander Nieuwenhuis, Wido La Heij, and Bernhard Hommel. 2008. How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 34: 302–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Costa, Albert, Mireia Hernández, and Núria Sebastián-Gallés. 2008. Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition 106: 59–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Costa, Albert, Mireia Hernández, Jordi Costa-Faidella, and Núria Sebastián-Gallés. 2009. On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition 113: 135–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cotton, Sue M., Petricia M. Kiely, David P. Crewther, Brenda Thomson, Robin Laycock, and Sheila G. Crewther. 2005. A normative and reliability study for the Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices for primary school aged children from Victoria, Australia. Personality and Individual Differences 39: 647–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cunha, Flavio, and James J. Heckman. 2008. Formulating, identifying and estimating the technology of cognitive and noncognitive skill formation. Journal of Human Resources 43: 738–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Villiers, Jill. 2007. The Interface of Language and Theory of Mind. Lingua 117: 1858–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Devy, Ganesh. 2018. People’s Linguistic Survey of India. Gujarat: Bhasha Research and Publication Centre, Available online: http://www.peopleslinguisticsurvey.org/ (accessed on 8 February 2020).
- Diaz, Rafael M. 1985. Bilingual cognitive development: Addressing three gaps in current research. Child Development 56: 1376–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Drèze, Jean, and Geeta Gandhi Kingdon. 2001. School Participation in Rural India. Review of Development Economics 5: 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engel de Abreu, Pascale M. J., Anabela Cruz-Santos, Carlos J. Tourinho, Romain Martin, and Ellen Bialystok. 2012. Bilingualism enriches the poor: Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science 23: 1364–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Epstein, Jeffery N., Joshua M. Langberg, Paul J. Rosen, Amanda Graham, Megan E. Narad, Tanya N. Antonini, William B. Brinkman, Tanya Froehlich, John O. Simon, and Mekibib Altaye. 2011. Evidence for higher reaction time variability for children with ADHD on a range of cognitive tasks including reward and event rate manipulations. Neuropsychology 25: 427–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- ESRC. 2010. ESRC Framework for Research Ethics (FRE) 2010. Available online: https://esrc.ukri.org/files/funding/guidance-for-applicants/esrc-framework-for-research-ethics-2010/ (accessed on 10 March 2020).
- Fernald, Lia C. H., Patricia Kariger, Melissa Hidrobo, and Paul J. Gertler. 2012. Socioeconomic gradients in child development in very young children: Evidence from India, Indonesia, Peru, and Senegal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109: 17273–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gentner, Dedre, and Jeffrey Loewenstein. 2002. Relational language and relational thought. In Language, Literacy, and Cognitive Development: The Development and Consequences of Symbolic Language. Edited by Eric Amsel and James P. Byrnes. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 87–120. [Google Scholar]
- Ghosh, Satabdi, Sutanu Dutta Chowdhury, Ananga Mohan Chandra, and Tusharkanti Ghosh. 2015. Grades of undernutrition and socioeconomic status influence cognitive development in school children of Kolkata. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 156: 274–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Glaeser, Edward, and Helen Sims. 2015. Contagion, Crime, and Congestion: Overcoming the Downsides of Density. Growth Brief, May 2015. London: International Growth Centre. [Google Scholar]
- Government of National Capital Territory of Delhi. 2015. Urban Slums in Delhi; Delhi: Directorate of Economics and Statistics.
- Hackman, Daniel A., and Martha J. Farah. 2009. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 65–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hackman, Daniel A., Martha J. Farah, and Michael J. Meaney. 2010. Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11: 651–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hamadani, Jena D., Fahmida Tofail, Syed N. Huda, Dewan S. Alam, Deborah A. Ridout, Orazio Attanasio, and Sally M. Grantham-McGregor. 2014. Cognitive deficit and poverty in the first 5 years of childhood in Bangladesh. Pediatrics 134: e1001–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hansen, Laura Birke, Pedro Macizo, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia, David Saldaña, Manuel Carreiras, Luis J. Fuentes, and M. Teresa Bajo. 2016. Emergent Bilingualism and Working Memory Development in School Aged Children. Language Learning 66: 51–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- ICMR. 2006. Ethical Guidelines for Biomedical Research on Human Participants. Ethical Review Prodedures 11. Available online: http://icmr.nic.in/ethical_guidelines.pdf (accessed on 10 June 2016).
- Jarvis, Lorna H., Joseph H. Danks, and William E. Merriman. 1995. The effect of bilingualism on cognitive ability: A test of the level of bilingualism hypothesis. Applied Psycholinguistics 16: 293–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jayachandran, Usha. 2002. Socio-Economic Determinants of School Attendance in India. In Working Papers 103. Delhi: Centre for Development Economics, Delhi School of Economics. [Google Scholar]
- Jerrim, John, and Anna Vignoles. 2013. Social mobility, regression to the mean and the cognitive development of high ability children from disadvantaged homes. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (Statistics in Society) 176: 887–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaltsa, Maria, Alexandra Prentza, and Ianthi Maria Tsimpli. 2019. Input and literacy effects in simultaneous and sequential bilinguals: The performance of Albanian-Greek speaking children in sentence repetition. International Journal of Bilingualism. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelly, Yvonne, Amanda Sacker, Emilia Del Bono, Marco Francesconi, and Michael Marmot. 2011. What role for the home learning environment and parenting in reducing the socioeconomic gradient in child development? Findings from the Millennium Cohort Study. Archives of Disease in Childhood 96: 832–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kirchner, Wayne K. 1958. Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. Journal of Experimental Psychology 55: 352–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Levin, Harvey S., Gerri R. Hanten, Ching-Chi Chang, Lifang Zhang, Russell Schachar, Linda Ewing-Cobbs, and Jeffrey E. Max. 2002. Working memory after traumatic brain injury in children. Annals of Neurology 52: 82–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- López-Vicente, Mónica, Joan Forns, Elisabet Suades-González, Mikel Esnaola, Raquel García-Esteban, Mar Álvarez-Pedrerol, Jordi Júlvez, Miguel Burgaleta, Núria Sebastián-Gallés, and Jordi Sunyer. 2016. Developmental Trajectories in Primary Schoolchildren Using n-Back Task. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Mackworth, Jane F. 1959. Paced memorizing in a continuous task. Journal of Experimental Psychology 58: 206–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, Akira, Naomi P. Friedman, Michael J. Emerson, Alexander H. Witzki, Amy Howerter, and Tor D. Wager. 2000. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41: 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mohanty, Ajit K. 2019. The Multilingual Reality: Living with Languages. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. [Google Scholar]
- Mohanty, Ajit K., and Prasad Sankar Das. 1987. Cognitive and metalinguistic ability of unschooled bilingual and unilingual tribal children. Psychological Studies 32: 5–8. [Google Scholar]
- Mohanty, Ajit K., Mahendra Kumar Mishra, N. Upender Reddy, and Gumidyala Ramesh. 2009. Overcoming the language barrier for tribal children: MLE in Andhra Pradesh and Orissa, India. In Multilingual Education for Social Justice: Globalising the Local. Edited by Ajit K. Mohanty, Minati Panda, Robert Phillipson and Tove Skutnabb-Kangas. New Delhi: Orient Blackswan, pp. 278–91. [Google Scholar]
- Moore, Mary E., and Bruce M. Ross. 1963. Context effects in running memory. Psychological Reports 12: 451–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moradzadeh, Linda, Galit Blumenthal, and Melody Wiseheart. 2015. Musical Training, Bilingualism, and Executive Function: A Closer Look at Task Switching and Dual-Task Performance. Cognitive Science 39: 992–1020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morris, Neil, and Dylan Jones. 1990. Memory updating in working memory: The role of the central executive. British Journal of Psychology 81: 111–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Noble, Kimberly G., M. Frank Norman, and Martha J. Farah. 2005. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science 8: 74–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ochs, Elinor, and Bambi B. Schieffelin. 2008. Language socialization: An historical overview. In Encyclopedia of Language and Education: Vol. 8. Language Socialization. Edited by Patricia A. Duff and Nancy H. Hornberger. New York: Springer, pp. 3–15. [Google Scholar]
- Paap, Kenneth R., and Zachary I. Greenberg. 2013. There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology 66: 232–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Paap Kenneth R., Hunter A. Myuz, Regina T. Anders, Morgan F. Bockelman, Roman Mikulinsky, and Oliver M. Sawi. 2017. No compelling evidence for a bilingual advantage in switching or that frequent language switching reduces switch cost. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 29: 89–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pace, Amy, Rufan Luo, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, and Roberta Michnick Golinkoff. 2017. Identifying Pathways Between Socioeconomic Status and Language Development. Annual Review of Linguistics 3: 285–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pattanaik, Kabita. 2004. Relationship between Metalinguistic and Cognitive Development of Bilingual and Monolingual Tribal Children. Ph.D. dissertation, Utkal University, Bhubaneswar, Indian. [Google Scholar]
- Pattanaik, Kabita, and Ajit K. Mohanty. 1984. Relationship between metalinguistic and cognitive development of bilingual and unilingual tribal children. Psycholingua 14: 63–70. [Google Scholar]
- Paxson, Christina, and Norbert Schady. 2007. Cognitive development among young children in Ecuador the roles of wealth, health, and parenting. Journal of Human Resources 42: 49–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peal, Elizabeth, and Wallace E. Lambert. 1962. The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs 76: 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peirce, Jonathan W. 2007. PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 162: 8–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Petrou, Stavros, and Emil Kupek. 2010. Poverty and childhood undernutrition in developing countries: A multi-national cohort study. Social Science & Medicine 71: 1366–73. [Google Scholar]
- Pollack, Irwin, and Donald A. Norman. 1964. Non-parametric analysis of recognition experiments. Psychonomic Science 1: 125–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pratham. 2014. Annual Status of Education Report (Rural) 2013. New Delhi: Pratham, Available online: http://img.asercentre.org/docs/Publications/ASER%20Reports/ASER%202014/fullaser2014mainreport_1.pdf (accessed on 2 February 2019).
- Pratham. 2017. Annual Status of Education Report (Rural) 2016. Report for the Annual Status of Education Report Centre, 18 January 2017. New Delhi: Pratham, Available online: http://img.asercentre.org/docs/Publications/ASER%20Reports/ASER%202016/aser_2016.pdf (accessed on 15 November 2017).
- Raven, John Carlyle. 1938. Progressive Matrices: A Perceptual Test of Intelligence, Sets A, B, C, D, and E. London: H. K. Lewis. [Google Scholar]
- Raven, John Carlyle. 1956. Progressive Matrices, Sets A, B, C, D and E. London: H. K. Lewis. [Google Scholar]
- Raven, John. 2012. Coloured Progressive Matrices and Crichton Vocabulary Scale: India Edition. India: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Raven, John, John Carlylr Raven, and John Hugh Court. 2008. Ravens Coloured Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. London: Pearson Education. [Google Scholar]
- Revelle, William. 2019. Psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. R package version 1.9.12. Evanston: Northwestern University, Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych (accessed on 28 January 2020).
- Rothou, Kyriakoula M., and Ianthi Maria Tsimpli. 2017. Biliteracy and reading ability in children who learn Greek as a second language. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roy, Debraj, Bharath Palavalli, Niveditha Menon, Robin King, Karin Pfeffer, Michael Lees, and Peter M. A. Sloot. 2018. Survey-based socio-economic data from slums in Bangalore, India. Scientific Data 5: 170200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Sanchez-Azanza, Victor A., Raúl López-Penadés, Lucía Buil-Legaz, Eva Aguilar-Mediavilla, and Daniel Adrover-Roig. 2017. Is bilingualism losing its advantage? A bibliometric approach. PLoS ONE 12: e0176151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Scudder, Mark R., Kate Lambourne, Eric S. Drollette, Stephen D. Herrmann, Richard A. Washburn, Joseph E. Donnelly, and Charles H. Hillman. 2014. Aerobic capacity and cognitive control in elementary school-age children. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 46: 1025–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Simons, Gary F., and Charles D. Fennig. 2018. Ethnologue: Languages of the World, Twenty-first edition. Dallas: SIL International, Available online: http://www.ethnologue.com (accessed on 6 February 2020).
- Soveri, Anna, Matti Laine, Heikki Hämäläinen, and Kenneth Hugdahl. 2011a. Bilingual advantage in attentional control: Evidence from the forced-attention dichotic listening paradigm. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14: 371–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soveri, Anna, Antoni Rodriguez-Fornells, and Matti Laine. 2011b. Is There a Relationship between Language Switching and Executive Functions in Bilingualism? Introducing a within group Analysis Approach. Frontiers in Psychology 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Streiner, David L. 2003. Starting at the Beginning: An Introduction to Coefficient Alpha and Internal Consistency. Journal of Personality Assessment 80: 99–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Teubner-Rhodes, Susan E., Alan Mishler, Ryan Corbett, Llorenç Andreu, Monica Sanz-Torrent, John C. Trueswell, and Jared M. Novick. 2016. The effects of bilingualism on conflict monitoring, cognitive control, and garden-path recovery. Cognition 150: 213–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torchiano, Marco. 2019. Effsize: Efficient Effect Size Computation, R package version 0.7.6; Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=effsize (accessed on 28 January 2020). [CrossRef]
- Torregrossa, Jacopo, Christiane Bongartz, and Ianthi Maria Tsimpli. 2019. Bilingual reference production: A cognitive-computational account. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 9: 569–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsimpli, Ianthi, Lina Mukhopadhyay, Jeanine Treffers-Daller, Suverna Alladi, Theodoros Marinis, Minati Panda, Anusha Balasubramanian, and Pallawi Sinha. 2019. Multilingualism and multiliteracy in primary education in India: A discussion of some methodological challenges of an interdisciplinary research project. Research in Comparative and International Education 14: 54–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tsujita, Yuko. 2009. Deprivation of Education in Urban Areas: A Basic Profile of Slum Children in Delhi, India. Institute of Developing Economies, Japan External Trade Organization (JETRO) 199: 8–10. [Google Scholar]
- Unsworth, Sharon. 2013. Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16: 86–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ursache, Alexandra, and Kimberly G. Noble. 2016. Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context: Multiple mechanisms and implications for measuring socioeconomic status. Psychophysiology 53: 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ursachi, George, Ioana Alexandra Horodnic, and Adriana Zait. 2015. How Reliable are Measurement Scales? External Factors with Indirect Influence on Reliability Estimators. Procedia Economics and Finance 20: 679–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vasanta, Duggirala, Suvarna Alladi, Jala Sireesha, and Surampudi Bapi Raju. 2010. Language Choice and Language use Patterns among Telugu-Hindi/Urdu-English Speakers in Hyderabad, India. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Language, Society and Culture in Asian Contexts. Maha Sarakham: Mahasarakam University. [Google Scholar]
- Vuontela, Virve, Maija-Riikka Steenari, Synnove Carlson, Juha Koivisto, Mika Fjällberg, and Eeva T. Aronen. 2003. Audiospatial and visuospatial working memory in 6–13 year old school children. Learning and Memory 10: 74–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Walker, Susan P., Theodore D. Wachs, Sally Grantham-McGregor, Maureen M. Black, Charles A. Nelson, Sandra L. Huffman, Helen Baker-Henningham, Susan M. Chang, Jena D. Hamadani, Betsy Lozoff, and et al. 2011. Inequality in early childhood: Risk and protective factors for early child development. The Lancet 378: 1325–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weichbold, Viktor, and Hermann Herka. 2003. Performance of hearing impaired children on Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices Test. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 67: 1213–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weyandt, Lisa L., and W. Grant Willis. 1994. Executive functions in school-aged children: Potential efficacy of tasks in discriminating clinical groups. Developmental Neuropsychology 10: 27–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yow, W. Quin, and Xiaoqian Li. 2015. Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: Why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in Psychology 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zhang, Jun, and Shane T. Mueller. 2005. A note on ROC analysis and non-parametric estimate of sensitivity. Psychometrika 70: 203–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
1 | In India there is no one national language. The Indian constitution in its 8th schedule recognizes 22 languages as scheduled languages. They are mostly also used as media of instruction as relevant to different Indian states. The languages are Assamese, Bangla, Bodo, Dogri, Gujarati, Hindi, Kashmiri, Kannada, Konkani, Maithili, Malayalam, Manipuri, Marathi, Nepali, Oriya, Punjabi, Tamil, Telugu, Sanskrit, Santali, Sindhi, and Urdu. Two of these are classical languages, Sanskrit and Tamil, as recognized by the Indian constitution. Hindi and English function as link languages, with the central government recognizing Hindi as the official language and English as the provisional sub-language (Devy 2018). |
2 | Based on this finding, we re-examined whether the findings on bilingualism and sociolinguistic diversity influencing Raven’s scores still held when city was taken into account as an independent variable in the model. The results show that the effects of bilingualism and the interaction between bilingualism and sociolinguistic diversity were unaffected by the addition of city as a factor. |
Location | Number | Gender | Age (SD) | Age Range | Medium of Instruction | School Site |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Delhi | 371 | 178 f, 193 m | 8.75 (0.65) | 8–12 | 75 Hindi, 296 English | 173 slum, 198 non-slum |
Hyderabad | 316 | 176 f, 140 m | 9.56 (1.20) | 8–15 | 199 Telugu, 117 English | 170 slum, 146 non-slum |
Total | 687 | 354 f, 333 m | 9.12 (1.03) | 8–15 | 274 regional language, 413 English | 343 slum, 344 non-slum |
Location | Raven’s Raw Score (SD) | n-Back A-Prime (SD) | Sociolinguistic Diversity max. 21 (SD) | SES max. 10 (SD) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Delhi | monolinguals (n = 228, 61%) | 20.1 (5.9) | 0.69 (0.14) | 14.9 (1.1) | 6.8 (1.0) |
bilinguals (n = 143, 39%) | 21.1 (5.6) | 0.70 (0.15) | 15.4 (1.4) | 7.1 (1.2) | |
total Delhi | 20.5 (5.8) | 0.69 (0.14) | 15.1 (1.3) | 6.9 (1.0) | |
Hyderabad | monolinguals (n = 175, 55%) | 15.7 (5.1) | 0.65 (0.17) | 13.7 (3.0) | 8.2 (1.0) |
bilinguals (n = 141, 45%) | 17.3 (6.2) | 0.69 (0.15) | 15.8 (2.6) | 8.4 (0.9) | |
total Hyderabad | 16.4 (5.7) | 0.67 (0.17) | 14.7 (3.0) | 8.3 (1.0) | |
Total average | monolinguals (n = 403, 59%) | 18.2 (6.0) | 0.67 (0.15) | 14.4 (2.2) | 7.4 (1.2) |
bilinguals (n = 284, 41%) | 19.2 (6.2) | 0.70 (0.15) | 15.6 (2.1) | 7.8 (1.2) | |
Total | 18.6 (6.1) | 0.68 (0.15) | 14.9 (2.3) | 7.5 (1.2) |
Location | Monolinguals | Bilinguals | Sociolinguistic Diversity |
---|---|---|---|
Delhi | 46.1% EMI–53.9% HMI | 65.0% EMI–35.0% HMI | 15.0 EMI–15.2 HMI |
Hyderabad | 18.3% EMI–81.7% TMI | 60.3% EMI–39.7% TMI | 15.5 EMI–14.2 TMI |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Tsimpli, I.M.; Vogelzang, M.; Balasubramanian, A.; Marinis, T.; Alladi, S.; Reddy, A.; Panda, M. Linguistic Diversity, Multilingualism, and Cognitive Skills: A Study of Disadvantaged Children in India. Languages 2020, 5, 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5010010
Tsimpli IM, Vogelzang M, Balasubramanian A, Marinis T, Alladi S, Reddy A, Panda M. Linguistic Diversity, Multilingualism, and Cognitive Skills: A Study of Disadvantaged Children in India. Languages. 2020; 5(1):10. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5010010
Chicago/Turabian StyleTsimpli, Ianthi Maria, Margreet Vogelzang, Anusha Balasubramanian, Theodoros Marinis, Suvarna Alladi, Abhigna Reddy, and Minati Panda. 2020. "Linguistic Diversity, Multilingualism, and Cognitive Skills: A Study of Disadvantaged Children in India" Languages 5, no. 1: 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5010010
APA StyleTsimpli, I. M., Vogelzang, M., Balasubramanian, A., Marinis, T., Alladi, S., Reddy, A., & Panda, M. (2020). Linguistic Diversity, Multilingualism, and Cognitive Skills: A Study of Disadvantaged Children in India. Languages, 5(1), 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5010010